Synchronization, Attention and Transformation: Multidimensional Exploration of the Aesthetic Experience of Contemporary Dance Spectators
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Slowness, Continuum and Duration: The Choreographies of Myriam Gourfink
2.1. Phenomenological Approach to Myriam Gourfink’s Work
2.2. The Making of Slow Movement: Yoga and Meditation or Attention and Respiration
3. The Shared Present: The Specious Present as a Window into Interpersonal Resonance
3.1. The Present Moment
3.2. Modulations of Our Perception in the Present Moment—Attention and Vigilance
3.3. Meditation and the Present Moment
4. The Shared Present and Interpersonal Resonance
Resonance with Gourfink’s choreographies
5. Methods
5.1. Spontaneous Tempo Production
5.2. Temporal Window of the Apparent Motion Illusion
5.3. Physiological Measurements
5.4. Subjective Reports
- Evaluation of the choreography/dance:Several studies in the field of neuroaesthetics, have shown a relation between the judgment of taste (I like/I do not like) regarding a dance sequence and the physiological response while spectating it [20]. Given these studies, and given the sometimes radical differences between spectators in the appreciation of the choreographies of Myriam Gourfink, it seemed to us essential to query their personal appreciation, through the following question: “I enjoyed the performance”. Alongside judgment of taste, we included other questions not probing the fine granularity of the spectators’ lived experience during the choreography, but rather their overall appreciation and understanding of the choreography. This section was constructed based on the cognitive interpretation section of the Audience Response Tool (ART) questionnaire ([26]). The ART questionnaire was created in order to evaluate the impact of choreographic intention, knowledge and dance expertise on psychological reactions to contemporary dance. According to the authors, cognitive interpretation involves “the recognition, attribution and understanding of character, qualities and meanings in a particular contemporary dance work”. We have extracted and modified some questions from their questionnaire, for instance “I found these performances intellectually stimulating” and added others such as “this piece tells a story”.
- Temporal modulations and qualities: rhythm, duration, speed, flow:As we discussed in the introduction, intersubjective temporality does not correspond so much to being in the same moment of objective time (clock time) but rather the shared experience of time and sharing temporal structure of action and affect [11,15,19,117,120,132]. We included questions that sought to explore the lived experience of temporality and rhythm for the audience. For example, through the following statements: “The movement of the dancers evoked water and waves”, “The rhythm of my body slowed down”, “I had the impression of going on a trip or a promenade during this performance”. Most of these declarations came from the input of spectators from precedent sessions.
- Attention: The spectator’s attention plays a preponderant role in the aesthetic experience [133] and in the experience of art, all artistic categories combined [134]. Nevertheless, each choreography solicits and directs the viewer’s attention differently [135], and the co-presence of the bodies specific to the live performance invites us to wonder if the singular presence of the dancer underpinned by his attention in the present moment [42] may inform that of the audience [136]. In this section, we looked at three different dimensions of attention: the degree of absorption in the choreography (3a), the directions of attention (3b), and the degree of “attention to attention”, in other terms, of meta-cognitive attention (3c). According to aesthetic theories, the degree of absorption in a work, regardless of the artistic category and whether it is a classical or contemporary work, is one of the first factors of the aesthetic experience and the resonance with the work [31,137]. As M. Massin says in her analysis of aesthetic experience in relation to contemporary art, we do not have such an experience without fully engaging with the work [134]. The two others dimensions are specifically related to the choreographies of Myriam Gourfink and to the meditative state of the dancers. One of the effects of the choreographies of Myriam Gourfink, reported by all the phenomenological studies, is that spectators’ attention travels “from the most intimate corners of their bodies, revealed by the dance, to the performers’ uninterrupted movements” ([125], p. 172). In addition, we know that inward directed attention (internal focus) is increased for the dancer and that they pay attention to their breathing during all the choreography. That is why we wanted to explore the directions of attention of spectators, especially towards their own body (internal focus) and/or towards the body of dancers (external focus). Thirdly, the attention to one’s own attention (meta-cognitive attention or meta-awareness [52]) is a constituent element of the meditative state and has been reported to arise, for some spectators, during the observation of Myriam Gourfink’s choreographies.
