Does Growing up in Urban Compared to Rural Areas Shape Primary Emotional Traits?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Sample
2.2. Questionnaires
(Nyears(≥1,000,000; <5,000,000) × 4) + (Nyears(≥5,000,000) × 5)
2.3. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics
3.2. Effects of Age and Gender
3.3. Correlations between Urbanicity Index and Size of Birth-Town
3.4. Associations between the ANPS and Urbanicity Variables
3.5. Regression Analyses
3.6. Additional Analyses in a “Small” Chinese Sample Adjusted to the Urbanicity Index Categories in the German Sample
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- World Population. Available online: http://www.worldometers.info/world-population/ (accessed on 6 January 2017).
- Kennedy, D.P.; Adolphs, R. Social neuroscience: Stress and the city. Nature 2011, 474, 452–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lederbogen, F.; Kirsch, P.; Haddad, L.; Streit, F.; Tost, H.; Schuch, P.; Wüst, S.; Pruessner, J.C.; Rietschel, M.; Deuschle, M.; et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature 2011, 474, 498–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amato, P.R. Urban-rural differences in helping friends and family members. Soc. Psychol. Q. 1993, 56, 249–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuligni, A.J.; Zhang, W. Attitudes toward family obligation among adolescents in contemporary urban and rural China. Child Dev. 2004, 74, 180–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofferth, S.L.; Iceland, J. Social capital in rural and urban communities. Rural Sociol. 1998, 63, 574–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paykel, E.S.; Abbott, R.; Jenkins, R.; Brugha, T.S.; Meltzer, H. Urban-rural mental health differences in Great Britain: Findings from the National Morbidity Survey. Psychol. Med. 2000, 30, 269–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bernam, M.G.; Kross, E.; Krpan, K.M.; Askren, M.K.; Burson, A.; Deldin, P.J.; Kaplan, S.; Sherdell, L.; Gotlib, I.H.; Jonides, J. Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2012, 140, 300–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Vries, S.; Verheij, R.A.; Groenewegen, P.P.; Spreeuwenberg, P. Natural environments—Healthy environments? An exploratory analysis of the relationship between greenspace and health. Environ. Plan. 2003, 35, 1717–1731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Srinivasan, S.; O’Fallon, L.R.; Dearry, A. Creating healthy communities, healthy homes, healthy people: Initiating a research agenda on the built environment and public health. Rev. Evid. 2003, 93, 1446–1450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peen, J.; Dekker, J. Is urbanicity an environmental risk-factor for psychiatric disorders? Lancet 2004, 363, 2012–2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pedersen, C.B.; Mortensen, P.B. Evidence of a dose-response relationship between urbanicity during upbringing and schizophrenia risk. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2001, 58, 1039–1046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sundquist, K.; Frank, G.; Sundquist, J. Urbanisation and incidence of psychosis and depression. Br. J. Psychiatry 2004, 184, 293–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vassos, E.; Pedersen, C.B.; Murray, R.M.; Collier, D.A.; Lewis, C.M. Meta-analysis of the association of urbanicity with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2012, 38, 1118–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, J.L. Rural—Urban differences in the prevalence of major depression and associated impairment. Soc. Psychaitry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2004, 39, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Panksepp, J. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2004; ISBN 10-0-19-509673-8. [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp, J. Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e21236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Panksepp, J. Emotional endophenotypes in evolutionary psychiatry. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. 2006, 30, 774–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montag, C.; Widenhorn-Müller, K.; Panksepp, J.; Kiefer, M. Individual differences in Affective Neuroscience Personality Scale (ANPS) primary emotional traits and depressive tendencies. Compr Psychiatry 2016, 73, 136–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montag, C.; Hahn, E.; Reuter, M.; Spinath, F.M.; Davis, K.; Panksepp, J. The role of nature and nurture for individual differences in primary emotional systems: Evidence from a twin study. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0151405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montag, C.; Panksepp, J. Personality Neuroscience: Why it is of importance to consider primary emotional systems! In Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences, 1st ed.; Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T.K., Eds.; Springer: Heidelberg, Germany, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pechtel, P.; Pizzagalli, D.A. Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: An integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology 2011, 214, 55–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montag, C.; Panksepp, J. Primary emotional systems and personality: An evolutionary perspective. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montag, C. The brain derived neurotrophic factor and personality. Adv. Biol. 2014, 2014, 719723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, K.L.; Panksepp, J.; Normansell, L. The affective neuroscience personality scales: Normative data and implications. Neuropsychoanalysis 2003, 5, 57–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, K.L.; Panksepp, J. The brain’s emotional foundations of human personality and the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 35, 1946–1958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reuter, M.; Panksepp, J.; Davis, K.; Montag, C. Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS)—Deutsche Version, 1st ed.; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger, C.R.; Svrakic, D.M.; Przybeck, T.R. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1993, 50, 975–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mortensen, P.B.; Pedersen, C.B.; Westgaard, T.; Wohlfahrt, J.; Ewald, H.; Mors, O.; Andersen, P.K.; Melbye, M. Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia. New Engand. J. Med. 1999, 340, 603–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neff, J.A. Urbanicity and depression reconsidered: The evidence regarding depressive symptomatology. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1983, 171, 546–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reiss, D.; Leve, L.D.; Neiderhiser, J.M. How genes and the social environment moderate each other. Framing Health Matters 2013, 103 (Suppl. 1), S111–S121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Guillin, O.; Abi-Dargham, A.; Laruelle, M. Neurobiology of dopamine in schizophrenia. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 2007, 78, 1–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tang, Y.L.; Gillespie, C.F.; Epstein, M.P.; Mao, P.X.; Jiang, F.; Chen, Q.; Cai, Z.J.; Mitchell, P.B. Gender differences in 542 Chinese inpatients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2007, 97, 88–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Os, J.; Hanssen, M.; Bijl, R.V.; Vollebergh, W. Prevalence of psychotic disorder and community level of psychotic symptoms: An urban-rural comparison. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2001, 58, 663–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olaru, G.; Witthöft, M.; Wilhelm, O. Methods matter: Testing competing models for designing short-scale big-five assessments. J. Res. Personal. 