Psychology of the Embrace: How Body Rhythms Communicate the Need to Indulge or Separate
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Bidirectionality of Movement and Affective-Cognitive System
1.2. Dynamic and Interactive Effects of Body Feedback: Including Kinaesthetic and Haptic Communication
1.3. Primacy of Movement and Expression: Roots of Rhythms Research in “Ausdruckspsychologie”
1.4. Basic Movement Dimensions: Tension-Flow Rhythms and Shape-Flow Rhythms in the KMP
1.4.1. Tension-Flow Rhythms
1.4.2. Shape-Flow Rhythms
1.5. From Individual to Interpersonal Body Feedback: An Embodied Approach to Embraces
1.6. The Influence of Tactile Feedback in Interpersonal Communication
1.6.1 Preliminary Study 1: What do Handshakes Communicate? (Koch, Berger, & Schorr; in Koch, 2011) [23]
1.6.2 Preliminary Study 2: Embraces in Natural Contexts (Koch, Skibka, Steiner, & Grassinger, 2011) [61]
1.6.3 Aim of this Study
1.7. Hypothesis and Operationalizations
2. Methods
2.1. Study Design
2.2. Sample
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Materials & Instruments
2.4.1. Time of Signal (Duration up to the Drop of the Handkerchief)
2.4.2. Affect Measure (MBAS-Questionnaire; Koch, 2014) [24]
2.4.3. Experienced Naturalness of the Embrace (Stimmigkeitsskala; Rautner, 2012) [74]
2.4.4. Need-for-Interpersonal-Touch-Scale (NFIPT; Nuszbaum et al., 2010; 2014) [68,75]
2.4.5. Other Variables
2.4.6. Manipulation-Check
2.5. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics
3.2. Inferential Statistics
3.2.1. Phase of Signal: In Which Phase Was the Handkerchief Most Frequently Dropped?
3.2.2. Main Analysis
3.3. Manipulation Check
4. Discussion
4.1. Effects of Movement Rhythms in Embraces
4.2. Limitations and Future Directions
- (a)
- In a natural embrace, the sharp phase would not continue for 10 s but just until the point that the embracers actually separate, which is quite soon after the onset of a sharp rhythm. In our study, the drop of the handkerchief happened with a 6–7 s delay in sequence B (where the sharp rhythm was the second rhythm to be applied), whereas it happened with a delay of 3–4 s in sequence A (where the sharp rhythm was the third rhythm applied). This difference may be due to the fact that in Sequence A the embrace had already lasted a lot longer than in Sequence B. We would thus assume that a delay of 3–4 s reflects the more natural conditions of a separation after an indulgent embrace;
- (b)
- In natural settings, shape changes almost always accompany the tension-flow changes featured in this study, that is, embracers may retreat their upper bodies or even start to take a step back right after the onset of the sharp rhythm. So in natural conditions, tension-flow and shape- flow go together (they systematically vary together), while in our experiment only tension-flow was varied in order to isolate the effect of smooth vs. sharp rhythms. Again, this increased the artificiality of the embrace, increasing internal validity at the expense of decreasing external validity.
4.3. Conclusions and Implications for Future Research
Supplementary Materials
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
NA/B | MA/B | SDA/B | F | p | eta2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Signal I (preliminary trail) | 48 (26/22) | 21.31 vs. 15.73 | 4.28 vs. 5.48 | 17.70 | 0.000 | 0.29 |
Signal II (main trail) | 51 (29/22) | 23.72 vs. 18.09 | 4.39 vs. 6.05 | 14.06 | 0.000 | 0.23 |
Signal III (n = 8 from I, and n = 52 from II) | 60 (31/29) | 23.13 vs. 17.17 | 4.85 vs. 5.72 | 18.53 | 0.000 | 0.25 |
Appendix B
References
- Allesch, C.G. Einführung in die Psychologische Ästhetik (Introduction to Psychological Aesthetics); WUV: Vienna, Austria, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Aronoff, J.; Woike, B.A.; Hyman, L.M. Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configurational bases of emotion recognition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1992, 62, 1050–1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 191–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bar, M.; Neta, M. Humans prefer curved visual objects. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 645–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bargh, J.A.; Chen, M.; Burrows, L. Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 71, 230–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bartenieff, I.; Lewis, D. Body Movement. Coping with the Environment; Gordon & Breach: New York, NY, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Bortz, J.; Döring, N. Forschungsmethoden und Evaluation; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2013/1995. [Google Scholar]
- Bruner, J.S. Acts of Meaning; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Bruner, J.S.; Postman, L. Tension and tension release as organizing factors in perception. J. Personal. 1947, 15, 300–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burgoon, J.K.; Guerrero, L.K.; Floyd, K. Nonverbal Communication; Routledge: London, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo, J.T.; Priester, J.R.; Berntson, G. Rudimentary determinants of attitudes II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 65, 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, J. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 112, 155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Damasio, A.R. Descartes’ Error. In Emotion, Rationality and the Human Brain; Putnam: New York, NY, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. The Expression of Emotions in Men and Animals; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1965/1872. [Google Scholar]