- (a)
- Degree of absorption:Absorption is a form of attentional modulation, a form of concentration during which the external world is momentarily withdrawn [105], independent of the content of absorption, as in rumination, mind wandering, “flow” [138], hypnosis, meditation, day dreaming or sleep [105]. Absorption is often characterized by “timelessness” and “self-forgetfulness” [139]. For instance, there can be a “forgetfulness” of the “muscular” body and body posture, as is the case when one is so immersed in a fiction, or a video game that one adopts a bad posture without noticing, with the muscular contraction becoming apparent only once out of absorption. In relation to the choreographies of Myriam Gourfink, in which the dancers are in a form of absorption, some spectators report being absorbed in the choreography while others do not manage to “enter the dance”. That is why we have tried to interrogate the degree of absorption of the spectators, but also the quality and modality of the absorption, given that attention can be decoupled from perceptual external input [140]. Items concerning absorption included: “I would be incapable to say if I was sitting comfortably or not”, “During the performance I was preoccupied with personal problems”, “I fell asleep during the performance”.
- (b)
- Direction and object of attention: Joint attention to an object, a stimulus, or a portion of the external environment is related to interpersonal resonance processes, and is a key step in the development of "social cognition" in children [141]. Regarding the attention to others, multiple studies have explored the impact of visual attention directed towards the other (their action [142], their face, their eyes [143], or their mouth [144]). Some recent studies have focused on mutual attention through mutual gaze and have shown that it impacts the neural synchronization between participants [6]. No study has investigated the effects of an individual’s internally oriented attention on the attention of an outside observer. However, Myriam Gourfink’s choreographies are based on extremely precise scores for the dancers’ outer movement but also on the circulation of their internal attention. The attention of the dancers is essentially directed towards several points of their body, as well as to their own breathing during the entire choreography. In this section, we wanted to explore the entrainment of the spectator’s attention by the attentive disposition of the dancer to bodily sensations. In particular, we wanted to explore whether the dancer’s breathing is an object of joint attention (“I was often attentive to the breathing of the dancers”), and/or, if there is an entrainment of the viewer’s attention by that of the dancer (“Many time I felt my own breathing”), and if so, how it relates to changes after spectating. Indeed, in our previous study we found that the strength of breathing entrainment between dancers and spectators during the choreography positively correlated with the responses post spectating regarding spectators’ attention both to the dancers’ breathing and their own breathing [37].
- (c)
- Extent of meta-cognitive attention: As we have described above, spectating Myriam Gourfink’s work can increase the spectator’s meta-cognitive attention such as paying attention to one’s own attention, which is a feature of the phenomenological attitude (epoché) and contemplative practices. For instance, all spectators experience moments of blindness to progressive and slow changes while spectating Myriam Gourfink’s choreography, but not all of them notice it. The questionnaire included items that explored the extent of spectators’ meta-cognitive attention during the performance such as: “Sometimes the movement was so slow that I did not see the change from one posture to another”.
6. Results
6.1. Cognitive Tasks
6.1.1. Spontaneous Tempo
6.1.2. Temporal Window of the Apparent Motion Illusion
6.1.3. Comparing the Two Tasks
6.2. Changes in Physiological Rhythms and Their Relations to Changes in Temporal Cognition
6.3. Subjective Reports and Changes in Temporal Cognition and Physiological Rhythms
7. Discussion
7.1. The Relationship Between Subjective Experience and Observed Cognitive and Physiological Changes
7.1.1. Evaluation of the Choreography
7.1.2. Temporal Modulations
7.1.3. Attention
7.2. Limits of the Current Study
8. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Labodanse questionnaire
- “The movement of the dancers evoked water and waves”
- “I felt excluded by the speed of the dancers, I could not get into the dance”
- “Sometimes the movement was so slow that I did not see the change from one posture to another”
- “Many times I felt my own breathing”
- “I fell asleep during the performance”
- “This piece tells a story”
- “I enjoyed the performance”
- “The length of each performance seemed shorter than 7 min”
- “I was often attentive to the breathing of the dancers”
- “I would be incapable to say if I was sitting comfortably or not”
- “The rhythm of my body slowed down”
- “On several occasions, my attention focused on the muscles or muscular tension of the dancers”
- “I found it was hard to concentrate on the performance”
- “I found this performance intellectually stimulating”
- “During the performance I was preoccupied with personal problems”
- “I had the impression that the dancers were suffering”
- “The dancers’ faces expressed a lot of emotions”
