Which Matters More: Intention or Outcome? The Asymmetry of Moral Blame and Moral Praise
Abstract
1. Introduction
1.1. Intentions and Consequences in Moral Judgement
1.2. Asymmetric Effects of Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Judgements
1.3. Self–Other Perspective and Moral Judgement
1.4. Severity of Harm and Moral Judgement
2. Study 1: Asymmetric Effects of Behavioural Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Blame and Praise
2.1. Experiment 1a: Effects of Behavioural Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Blame in Hurtful Situations
2.1.1. Pre-Experiment 1a: Screening of Moral Blame Materials
- (1)
- Demographic Information.
- (2)
- Story Scenarios: To standardize the story materials, each story scenario was presented in segments following the order of background-foreshadowing-behavioural intention-behavioural outcome. Each story scenario had four conditions, and an example of the experimental material (using the bicycle material as an example) is shown in Figure 1.
- (3)
- Comprehension Questions. Two comprehension questions were designed in this experiment to confirm whether participants could correctly infer the intentions of the story’s protagonist and the outcome experienced by another person in the story from each story scenario (Kurdi et al., 2020). Intentional comprehension questions include whether the protagonist believes that something bad will happen. The outcome comprehension question asks whether something bad happens to the other person in the story.
- (4)
- Intentionality Question. The story protagonist’s intentions were tested for manipulativeness on a 7-point scale (1 = completely unintentional, 7 = completely intentional), where larger numbers represent more negative intentions (Martin & Cushman, 2016).
- (5)
- Outcome-based Questions. A manipulative test of the outcome experienced by the other person in the story was carried out on a 7-point scale (1 = no harm, 7 = maximum harm), where a larger number represents a more negative outcome (Martin & Cushman, 2016).
2.1.2. Participants
2.1.3. Results
2.2. Experiment 1b: The Effect of Behavioural Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Praise in Helping Situations
2.2.1. Pre-Experiment 1b: Screening of Moral Praise Materials
2.2.2. Participants
2.2.3. Experimental Design and Procedures
2.2.4. Results
Types of Evaluation | Situation | Predictors | β | 95% CI | t | R2 | F |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Moral Blame (Experiment 1a) | Bicycle | 0.71 | 754.42 *** | ||||
Intention | 0.71 | [0.67, 0.75] | 34.38 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.23 | [0.18, 0.27] | 10.69 *** | ||||
Ham Sausage | 0.72 | 811.13 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.66 | [0.62, 0.70] | 32.64 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.28 | [0.24, 0.32] | 12.87 *** | ||||
Insecticide | 0.75 | 943.61 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.69 | [0.65, 0.73] | 35.95 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.29 | [0.25, 0.32] | 15.04 *** | ||||
Hair Curling Iron | 0.76 | 999.62 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.69 | [0.65, 0.73] | 34.94 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.28 | [0.24, 0.31] | 13.90 *** | ||||
Sashimi | 0.79 | 1148.98 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.74 | [0.70, 0.77] | 40.58 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.23 | [0.20, 0.27] | 12.53 *** | ||||
Sneakers | 0.77 | 1043.01 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.70 | [0.66, 0.74] | 36.62 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.25 | [0.21, 0.29] | 12.88 *** | ||||
Six Scenarios’ Average | 0.75 | 5656.89 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.70 | [0.68, 0.72] | 87.91 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.26 | [0.24, 0.28] | 31.82 *** | ||||
Moral Praise (Experiment 1b) | Necklace | 0.71 | 765.23 *** | ||||
Intention | 0.48 | [0.44, 0.52] | 24.84 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.48 | [0.44, 0.51] | 24.54 *** | ||||
Restaurant | 0.76 | 965.37 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.45 | [0.42, 0.49] | 24.43 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.51 | [0.47, 0.55] | 27.85 *** | ||||
Swimming Pool | 0.73 | 855.50 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.45 | [0.41, 0.49] | 24.65 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.52 | [0.48, 0.55] | 27.59 *** | ||||