2015, 59, 56–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pellegrini, A.D. Elementary-school children’s rough-and-tumble play and social competence. Dev. Psychol. 1988, 24, 802–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caspi, A.; Roberts, B.W. Personality development across the life course: The argument for change and continuity. Psychol. Inq. 2001, 12, 49–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edmonds, G.W.; Jackson, J.J.; Fayard, J.V.; Roberts, B.W. Is character fate, or is there hope to change my personality yet? Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2008, 2, 399–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krabbendamm, L.; van Os, J. Schizophrenia and Urbanicity: A Major Environmental Influence—Conditional on Genetic Risk. Schizophr. Bull. 2005, 31, 795–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maas, J.; Verheij, R.A.; Groeneweg, P.P.; de Vries, S.; Spreeuwenberg, P. Green space, urbanity, and health: How strong is the relation? J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 2006, 60, 587–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Primary Emotional System (PES) | Evolutionary Function |
---|---|
SEEKING | The SEEKING system provides mammals with psychological “energy” (i.e., enthusiasm) to explore the environment. This is necessary to find mating partners as well as food to nourish both the brain and body. |
LUST | LUST activity is of importance for attraction to the opposite sex and transfer one’s own genome (hence also of species homo sapiens) in terms of offspring. |
CARE | Humans are social mammals. In social groups survival chances are higher. Moreover, taking CARE of one’s own offspring helps assure that the young children grow into adults and themselves can have families. |
PLAY | PLAY behavior is of importance to learn social competencies and motoric skills. This aids living successfully in complex social groups as an adult. |
FEAR | Without a FEAR response (along with the learning it promotes) homo sapiens would not have optimal abilities to escape and avoid dangerous situations and to carefully monitor the safety of their environments. |
ANGER | Activity of the ANGER system is observed when mammals are in need of defending themselves (when a predator is closing in), but also in situations of frustration, when an expected reward is absent. ANGER activity is also displayed in the context of resolving territorial conflicts. |
SADNESS | PANIC/SADNESS reflects separation distress and signals a situation of having lost contact with an important person or being lost in the environment. As homo sapiens is a social animal, separation from a caregiver or another important person triggers a distress reaction leading to distress vocalization (crying) to reunite with a partner or a parent. Ultimately, as with CARE, homo sapiens is stronger in groups than when alone. CARE activities can also counteract and downregulate SADNESS arousal. |
ANPS Scale | German Sample | Chinese Sample |
---|---|---|
SEEKING | 0.70 | 0.69 |
FEAR | 0.86 | 0.81 |
CARE | 0.76 | 0.73 |
ANGER | 0.85 | 0.80 |
PLAY | 0.78 | 0.71 |
SADNESS | 0.75 | 0.74 |
Spirituality | 0.88 | 0.70 |
ANPS Scale/Urbanicity Index | German Sample (n = 324) | Chinese Sample (n = 713) |
---|---|---|
SEEKING | 40.17 (4.41) | 39.33 (4.13) |
[25; 52] | [23; 53] | |
FEAR | 36.14 (6.58) | 35.80 (5.41) |
[18; 53] | [16; 56] | |
CARE | 41.81 (5.55) | 38.98 (4.99) |
[24; 54] | [18; 53] | |
ANGER | 35.83 (6.56) | 35.41 (5.59) |
[17; 56] | [17; 56] | |
PLAY | 42.59 (5.43) | 39.02 (4.62) |
[24; 56] | [23; 52] | |
SADNESS | 34.19 (5.26) | 36.46 (5.03) |
[20; 54] | [24; 52] | |
Spirituality | 26.51 (7.01) | 34.19 (3.95) |
[13; 47] | [20; 47] | |
Urbanicity index | 25.85 (11.42) | 47.87 (21.22) |
[15; 45] | [15; 75] |
German Sample | Chinese Sample | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total (n = 324) | Males (n = 111) | Females (n = 213) | Total (n = 713) | Males (n = 434) | Females (n = 279) | |
SEEKING | 0.01 | 0.03 | (−)0.00 | 0.03 | 0.05 | −0.01 |