- “I had the impression to go on a trip or a promenade during this performance”
References
- Fuchs, T.; De Jaegher, H. Enactive intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2009, 8, 465–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mühlhoff, R. Affective resonance and social interaction. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2015, 14, 1001–1019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freedberg, D.; Gallese, V. Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 197–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Phillips-Silver, J.; Keller, P.E. Searching for roots of entrainment and joint action in early musical interactions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stupacher, J.; Hove, M.J.; Novembre, G.; Schütz-Bosbach, S.; Keller, P.E. Musical groove modulates motor cortex excitability: A TMS investigation. Brain Cogn. 2013, 82, 127–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Saito, D.N.; Tanabe, H.C.; Izuma, K.; Hayashi, M.J.; Morito, Y.; Komeda, H.; Uchiyama, H.; Kosaka, H.; Okazawa, H.; Fujibayashi, Y.; et al. “Stay tuned”: Inter-individual neural synchronization during mutual gaze and joint attention. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2010, 4, 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dumas, G.; Nadel, J.; Soussignan, R.; Martinerie, J.; Garnero, L. Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e12166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Richardson, M.J.; Marsh, K.L.; Isenhower, R.W.; Goodman, J.R.; Schmidt, R.C. Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2007, 26, 867–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nessler, J.A.; Gilliland, S.J. Interpersonal synchronization during side by side treadmill walking is influenced by leg length differential and altered sensory feedback. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2009, 28, 772–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Codrons, E.; Bernardi, N.F.; Vandoni, M.; Bernardi, L. Spontaneous group synchronization of movements and respiratory rhythms. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e107538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stern, D.N. The Present Moment in Psychotherapy and Everyday Life (Norton Series on Interpersonal Neurobiology); WW Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, D.N. Forms of Vitality: Exploring Dynamic Experience in Psychology, the Arts, Psychotherapy, and Development; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, D.N. Vitality contours: The temporal contour of feelings as a basic unit for constructing the infant’s social experience. In Early Social Cognition: Understanding Others in the First Months of Life; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1999; pp. 67–80. [Google Scholar]
- Froese, T.; Fuchs, T. The extended body: A case study in the neurophenomenology of social interaction. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2012, 11, 205–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuchs, T. Temporality and psychopathology. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2013, 12, 75–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sbriscia-Fioretti, B.; Berchio, C.; Freedberg, D.; Gallese, V.; Umiltà, M.A. ERP modulation during observation of abstract paintings by Franz Kline. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e75241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Umilta, M.A.; Berchio, C.; Sestito, M.; Freedberg, D.; Gallese, V. Abstract art and cortical motor activation: An EEG study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Repp, B.H. Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2005, 12, 969–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laroche, J.; Berardi, A.M.; Brangier, E. Embodiment of intersubjective time: Relational dynamics as attractors in the temporal coordination of interpersonal behaviors and experiences. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cross, E.S.; Kirsch, L.; Ticini, L.F.; Schütz-Bosbach, S. The impact of aesthetic evaluation and physical ability on dance perception. Fronti. Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bläsing, B.; Calvo-Merino, B.; Cross, E.S.; Jola, C.; Honisch, J.; Stevens, C.J. Neurocognitive control in dance perception and performance. Acta Psychol. 2012, 139, 300–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Samaritter, R.; Payne, H. Kinaesthetic intersubjectivity: A dance informed contribution to self-other relatedness and shared experience in non-verbal psychotherapy with an example from autism. Arts Psychother. 2013, 40, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Behrends, A.; Müller, S.; Dziobek, I. Moving in and out of synchrony: A concept for a new intervention fostering empathy through interactional movement and dance. Arts Psychother. 2012, 39, 107–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jola, C.; Grosbras, M.H. In the here and now: Enhanced motor corticospinal excitability in novices when watching live compared to video recorded dance. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013, 4, 90–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stevens, C.J.; Schubert, E.; Morris, R.H.; Frear, M.; Chen, J.; Healey, S.; Schoknecht, C.; Hansen, S. Cognition and the temporal arts: Investigating audience response to dance using PDAs that record continuous data during live performance. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2009, 67, 800–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schubert, E.; Vincs, K.; Stevens, C.J. Identifying regions of good agreement among responders in engagement with a piece of live dance. Empir. Stud. Arts 2013, 31, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Konvalinka, I.; Xygalatas, D.; Bulbulia, J.; Schjødt, U.; Jegindø, E.M.; Wallot, S.; Van Orden, G.; Roepstorff, A. Synchronized arousal between performers and related spectators in a fire-walking ritual. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 8514–8519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shaner, D.E. The Bodymind Experience in Japanese Buddhism: A phenomenological study of Kukai and Dogen; SUNY Press: Albany, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Calvo-Merino, B.; Glaser, D.E.; Grèzes, J.; Passingham, R.E.; Haggard, P. Action observation and acquired motor skills: An FMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb. Cortex 2004, 15, 1243–1249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goldberg, E.; Funk, B.A.; Podell, K. How the brain deals with novelty and ambiguity: Implications for neuroaesthetics. Rend. Lincei 2012, 23, 227–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dufrenne. Phénoménologie de L’expérience Esthétique; Epithémée, PUF: Paris, France, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Lengger, P.G.; Fischmeister, F.P.S.; Leder, H.; Bauer, H. Functional neuroanatomy of the perception of modern art: A DC–EEG study on the influence of stylistic information on aesthetic experience. Brain Res. 2007, 1158, 93–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Minissale, G. Conceptual art: A blind spot for neuroaesthetics? Leonardo 2012, 45, 43–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuchs, T.; Koch, S.C. Embodied affectivity: On moving and being moved. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gourfink, M. Dance, borrow, create? From the breath to ideas, from ideas to gestures. Int. J. Perform. Arts Digit. Media 2013, 9, 14–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joufflineau, C.; Bachrach, A. Spectating Myriam Gourfink’s Dances; Transdisciplinary Explorations. In Aesthetics and Neuroscience; Springer International Publishing: Zurich, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 93–116. [Google Scholar]
- Bachrach, A.; Fontbonne, Y.; Joufflineau, C.; Ulloa, J.L. Audience entrainment during live contemporary dance performance: Physiological and cognitive measures. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gourfink, M.G.P.S. Temps tiraillés et perceptions infimes. Geste 2009, 6, 126–138. [Google Scholar]
- Simons, D.J. Attentional capture and inattentional blindness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 147–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Auvray, M.; O’Regan, J.K. L’influence des facteurs sémantiques sur la cécité aux changements progressifs dans les scènes visuelles. L’année Psychol. 2003, 103, 9–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rensink, R.A.; O’Regan, J.K.; Clark, J.J. To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychol. Sci. 1997, 8, 368–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Louppe, L. Poétique de la Danse Contemporaine; Contredanse: Bruxelles, Belgium, 2004; Volume 2. [Google Scholar]
- Fontaine, G. Les Danses Du Temps: Recherches Sur La Notion De Temps En Danse Contemporaine; Centre National De La Danse: Pantin, France, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Gunaratana, H. Méditer au Quotidien, Une Pratique Simple Du Bouddhisme; Marabout: Mulhous, France, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Ruby, C. La Figure du Spectateur; Armand Colin: Malakoff, France, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Clerc, R. Yoga de l’Energie; Le Courrier du Livre: Paris, France, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Clerc, R. Manuel de yoga—Tome I; Le Courrier du Livre: Paris, France, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Clerc, R. Manuel de yoga—Tome II; Le Courrier du Livre: Paris, France, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Ginot, I. Douceurs somatiques. Repères, Cahier De Danse 2013, 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eddy, M. A brief history of somatic practices and dance: Historical development of the field of somatic education and its relationship to dance. J. Dance Somat. Pract. 2009, 1, 5–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lutz, A.; Slagter, H.A.; Dunne, J.D.; Davidson, R.J. Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 163–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lutz, A.; Jha, A.P.; Dunne, J.D.; Saron, C.D. Investigating the phenomenological matrix of mindfulness-related practices from a neurocognitive perspective. Am. Psychol. 2015, 70, 632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Warzer, R.; Torres, E.B.; Bachrach, A. Micro-movement as physical signature of movement intention in work of choreographer Myriam Gourfink. In Proceedings of the 2014 International Workshop on Movement and Computing, Paris, France, 16–17 June 2014; p. 156. [Google Scholar]
- Husserl. Leçons Pour une Phénoménologie de la Conscience Intime du Temps; Epithémée, PUF: Paris, France, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Varela, F.J. The specious present: A neurophenomenology of time consciousness. Nat. Phenomenol. 1999, 64, 266–329. [Google Scholar]