Ski Resort | 0.75 | 959.72 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.42 | [0.39, 0.46] | 24.62 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.52 | [0.49, 0.56] | 28.52 *** | ||||
Amusement Park | 0.75 | 935.80 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.45 | [0.42, 0.48] | 25.88 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.51 | [0.47, 0.54] | 27.58 *** | ||||
Hospitals | 0.76 | 986.56 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.39 | [0.35, 0.42] | 22.07 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.55 | [0.51, 0.59] | 29.50 *** | ||||
Six Scenarios’ Average | 0.74 | 5415.42 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.44 | [0.43, 0.46] | 59.78 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.51 | [0.50, 0.53] | 67.32 *** |
3. Study 2: The Effects of Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Blame and Praise: The Moderating Role of Self–Others
3.1. Experiment 2a: The Effects of Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Blame: The Moderating Role of Self–Others
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Experimental Design and Procedures
3.1.3. Results
3.2. Experiment 2b Effects of Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Praise: The Moderating Role of Self–Others
3.2.1. Participants
3.2.2. Experimental Design and Procedures
3.2.3. Results
Types of Evaluation | Predictors | β | 95% CI | t | R2 | F | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Moral Blame (Experiment 1a) | Others-Blame | 0.77 | 997.81 *** | ||||
Intention | 0.62 | [0.58, 0.65] | 32.41 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.35 | [0.31, 0.38] | 17.57 *** | ||||
Self-Blame | 0.74 | 827.78 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.40 | [0.36, 0.44] | 19.92 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.52 | [0.48, 0.57] | 24.91 *** | ||||
Moral Praise (Experiment 1b) | Others-Praise | 0.71 | 665.14 *** | ||||
Intention | 0.36 | [0.32, 0.40] | 17.22 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.55 | [0.51, 0.59] | 25.38 *** | ||||
Self-Praise | 0.76 | 835.16 *** | |||||
Intention | 0.36 | [0.32, 0.40] | 17.71 *** | ||||
Outcome | 0.58 | [0.54, 0.62] | 27.55 *** |
4. Study 3: Effects of Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Blame and Praise: Moderating Effects of Self–Other and Outcome Severity
4.1. Experiment 3a Effects of Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Blame: Moderating Effects of Self–Other and Outcome Severity
4.1.1. Participants
4.1.2. Experimental Design and Procedures
4.1.3. Results
Outcome Variable | Level of Outcome | Predictors | β | 95% CI | t |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 Others-Blame | |||||
Minor outcome | Intention | 0.53 | [0.49, 0.57] | 26.05 *** | |
Outcome | 0.35 | [0.26, 0.45] | 7.36 *** | ||
Severe outcome | Intention | 0.33 | [0.30, 0.37] | 18.03 *** | |
Outcome | 0.38 | [0.28, 0.49] | 7.28 *** | ||
2 Self-Blame | |||||
Minor outcome | Intention | 0.41 | [0.37, 0.46] | 18.68 *** | |
Outcome | 0.54 | [0.44, 0.65] | 9.94 *** | ||
Severe outcome | Intention | 0.35 | [0.30, 0.39] | 14.83 *** | |
Outcome | 0.57 | [0.42, 0.72] | 7.51 *** |
4.2. Experiment 3b Effects of Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Praise: The Moderating Role of Self–Other and Outcome Severity
4.2.1. Participants
4.2.2. Experimental Design and Procedures
4.2.3. Results
Outcome Variable | Level of Outcome | Predictors | β | 95% CI | t |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 Others-Praise | |||||
Minor outcome | Intention | 0.32 | [0.27, 0.37] | 12.14 *** | |
Outcome | 0.34 | [0.26, 0.43] | 7.71 *** | ||
Severe outcome | Intention | 0.31 | [0.26, 0.36] | 12.68 *** | |
Outcome | 0.38 | [0.28, 0.47] | 8.16 *** | ||
2 Self-Praise | |||||
Minor outcome | Intention | 0.25 | [0.20, 0.29] | 10.54 *** | |
Outcome | 0.52 | [0.42, 0.61] | 10.78 *** | ||
Severe outcome | Intention | 0.13 | [0.09, 0.17] | 6.30 *** | |
Outcome | 0.57 | [0.49, 0.66] | 12.84 *** |
5. Discussion
5.1. Asymmetric Effects of Behavioural Intentions-Outcomes on Moral Blame and Praise
5.2. Asymmetric Effects of Behavioural Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Blame and Praise: Self–Other Perspective Differences
5.3. Asymmetric Effects of Behavioural Intentions and Outcomes on Moral Blame and Praise: The Moderating Role of Outcome Severity
5.4. Significance of the Study
5.5. Research Limitations and Future Research Perspectives
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix A.1. “Others” Group on Moral Blame
- Neutral Intention—Minor Outcome
- 1.