- Dennett, D. Are we explaining consciousness yet? Cognition 2001, 79, 221–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DePaulo, B.M. Nonverbal behavior and self-presentation. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 111, 203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Erceau, D.; Guéguen, N. Tactile contact and evaluation of the toucher. J. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 147, 441–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Erdfelder, E.; Faul, F.; Buchner, A. G*POWER: A general power analysis program. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1996, 28, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erikson, E.H. Childhood and Society; Norton: New York, NY, USA, 1963. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher, J.D.; Rytting, M.; Heslin, R. Hands touching hands: Affective and evaluative effects of an interpersonal touch. Sociometry 1976, 39, 416–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fischman, D. Therapeutic relationships and kinesthetic empathy. In The Art and Science of Dance/Movement Therapy: Life Is Dance; Wengrower, H., Chaiklin, S., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2009; pp. 33–53. [Google Scholar]
- Freedberg, D.; Gallese, V. Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience. Trends Cognit. Sci. 2007, 11, 197–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Freud, A. The Ego and the Mechanisms of Defence; Karnac Books: London, UK, 1992/1937. [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs, T. Leib Raum Person; Klett-Cotta: Stuttgart, Germany, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs, T.; DeJaegher, H. Enactive intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenol. Cognit. Sci. 2009, 8, 465–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuchs, T.; Koch, S.C. Embodied Affectivity: On moving and being moved. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gallace, A.; Spence, C. The cognitive and neural correlates of tactile memory. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 380–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gallace, A.; Spence, C. The science of interpersonal touch: An overview. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 34, 246–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gallagher, S. How the Body Shapes the Mind; Clarendon Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs, R.W. Embodiment and Cognitive Science; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Greenbaum, P.E.; Rosenfeld, H.W. Varieties of touching in greeting: Sequential structure and sex-related differences. J. Nonverbal Behav. 1980, 5, 13–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, J.A. Nonverbal Sex Differences. In Accuracy of Communication and Expressive Style; Johns Hopkins University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Hall, J.A.; Veccia, E.M. More “touching” observations: New insights on men, women, and interpersonal touch. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 59, 1155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Havas, D.A.; Glenberg, A.M.; Gutowski, K.A.; Lucarelli, M.J.; Davidson, R.J. Cosmetic use of botulinum toxin-A affects processing of emotional language. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 895–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Henley, N.M. The politics of touch. In Radical Psychology; Brown, P., Ed.; Tavistock: London, UK, 1973; pp. 420–433. [Google Scholar]
- Hertenstein, M.J.; Keltner, D. Gender and the communication of emotion via touch. Sex Roles 2011, 64, 70–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heslin, R.; Boss, D. Nonverbal intimacy in airport arrival and departure. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1980, 6, 248–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, M. The meaning of the body. In Aesthetics of Human Understanding; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy, A. Bewegtes Wissen: Laban Bartenieff-Bewegungsstudien Verstehen und Erleben; Logos: Berlin, Germany, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Kestenberg, J.S. Sexuality, Body Movement, and the Rhythms of Development; Jason Aronson: Northvale, NJ, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Kestenberg, J.S.; Sossin, K.M. Developmental Patterns in Movement I + II; Dance Notation Bureau: New York, NY, USA, 1973/1979. [Google Scholar]
- Kestenberg-Amighi, J.; Loman, S.; Lewis, P.; Sossin, K.M. The Meaning of Movement: Developmental and Clinical Perspectives of the Kestenberg Movement Profile; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Keysers, C. The Empathic Brain: How the Discovery of Mirror Neurons Changes Our Understanding of Human Nature; Kindle e-Book. 2011. Available online: https://books.google.com.hk/books?id=Ag9cAwAAQBAJ&printsec=frontcover&dq=The+Empathic+Brain:+How+the+Discovery+of+Mirror+Neurons+Changes+Our+Understanding+of+Human+Nature&hl=zh-TW&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwjv4bSC4uLXAhWEm5QKHVoDCOgQ6AEIJDAA#v=onepage&q=The%20Empathic%20Brain%3A%20How%20the%20Discovery%20of%20Mirror%20Neurons%20Changes%20Our%20Understanding%20of%20Human%20Nature&f=false (accessed on 27 November 2017).