- Varela, F.J.; Depraz, N. Au cœur du temps: L’auto-antécédance II. Intellectica 2003, 36, 183–203. [Google Scholar]
- Berthoz, A.; Debru, C. Anticipation et Prédiction: Du Geste au Voyage Mental; Odile Jacob: Neuilly-sur-Seine, France, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Pöppel, E. A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1997, 1, 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittmann, M. Moments in time. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2011, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bergson, H.; Paul, N.M.; Palmer, W.S. Matter and Memory; Courier Corporation: North Chelmsford, MA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Michon, J.A. The making of the present: A tutorial review. In Attention and performance VII; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1978; pp. 89–111. [Google Scholar]
- Fraisse, P. Perception and estimation of time. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1984, 35, 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wittmann, M.; van Wassenhove, V. Introduction: The Experience of Time: Neural Mechanisms and the Interplay of Emotion, Cognition and Embodiment. Philos. Trans. Biol. Sci. 2009, 1809–1813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gil, S.; Droit-Volet, S. Emotional time distortions: The fundamental role of arousal. Cogn. Emot. 2012, 26, 847–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Droit-Volet, S.; Fayolle, S.L.; Gil, S. Emotion and time perception: Effects of film-induced mood. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2011, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Droit-Volet, S.; Heros, J. Time judgments as a function of mindfulness meditation, anxiety, and mindfulness awareness. Mindfulness 2017, 8, 266–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buser, P.; Debru, C. Temps, Instant et durée (le): De la Philosophie Aux Neurosciences; Odile Jacob: Neuilly-sur-Seine, France, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Orgs, G.; Bestmann, S.; Schuur, F.; Haggard, P. From body form to biological motion: The apparent velocity of human movement biases subjective time. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 22, 712–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carrozzo, M.; Lacquaniti, F. Effects of speeding up or slowing down animate or inanimate motions on timing. Exp. Brain Res. 2013, 224, 581–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chambon, M.; Droit-Volet, S.; Niedenthal, P.M. The effect of embodying the elderly on time perception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 672–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nather, F.C.; Bueno, J.L.; Bigand, E.; Droit-Volet, S. Time changes with the embodiment of another’s body posture. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e19818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moelants, D. Preferred tempo reconsidered. In Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Sydney, Australia, 17–21 July 2002; Volume 2002, pp. 1–4. [Google Scholar]
- Van Noorden, L.; Moelants, D. Resonance in the perception of musical pulse. J. New Music Res. 1999, 28, 43–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murray, M.P.; Drought, A.B.; Kory, R.C. Walking patterns of normal men. J. Bone Joint Surg. Am. 1964, 46, 335–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- MacDougall, H.G.; Moore, S.T. Marching to the beat of the same drummer: The spontaneous tempo of human locomotion. J. Appl. Physiol. 2005, 99, 1164–1173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McAuley, J.D.; Jones, M.R.; Holub, S.; Johnston, H.M.; Miller, N.S. The time of our lives: Life span development of timing and event tracking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2006, 135, 348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vanneste, V.; Pouthas, J.; Wearden, S. Temporal control of rhythmic performance: A comparison between young and old adults. Exp. Aging Res. 2001, 27, 83–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moussay, S.; Dosseville, F.; Gauthier, A.; Larue, J.; Sesboüe, B.; Davenne, D. Circadian rhythms during cycling exercise and finger-tapping task. Chronobiol. Int. 2002, 19, 1137–1149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Buhmann, J.; Desmet, F.; Moens, B.; Van Dyck, E.; Leman, M. Spontaneous velocity effect of musical expression on self-paced walking. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0154414. [Google Scholar]