- Comprehension Questions (Yes/No)
- 2.
- Intentionality Question (1–7)
- 3.
- Outcome-related Question (1–7)
- 4.
- Moral Blame Evaluation (1–7)
- Neutral Intention—Severe Outcome
- Negative Intention—Minor Outcome
- Negative Intention-Severe Outcome
Appendix A.2. “Self” Group on Moral Praise
- Neutral Intention-Minor Outcome
- 1.
- Comprehension Questions (Yes/No)
- 2.
- Intentionality Question (1–7)
- 3.
- Outcome-related Question (1–7)
- 4.
- Moral Praise Evaluation (1–7)
- Neutral Intention-Severe Outcome
- Positive Intention—Minor Outcome
- Positive Intention—Severe Outcome
References
- Adams, G. S., & Inesi, M. E. (2016). Impediments to forgiveness: Victim and transgressor attributions of intent and guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(6), 866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Agerström, J., Björklund, F., & Carlsson, R. (2013). Look at yourself! Visual perspective influences moral judgment by level of mental construal. Social Psychology, 44(1), 42–46. [Google Scholar]
- Albrecht, K., Volz, K. G., Sutter, M., Laibson, D. I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2011). What is for me is not for you: Brain correlates of intertemporal choice for self and others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(2), 218–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Algoe, S. B., Kurtz, L. E., & Hilaire, N. M. (2016). Putting the “You” in “Thank You”: Examining other-praising behavior as the active relational ingredient in expressed gratitude. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(7), 658–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, R. A., Crockett, M. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2020). A theory of moral praise. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(9), 694–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aquino, K., Tripp, T. M., & Bies, R. J. (2001). How employees respond to personal offense: The effects of blame attribution, victim status, and offender status on revenge and reconciliation in the workplace. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(1), 52–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baumeister, R. F. (Ed.). (1999). The self in social psychology. Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beisswanger, A. H., Stone, E. R., Hupp, J. M., & Allgaier, L. (2003). Risk taking in relationships: Differences in deciding for oneself versus for a friend. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 25(2), 121–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blair, R. J. R. (2007). The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 387–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1990). Mood and persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 331–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bostyn, D. H., & Roets, A. (2016). The morality of action: The asymmetry between judgments of praise and blame in the action-omission effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 63, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buon, M., Jacob, P., Loissel, E., & Dupoux, E. (2013). A non-mentalistic cause-based heuristic in human social evaluations. Cognition, 126(2), 149–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carnes, N. C., Allmon, B., Alva, J., Cousar, K. A., & Varnam, Z. D. (2022). How morality signals, benefits, binds and teaches. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 101, 104313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1982). Toward understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behavior. Cognitive Social Psychology, 73, 108. [Google Scholar]
- Coates, D. J., & Tognazzini, N. A. (2012). The nature and ethics of blame. Philosophy Compass, 7(3), 197–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa-Lopes, R., Mata, A., & Mendonça, C. (2021). Real people or mere numbers? The influence of kill-save ratios and identifiability on moral judgments. International Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 378–395. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan, K., & Yazdanparast, A. (2019). Moral foundations and judgment: Conceptualizing boundaries. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 36(3), 356–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crockett, M. J. (2013). Models of morality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 363–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cumming, G. (2009). Inference by eye: Reading the overlap of independent confidence intervals. Statistics in Medicine, 28(2), 205–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Curry, O. S. (2016). Morality as cooperation: A problem-centred approach. In The evolution of morality (pp. 27–51). Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cushman, F. (2015). Deconstructing intent to reconstruct morality. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cushman, F., & Greene, J. D. (2012). Finding faults: How moral dilemmas illuminate cognitive structure. Social Neuroscience, 7(3), 269–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cushman, F., Sheketoff, R., Wharton, S., & Carey, S. (2013). The development of intent-based moral judgment. Cognition, 127(1), 6–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dreber, A., Rand, D. G., Fudenberg, D., & Nowak, M. A. (2008). Winners don’t punish. Nature, 452(7185), 348–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duan, L., Mo, S., Fan, C., & Liu, H. (2012). The role of mental states and event causation in moral judgment: A test of the dual-processing theory of moral judgment. Psychological Journal, 44(12), 1607–1617. [Google Scholar]