- Kirchhoff, R. Handbuch Psychologie; Verlag für Psychologie: Göttingen, Germany, 1965; Volume 5. [Google Scholar]
- Koch, S.C. Arts and Health. Active factors and a theory framework of embodied aesthetics. Arts Psychother. 2017, 54, 85–91. [Google Scholar]
- Koch, S.C. Rhythm is it: Effects of dynamic body feedback on affect and attitudes. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koch, S.C. Basic Body Rhythms and Embodied Intercorporality: From Individual to Interpersonal Movement Feedback. In The Implications of Embodiment: Cognition and Communication; Tschacher, W., Bergomi, C., Eds.; Imprint Academic: Exeter, UK, 2011; pp. 151–171. [Google Scholar]
- Koch, S.C. The Kestenberg Movement Profile (KMP)—Reliability among Novice Raters; Klett-Cotta: Stuttgart, Germany, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Koch, S.C.; Fuchs, T. Embodied Arts Therapies. Arts Psychother. 2011, 38, 276–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koch, S.C.; Cruz, R.F.; Goodill, S.W. The Kestenberg Movement Profile: Performance of novice raters. Am. J. Dance Ther. 2002, 23, 71–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koch, S.C.; Martin, L.; Schubert, A.; Fuchs, T.; Mombauer, K. The rhythmic self: A body-based developmental model of the minimal self. In Proceedings of the Poster at DFG-Workshop “The Active Self”, Fulda, Germany, 27–28 July 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Koch, S.C.; Müller, S.M. The KMP-questionnaire and the brief KMP-based affect scale. In Movement Analysis—Bewegungsanalyse. The Legacy of Laban, Bartenieff, Lamb and Kestenberg; Koch, S.C., Bender, S., Eds.; Logos: Berlin, Germany, 2007; pp. 195–202. [Google Scholar]
- Koch, S.C.; Skibka, K.; Steiner, M.; Grassinger, U. Observation of Embraces and Their Phases in Public Places. Heidelberg, Germany, Unpublished Data. 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Koch, S.C.; Sossin, K.M. Kestenberg Movement Analysis. In Handbook of Language, Communication and Gesture; Mueller, C., Fricke, E., Cienki, A., McNeill, D., Eds.; DeGruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2012; Chapter 65. [Google Scholar]
- Köhler, W. Gestalt Psychology; Liveright: New York, NY, USA, 1929. [Google Scholar]
- Laban, R.V. The Mastery of Movement, 3rd ed.; MacDonald & Evans: London, UK, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- LaFrance, M. Postural mirroring and intergroup relations. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1985, 11, 207–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laird, J.D. The real role of facial response in the experience of emotion: A response to Tourangeau and Ellsworth, and others. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 47, 909–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lamb, W. Posture and Gesture; Duckworth & Co.: London, UK, 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Lipps, T. Empathy, inner imitation and organ sensation. Arch. Gesamte Psychol. 1903, 1, 185–204. [Google Scholar]
- Loman, S. Fetal Movement Notation: A method of attuning to the fetus. In The Body-Mind-Connection in Human Movement Analysis; Loman, S., Brandt, R., Eds.; Antioch New England Graduate School: Keene, NH, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- McDaniel, E.; Andersen, P.A. International patterns of interpersonal tactile communication: A field study. J. Nonverbal Behav. 1998, 22, 59–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merleau-Ponty, M. Die Phänomenologie der Wahrnehmung; DeGruyter: Berlin, Germany, 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Montagu, A. Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Neumann, R.; Strack, F. Approach and avoidance: The influence of proprioceptive and exteroceptive cues on encoding of affective information. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 79, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Niedenthal, P.M. Embodying emotion. Science 2007, 316, 1002–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Niedenthal, P.M.; Barsalou, L.W.; Winkielman, P.; Krauth-Gruber, S.; Ric, F. Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 9, 184–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nuszbaum, M.; Voss, A.; Klauer, C.K.; Betsch, T. Assessing individual differences in the use of haptic information using a German translation of the Need for Touch Scale. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 41, 263–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nuszbaum, M.; Voss, A.; Klauer, K.C. Assessing individual differences in the need for interpersonal touch and need for touch. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 45, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oseretzky, N.I. Methodik der kollektiven Prüfung der Motorik bei Kindern und Minderjährigen (Method of the collective assessment of motor behavior in children and adolescents). MIT 1929, 6, 332–372. [Google Scholar]