- Avanzino, L.; Lagravinese, G.; Bisio, A.; Perasso, L.; Ruggeri, P.; Bove, M. Action observation: Mirroring across our spontaneous movement tempo. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 10325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lagravinese, G.; Bisio, A.; Ruggeri, P.; Bove, M.; Avanzino, L. Learning by observing: The effect of multiple sessions of action-observation training on the spontaneous movement tempo and motor resonance. Neuropsychologia 2017, 96, 89–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bove, M.; Tacchino, A.; Pelosin, E.; Moisello, C.; Abbruzzese, G.; Ghilardi, M.F. Spontaneous movement tempo is influenced by observation of rhythmical actions. Brain Res. Bull. 2009, 80, 122–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oullier, O.; De Guzman, G.C.; Jantzen, K.J.; Lagarde, J.; Scott Kelso, J. Social coordination dynamics: Measuring human bonding. Soc. Neurosci. 2008, 3, 178–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baudouin, A.; Vanneste, S.; Isingrini, M.; Pouthas, V. Differential involvement of internal clock and working memory in the production and reproduction of duration: A study on older adults. Acta Psychol. 2006, 121, 285–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sauer, S.; Lemke, J.; Wittmann, M.; Kohls, N.; Mochty, U.; Walach, H. How long is now for mindfulness meditators? Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 750–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ichikawa, M.; Masakura, Y. Auditory stimulation affects apparent motion. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 2006, 48, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, S.W. Time perception and attention: The effects of prospective versus retrospective paradigms and task demands on perceived duration. Percept. Psychophys. 1985, 38, 115–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zakay, D. Subjective time and attentional resource allocation: An integrated model of time estimation. Adv. Psychol. 1989, 59, 365–397. [Google Scholar]
- Grondin, S. Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2010, 72, 561–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cahoon, R.L. Physiological arousal and time estimation. Percept. Motor Skills 1969, 28, 259–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zakay, D.; Block, R.A. Temporal cognition. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1997, 6, 12–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lamotte, M.; Izaute, M.; Droit-Volet, S. Awareness of time distortions and its relation with time judgment: A metacognitive approach. Conscious. Cogn. 2012, 21, 835–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Horowitz, T.; Treisman, A. Attention and apparent motion. Spat. Vis. 1994, 8, 193–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Folk, C.L.; Remington, R.W.; Wright, J.H. The structure of attentional control: Contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1994, 20, 317–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marusich, L.R.; Gilden, D.L. Assessing temporal integration spans in ADHD through apparent motion. Neuropsychology 2014, 28, 585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Puyjarinet, F.; Bégel, V.; Lopez, R.; Dellacherie, D.; Dalla Bella, S. Children and adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder cannot move to the beat. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 11550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pollatos, O.; Laubrock, J.; Wittmann, M. Interoceptive focus shapes the experience of time. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e86934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dehaene, S.; Changeux, J.P.; Naccache, L.; Sackur, J.; Sergent, C. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 204–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sysoeva, O.V.; Wittmann, M.; Mierau, A.; Polikanova, I.; Strüder, H.K.; Tonevitsky, A. Physical exercise speeds up motor timing. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tinklenberg, J.R.; Roth, W.T.; Kopell, B.S. Marijuana and ethanol: Differential effects on time perception, heart rate, and subjective response. Psychopharmacology 1976, 49, 275–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boltz, M.G. Changes in internal tempo and effects on the learning and remembering of event durations. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1994, 20, 1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dosseville, F.; Moussay, S.; Larue, J.; Gauthier, A.; Davenne, D. Physical exercise and time of day: Influences on spontaneous motor tempo. Percept. Motor Skills 2002, 95, 965–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schwarz, M.A.; Winkler, I.; Sedlmeier, P. The heart beat does not make us tick: The impacts of heart rate and arousal on time perception. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2013, 75, 182–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ricard, M. The art of Meditation; Atlantic Books Ltd.: London, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Lachaux, J.P. Cerveau Attentif (Le): Contrôle, maîtrise, Lâcher-Prise; Odile Jacob: Neuilly-sur-Seine, France, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd, J. The science of meditation and the state of hypnosis. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 2003, 46, 109–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kramer, R.S.; Weger, U.W.; Sharma, D. The effect of mindfulness meditation on time perception. Conscious. Cogn. 2013, 22, 846–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carter, O.L.; Presti, D.; Callistemon, C.; Ungerer, Y.; Liu, G.B.; Pettigrew, J.D. Meditation alters perceptual rivalry in Tibetan Buddhist monks. Curr. Biol. 2005, 15, R412–R413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wittmann, M.; Schmidt, S. Mindfulness meditation and the experience of time. In Meditation–Neuroscientific Approaches and Philosophical Implications; Springer International Publishing: Zurich, Switzerland, 2014; pp. 199–209. [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer, P.; Sasaki, Y.; Saito, Y. Zazen and cardiac variability. Psychosom. Med. 1999, 61, 812–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schuman, M. The psychophysiological model of meditation and altered states of consciousness: A critical review. In The Psychobiology of Consciousness; Springer: Zurich, Switzerland, 1980; pp. 333–378. [Google Scholar]