- Fiedler, K. (1988). The dependence of the conjunction fallacy on subtle linguistic factors. Psychological Research, 50(2), 123–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gaboriaud, A., Gautheron, F., Quinton, J.-C., & Smeding, A. (2022). The effects of Intent, outcome, and causality on moral judgments and decision processes. Psychologica Belgica, 62(1), 218–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gan, T., Lu, X., Li, W., Gui, D., Tang, H., Mai, X., Liu, C., & Luo, Y.-J. (2016). Temporal dynamics of the integration of intention and outcome in harmful and helpful moral judgment. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gan, T., Shi, R., Liu, C., & Luo, Y. (2018). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of the right temporoparietal joint area on the processing of helping intention. Psychological Journal, 50(1), 36–46. [Google Scholar]
- Gold, N., Pulford, B. D., & Colman, A. M. (2013). Your money or your life: Comparing judgements in trolley problems involving economic and emotional harms, injury and death. Economics & Philosophy, 29(2), 213–233. [Google Scholar]
- Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1996). Signal detection and psychophysics. Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(12), 517–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haidt, J. (2008). Morality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 65–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133(4), 55–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hauser, M. (2006). Moral minds: How nature designed our universal sense of right and wrong. Ecco/HarperCollins Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Hildebrand-Saints, L., & Weary, G. (1989). Depression and social information gathering. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15(2), 150–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffman, M. L. (2001). Toward a comprehensive empathy-based theory of prosocial moral development. In A. C. Bohart, & D. J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive & destructive behavior: Implications for family, school, & society (pp. 61–86). American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Huang, Y. X., & Luo, Y. J. (2006). Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 398(1–2), 91–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huebner, B., Dwyer, S., & Hauser, M. (2009). The role of emotion in moral psychology. Trends in cognitive sciences, 13(1), 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isen, A. M., & Means, B. (1983). The influence of positive affect on decision-making strategy. Social Cognition, 2(1), 18–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39(4), 341–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knobe, J. (2003). Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language. Analysis, 63(279), 190–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kurdi, B., Krosch, A. R., & Ferguson, M. J. (2020). Implicit evaluations of moral agents reflect intent and outcome. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 90, 103990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lammers, J. (2012). Abstraction increases hypocrisy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 475–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leight, K. A., & Ellis, H. C. (1981). Emotional mood states, strategies, and state-dependency in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20(3), 251–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levine, E. E., Bitterly, T. B., Cohen, T. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2018). Who is trustworthy? Predicting trustworthy intentions and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(3), 468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liu, Y. F., Bi, Y. F., & Wang, H. Y. (2010). The effects of emotion and task framing on risk preferences in self- and anticipatory-other decision making. Psychological Journal, 42(3), 317–324. [Google Scholar]
- Lowe, C. A., & Medway, F. J. (1976). Effects of valence, severity, and relevance on responsibility and dispositional attribution. Journal of Personality, 44(3), 518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malle, B. F. (2006). Intentionality, morality, and their relationship in human judgment. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 6(1–2), 87–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., & Monroe, A. E. (2014). A theory of blame. Psychological Inquiry, 25(2), 147–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., Voiklis, J., & Monroe, A. E. (2022). Cognitive blame is socially shaped. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(2), 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Malle, B. F., & Knobe, J. (1997). The folk concept of intentionality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(2), 101–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, J. W., Buon, M., & Cushman, F. (2021). The effect of cognitive load on intent-based moral judgment. Cognitive Science, 45(4), e12965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, J. W., & Cushman, F. (2016). Why we forgive what can’t be controlled. Cognition, 147, 133–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mata, A. (2019). Social metacognition in moral judgment: Decisional conflict promotes perspective taking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(6), 1061–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCullough, M. E., Fincham, F. D., & Tsang, J. A. (2003). Forgiveness, forbearance, and time: The temporal unfolding of transgression-related interpersonal motivations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(3), 540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCullough, M. E., Kurzban, R., & Tabak, B. A. (2013). Cognitive systems for revenge and forgiveness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 1–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McKenna, M. (2012). Conversation & responsibility. Oup Usa. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, S. L., Maner, J. K., & Becker, D. V. (2010). Self-protective biases in group categorization: Threat cues shape the psychological boundary between “us” and “them”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(1), 62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moran, J. M., Young, L. L., Saxe, R., Lee, S. M., O’Young, D., Mavros, P. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2688–2692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nobes, G., Panagiotaki, G., & Pawson, C. (2009). The influence of negligence, intention, and outcome on children’s moral judgments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104(4), 382–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ohtsubo, Y. (2007). Perceived intentionality intensifies blameworthiness of negative behaviors: Blame-praise asymmetry in intensification effect. Japanese Psychological Research, 49(2), 100–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peeters, G., & Czapinsky, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. European Review of Social Psychology, 1, 33–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pizarro, D. (2000). Nothing more than feelings? The role of emotions in moral judgment. Journal for the Theory Of Social Behaviour, 30(3). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pizarro, D., Uhlmann, E., & Salovey, P. (2003). Asymmetry in judgments of moral blame and praise: The role of perceived mental desires. Psychological Science, 14(3), 267–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Polman, E. (2012). Effects of self-other decision making on regulatory focus and choice overload. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(5), 980–993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rai, T. S., & Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review, 118(1), 57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robbennolt, J. K. (2000). Outcome severity and judgments of “responsibility”: A meta-analytic review 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(12), 2575–2609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 296–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schein, C., & Gray, K. (2018). The theory of dyadic morality: Reinventing moral judgment by redefining harm. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(1), 32–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schein, C., Jackson, J. C., Frasca, T., & Gray, K. (2020). Praise-many, blame-fewer: A common (and successful) strategy for attributing responsibility in groups. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(5), 855–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schleim, S., Spranger, T. M., Erk, S., & Walter, H. (2011). From moral to legal judgment: The influence of normative context in lawyers and other academics. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(4), 487–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, F., Djeriouat, H., & Trémolière, B. (2022). Judging accidental harm: Reasoning style modulates the weight of intention and harm severity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(12), 2366–2381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sinclair, R. C. (1988). Mood, categorization breadth, and performance appraisal: The effects of order of information acquisition and affective state on halo, accuracy, information retrieval, and evaluations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 42(1), 22–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Song, H. R., Chen, L. C., & Zhao, R. K. (1989). Ethics dictionary. Jilin People’s Publishing House. [Google Scholar]
- Sun, H. Y., Cui, L. Y., & Li, D. (2016). Negative discounting phenomenon: Self-other decision-making differences in intertemporal decision-making. Psychological Science, 39(4), 970–976. [Google Scholar]
- Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2000). Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 876–889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, T., Jin, S., Cheng, Z., Lou, Y., & Xie, X. (2024). Prediction bias in conspicuous altruism: Helpers underestimate social evaluations from bystanders. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(9), 1210–1224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, L., Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2010). Damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs judgment of harmful intent. Neuron, 65(6), 845–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(20), 8235–8240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, L., Scholz, J., & Saxe, R. (2011). Neural evidence for “intuitive prosecution”: The use of mental state information for negative moral verdicts. Social Neuroscience, 6(3), 302–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Li, Z.-M.; Xiao, L.; Sun, H.-Y. Which Matters More: Intention or Outcome? The Asymmetry of Moral Blame and Moral Praise. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1265. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091265
Li Z-M, Xiao L, Sun H-Y. Which Matters More: Intention or Outcome? The Asymmetry of Moral Blame and Moral Praise. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(9):1265. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091265
Chicago/Turabian StyleLi, Zhi-Meng, Lin Xiao, and Hong-Yue Sun. 2025. "Which Matters More: Intention or Outcome? The Asymmetry of Moral Blame and Moral Praise" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 9: 1265. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091265
APA StyleLi, Z.-M., Xiao, L., & Sun, H.-Y. (2025). Which Matters More: Intention or Outcome? The Asymmetry of Moral Blame and Moral Praise. Behavioral Sciences, 15(9), 1265. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091265