- Pert, C.B. Molecules of Emotion: Why You Feel the Way You Feel; Simon and Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Pfeifer, R.; Bongard, J.C. How the Body Shapes the Way We Think; A New View of Intelligence; MIT Press: Cambridge, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Poffenberger, A.T.; Barrows, B.E. The Feeling Value of Lines. J. Appl. Psychol. 1924, 8, 187–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rautner, H. Was Rhythmen kommunizieren. Zusammenhänge zwischen Rhythmus, Stimmung, Need for Separation and Need for Interpersonal Closeness. Bachelor’s Thesis, Heidelberg, Germany, Unpublished work. 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Remland, M.S.; Jones, T.S. Cultural and sex differences in touch avoidance. Percept. Motor Skills 1988, 67, 544–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Riskind, J.H. They stoop to conquer: Guiding and self-regulatory functions of physical posture after success and failure. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 47, 479–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rossberg-Gempton, I.; Poole, G.D. The relationship between body movement and affect: From historical and current perspectives. Arts Psychother. 1992, 19, 39–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schönhammer, R. Einführung in Die Wahrnehmungspsychologie (Introduction to Psychology of Perception); UTB: Frankfurt, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Schubert, T.W.; Waldzus, S.; Seibt, B. The embodiment of power and communalism in space and bodily contact. In Embodied Grounding: Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific Approaches; Semin, G.R., Smith, E.R., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 160–183. [Google Scholar]
- Seibt, B.; Neumann, R.; Nussinson, R.; Strack, F. Movement direction or change in distance? Self- and object-related approach–avoidance motions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 713–720. [Google Scholar]
- Shai, D.; Belsky, J. When words just won’t do: Introducing parental embodied mentalizing. Child Dev. Perspect. 2011, 5, 173–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shai, D.; Belsky, J. Parental embodied mentalizing: Let’s be explicit about what we mean by implicit. Child Dev. Perspect. 2011, 5, 187–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sheets-Johnstone, M. The Primacy of Movement; John Benjamin: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, D.N. Forms of Vitality: Exploring Dynamic Experience in Psychology, the Arts, Psychotherapy, and Development; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, D.N. The interpersonal world of the infant. In A View from Psycho Analysis and Developmental Psychology; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Stier, D.S.; Hall, J.A. Gender differences in touch: An empirical and theoretical review. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 47, 440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trevarthen, C. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before Speech: The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1979; pp. 530–571. [Google Scholar]
- Trevarthen, C. Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Music. Sci. 1999, 3 (Suppl. 1), 155–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trevarthen, C.; Delafield-Butt, J. Biology of shared experience and language development: Regulations for the intersubjective life of narratives. In The Infant Mind: Origins of the Social Brain; Legerstee, M., Haley, D.W., Bornstein, M.H., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Wallbott, H.G. Bewegungsstil und Bewegungsqualität: Untersuchungen zum Ausdruck und Eindruck Gestischen Verhaltens (Movement Style and Movement Quality); Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Wallbott, H.G. Mimik im Kontext: Die Bedeutung verschiedener Informationskomponenten für das Erkennen von Emotionen (Facial Expression in Context of Emotion Recognition); Göttingen: Hogrefe, Germany, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Winnicott, D.W. The theory of the parent-infant relationship. Int. J. Psychoanal. 1960, 41, 585–605. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Zajonc, R.B.; Markus, H. Affect and cognition: The hard interface. In Emotions, Cognition and Behavior; Izard, C., Kagan, J., Zajonc, R.B., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1984; pp. 73–102. [Google Scholar]
Sequence | Mean | SD | N | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Time of Signal | A: sm-sm-sh | 23.13 | 4.85 | 31 |
B: sm-sh-sm | 17.17 | 5.72 | 29 | |
Phase of Signal | A: sm-sm-sh | 2.74 | 0.51 | 31 |
B: sm-sh-sm | 2.24 | 0.43 | 29 | |
Affect Change | A: sm-sm-sh | 0.28 | 0.52 | 31 |
B: sm-sh-sm | 0.28 | 0.57 | 29 |
Rhythms Sequence | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Sequence A | Sequence B | Total | ||
Phase of Signal | Phase 2 (s 10–20) | 6 | 22 | 28 |
Phase 3 (s 20–30) | 24 | 7 | 31 | |
Total | 30 | 29 | 59 |
Sequence | F | P | eta2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Time of Signal (drop) | A: sm-sm-sh | 18.90 | 0.000 | 0.25 |
B: sm-sh-sm | ||||
Affect Change | A: sm-sm-sh | 0.007 | 0.934 | 0.00 |
B: sm-sh-sm |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Koch, S.C.; Rautner, H. Psychology of the Embrace: How Body Rhythms Communicate the Need to Indulge or Separate. Behav. Sci. 2017, 7, 80. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040080
Koch SC, Rautner H. Psychology of the Embrace: How Body Rhythms Communicate the Need to Indulge or Separate. Behavioral Sciences. 2017; 7(4):80. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040080
Chicago/Turabian StyleKoch, Sabine C., and Helena Rautner. 2017. "Psychology of the Embrace: How Body Rhythms Communicate the Need to Indulge or Separate" Behavioral Sciences 7, no. 4: 80. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040080
APA StyleKoch, S. C., & Rautner, H. (2017). Psychology of the Embrace: How Body Rhythms Communicate the Need to Indulge or Separate. Behavioral Sciences, 7(4), 80. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7040080