- Treisman, M.; Faulkner, A.; Naish, P.L. On the relation between time perception and the timing of motor action: Evidence for a temporal oscillator controlling the timing of movement. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1992, 45, 235–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittmann, M. Time perception and temporal processing levels of the brain. Chronobiol. Int. 1999, 16, 17–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schubotz, R.I.; Friederici, A.D.; Von Cramon, D.Y. Time perception and motor timing: A common cortical and subcortical basis revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 2000, 11, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pouthas, V.; Macar, F. Les bases neurales de la perception du temps et de la régulation temporelle de l’action. Psychol. Fr. 2005, 50, 27–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittmann, M.; Paulus, M.P. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 7–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gratier, M. Les rythmes de l’intersubjectivité. Spirale 2007, 47–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stern, D.N. the Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View From Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology; Karnac Books: London, UK, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Trevarthen, C. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before Speech: The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1979; Volume 1, pp. 530–571. [Google Scholar]
- Pokropski, M. Timing together, acting together. Phenomenology of intersubjective temporality and social cognition. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2015, 14, 897–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fuchs, T. The phenomenology and development of social perspectives. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2013, 12, 655–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tschacher, W.; Ramseyer, F.; Koole, S.L. Sharing the Now in the Social Present: Duration of Nonverbal Synchrony Is Linked With Personality. J. Personal. 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gioffredi, P. L’encadrement choré-graphique de l’expérience chez Myriam Gourfink, cadrages dans l’art des XXè et XXIè siècles. In Journée d’étude du Centre de Philosophie de l’art, Paris-1 Sorbonne; MSH Paris-Nord: Saint-Denis, France, 2008; Oral communication. [Google Scholar]
- Gioffredi, P. Phénoménologie de la danse contemporaine. Penser les enjeux éthiques et esthétiques des pièces de Myriam Gourfink avec Merleau-Ponty. Recherches en Danse 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gioffredi, P. L’expérience du spectateur, entre immersion et engagement. In Myriam Gourfink: Danser Sa Créature; Lesauvage, M., Piettre, C., Eds.; Leses Du Réel: Dijon, France, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Berkovich-Ohana, A.; Dor-Ziderman, Y.; Glicksohn, J.; Goldstein, A. Alterations in the sense of time, space, and body in the mindfulness-trained brain: A neurophenomenologically-guided MEG study. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mathôt, S.; Schreij, D.; Theeuwes, J. OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2012, 44, 314–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bates, D.; Maechler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S.; Christensen, R.H.B.; Singmann, H.; Dai, B.; Scheipl, F.; Grothendieck, G.; Green, P. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R Packag. Version 2014, 1, 1–23. [Google Scholar]
- Finlay, D.; von Grünau, M. Some experiments on the breakdown effect in apparent motion. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 1987, 42, 526–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gourfink, M.; Coconnier, C.; Glon, M. Enracinement aérien. Repères Cahier de Danse 2014, 13–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coconnier, C.; Gourfink, M.; Glon, M. Partager un état de conscience. Repères Cahier de Danse 2010, 22–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trevarthen, C.; Reddy, V. Consciousness in Infants. In The Blackwell Companion to Consciousness; Blackwell Pub.: Hoboken NJ, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Schaeffer, J.M. L’expérience Esthétique; Editions Gallimard: Paris, France, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Massin, M. Expérience Esthétique et Art Contemporain; Presses Universitaires de Rennes: Rennes, France, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Perrin, J. Figures de L’attention. Cinq Essais Sur La Spatialité En Danse; Les Presses Du Réel: Dijon, France, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Citton, Y. Pour une écologie de L’attention; Le Seuil: Paris, France, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Dewey, J. Art as Experience; Penguin: London, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Csikszentmihalyi, M.; Csikszentmihalyi, I.S. Optimal experience: Psychological Studies of Flow in Consciousness; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Wittmann, M. Modulations of the experience of self and time. Conscious. Cogn. 2015, 38, 172–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smallwood, J.; Brown, K.S.; Tipper, C.; Giesbrecht, B.; Franklin, M.S.; Mrazek, M.D.; Carlson, J.M.; Schooler, J.W. Pupillometric evidence for the decoupling of attention from perceptual input during offline thought. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e18298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tomasello, M. Joint attention as social cognition. In Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 1995; Volume 103130. [Google Scholar]