[−0.10; 0.10] | [−0.14; 0.20] | [−0.13; 0.13] | [−0.04; 0.10] | [−0.04; 0.13] | [−0.12; 0.10] | |
FEAR | −0.05 | −0.03 | −0.09 | −0.10 ** | −0.05 | −0.18 ** |
[−0.16; 0.05] | [−0.23; 0.17] | [−0.21; 0.04] | [−0.17; −0.02] | [−0.14; 0.05] | [−0.31; −0.05] | |
CARE | 0.08 | 0.12 | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 0.06 |
[−0.02; 0.18] | [−0.08; 0.32] | [−0.10; 0.16] | [−0.03; 0.11] | [−0.07; 0.12] | [−0.06; 0.18] | |
ANGER | 0.02 | 0.00 | 0.03 | −0.06 | −0.05 | −0.08 |
[−0.08; 0.12] | [−0.17; 0.18] | [−0.11; 0.17] | [−0.13; 0.02] | [−0.14; 0.05] | [−0.20; 0.04] | |
PLAY | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.10 ** | 0.13 ** | 0.06 |
[−0.10; 0.11] | [−0.19; 0.20] | [−0.10; 0.14] | [0.03; 0.17] | [0.02; 0.22] | [−0.04; 0.16] | |
SADNESS | −0.01 | (−)0.00 | −0.04 | −0.05 | −0.01 | −0.14 * |
[−0.12; 0.09] | [−0.20; 0.17] | [−0.17; 0.10] | [−0.13; 0.02] | [−0.10; 0.07] | [−0.26; −0.02] | |
Spirituality | 0.05 | 0.10 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.01 | −0.02 |
[−0.06; 0.15] | [−0.09; 0.29] | [−0.11; 0.13] | [−0.07; 0.07] | [−0.08; 0.10] | [−0.11; 0.08] |
Dependent Variable | Predictors | German Sample | Chinese Sample |
---|---|---|---|
FEAR | Gender | β = 0.313, t = 2.37, p = 0.019 | β = 0.345, t = 3.73, p < 0.001 |
Urbanicity index | β = −0.053, t = −0.57, p = 0.567 | β = −0.034, t = −0.72, p = 0.475 | |
Gender × Urbanicity index | β = −0.073, t = −0.47, p = 0.637 | β = −0.216, t = −2.16, p = 0.031 | |
PLAY | Gender | β = 0.011, t = 0.08, p = 0.936 | β = −0.039, t = −0.42, p = 0.677 |
Urbanicity index | β = −0.024, t = −0.25, p = 0.800 | β = 0.149, t = 3.08, p = 0.002 | |
Gender × Urbanicity index | β = −0.014, t = −0.09, p = 0.930 | β = −0.071, t = −0.71, p = 0.480 | |
SADNESS | Gender | β = 0.314, t = 2.37, p = 0.018 | β = 0.411, t = 4.50, p < 0.001 |
Urbanicity index | β = −0.017, t = −0.18, p = 0.858 | β = 0.036, t = 0.76, p = 0.449 | |
Gender × Urbanicity index | β = −0.057, t = −0.37, p = 0.713 | β = −0.206, t = −2.08, p = 0.038 |
Chinese “Small” Sample | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Correlations | ||||
M (SD) | Total (n = 290) | Males (n = 189) | Females (n = 101) | |
Urbanicity index | 28.51 (11.44) | |||
SEEKING | 39.18 (3.71) | 0.09 | 0.03 | 0.21 * |
[−0.02; 0.20] | [−0.12; 0.18] | [0.01; 0.38] | ||
FEAR | 36.36 (5.40) | −0.02 | 0.07 | −0.13 |
[−0.13; 0.10] | [−0.08; 0.20] | [−0.33; 0.11] | ||
CARE | 38.66 (4.39) | 0.03 | 0.10 | −0.11 |
[−0.08; 0.15] | [−0.03; 0.22] | [−0.29; 0.07] | ||
ANGER | 35.65 (4.99) | 0.00 | 0.11 | −0.17 |
[−0.11; 0.12] | [−0.04; 0.27] | [−0.37; 0.03] | ||
PLAY | 38.46 (4.23) | 0.09 | 0.12 | 0.02 |
[−0.03; 0.20] | [−0.02; 0.26] | [−0.19; 0.24] | ||
SADNESS | 36.29 (4.96) | 0.06 | 0.14 | −0.07 |
[−0.05; 0.17] | [−0.01; 0.30] | [−0.25; 0.13] | ||
Spirituality | 33.95 (3.71) | −0.04 | −0.05 | −0.02 |
[−0.16; 0.08] | [−0.20; 0.10] | [−0.23; 0.18] |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sindermann, C.; Kendrick, K.M.; Becker, B.; Li, M.; Li, S.; Montag, C. Does Growing up in Urban Compared to Rural Areas Shape Primary Emotional Traits? Behav. Sci. 2017, 7, 60. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7030060
Sindermann C, Kendrick KM, Becker B, Li M, Li S, Montag C. Does Growing up in Urban Compared to Rural Areas Shape Primary Emotional Traits? Behavioral Sciences. 2017; 7(3):60. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7030060
Chicago/Turabian StyleSindermann, Cornelia, Keith M. Kendrick, Benjamin Becker, Mei Li, Shijia Li, and Christian Montag. 2017. "Does Growing up in Urban Compared to Rural Areas Shape Primary Emotional Traits?" Behavioral Sciences 7, no. 3: 60. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7030060
APA StyleSindermann, C., Kendrick, K. M., Becker, B., Li, M., Li, S., & Montag, C. (2017). Does Growing up in Urban Compared to Rural Areas Shape Primary Emotional Traits? Behavioral Sciences, 7(3), 60. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7030060