- Gallese, V.; Goldman, A. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1998, 2, 493–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- George, N.; Driver, J.; Dolan, R.J. Seen gaze-direction modulates fusiform activity and its coupling with other brain areas during face processing. Neuroimage 2001, 13, 1102–1112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ferrari, P.F.; Gallese, V.; Rizzolatti, G.; Fogassi, L. Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003, 17, 1703–1714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Naranjo, J.R.; Schmidt, S. Is it me or not me? Modulation of perceptual-motor awareness and visuomotor performance by mindfulness meditation. BMC Neurosci. 2012, 13, 88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Allman, M.J.; Meck, W.H. Pathophysiological distortions in time perception and timed performance. Brain 2011, 135, 656–677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Depraz, N. Attention et Vigilance: À la Croisée de la Phénoménologie et des Sciences Cognitives; PUF: Paris, France, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Aardweg, J.; Karemaker, J. Respiratory variability and associated cardiovascular changes in adults at rest. Clin. Physiol. Funct. Imaging 1991, 11, 95–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vlemincx, E.; Taelman, J.; De Peuter, S.; Van Diest, I.; Van Den Bergh, O. Sigh rate and respiratory variability during mental load and sustained attention. Psychophysiology 2011, 48, 117–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Diest, I.; Thayer, J.F.; Vandeputte, B.; Van de Woestijne, K.P.; Van den Bergh, O. Anxiety and respiratory variability. Physiol. Behav. 2006, 89, 189–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boiten, F.A. The effects of emotional behaviour on components of the respiratory cycle. Biol. Psychol. 1998, 49, 29–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, J.L.; Penhune, V.B.; Zatorre, R.J. Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain. Cereb. Cortex 2008, 18, 2844–2854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Overy, K.; Molnar-Szakacs, I. Being together in time: Musical experience and the mirror neuron system. Music Percept. Interdiscip. J. 2009, 26, 489–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kornysheva, K.; von Cramon, D.Y.; Jacobsen, T.; Schubotz, R.I. Tuning-in to the beat: Aesthetic appreciation of musical rhythms correlates with a premotor activity boost. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2010, 31, 48–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Calvo-Merino, B.; Urgesi, C.; Orgs, G.; Aglioti, S.M.; Haggard, P. Extrastriate body area underlies aesthetic evaluation of body stimuli. Exp. Brain Res. 2010, 204, 447–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Deinzer, V.; Clancy, L.; Wittmann, M. The Sense of Time While Watching a Dance Performance. SAGE Open 2017, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicary, S.; Sperling, M.; Wvon Zimmermann, J.; Richardson, D.; Orgs, G. Joint action aesthetics. PloS ONE 2017, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pelowski, M. Satori, koan and aesthetic experience: Exploring the “realization of emptiness” in Buddhist enlightenment via an empirical study of modern art. Psyke Logos 2012, 33, 33. [Google Scholar]
- Bergson, H. Matière et Mémoire: Essai sur la Relation du Corps à L’esprit in Oeuvres; Le Livre de Poche; Presses Universitaires de France: Paris, France, 2015; Volume I. [Google Scholar]
- Rancière, J. La Partage du Sensible: Esthétique et Politique; La Fabrique Éditions: Paris, France, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Rancière, J. Le spectateur émancipé; La Fabrique éditions: Paris, France, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Rancière, J. Entretien avec Stéphanie Chiffaudel. Joséphine 2008, 6, 24–30. [Google Scholar]
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Joufflineau, C.; Vincent, C.; Bachrach, A. Synchronization, Attention and Transformation: Multidimensional Exploration of the Aesthetic Experience of Contemporary Dance Spectators. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 24. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8020024
Joufflineau C, Vincent C, Bachrach A. Synchronization, Attention and Transformation: Multidimensional Exploration of the Aesthetic Experience of Contemporary Dance Spectators. Behavioral Sciences. 2018; 8(2):24. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8020024
Chicago/Turabian StyleJoufflineau, Coline, Coralie Vincent, and Asaf Bachrach. 2018. "Synchronization, Attention and Transformation: Multidimensional Exploration of the Aesthetic Experience of Contemporary Dance Spectators" Behavioral Sciences 8, no. 2: 24. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8020024