Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Stereotype Content Model (SCM) and BIAS Map
1.2. Low Warmth—Low Competence (LW-LC) Social Groups
1.3. Response Pattern of Stereotype Activation
1.4. Electrophysiological Pattern of Stereotype Activation
1.4.1. Early ERP Component: P2
1.4.2. Later ERP Components: N400 and LPC
1.5. Current Research
2. Study 1: The Behavioral Pattern of Explicit Stereotype Activation for HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-HC and LW-LC Social Groups
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Design
2.1.3. Stimuli
2.1.4. Procedure
2.1.5. Data Analyses
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Accuracy
2.2.2. Response Times
2.3. Discussion
3. Study 2: The Electrophysiological Pattern of Explicit Stereotype Activation for HW-HC, HW-LC, LW-HC and LW-LC Social Groups
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Stimuli and Procedure
3.1.3. EEG Recording
3.1.4. Data Analyses
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Behavioral Results
3.2.2. ERPs Results
3.3. Discussion
4. General Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
SCM Quadrants | Prime Words in Chinese | Prime Words in English | Stereotype Congruent Target Words in Chinese | Stereotype Congruent Target Words in English | Stereotype Incongruent Target Words in Chinese | Stereotype Incongruent Target Words in English |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HW-HC | 军人 | soldiers | 保家卫国 | protect our homes and defend our country | 重利益 | regard interests as highly important |
威严 | dignified | 欺软怕硬 | bully the weak and fear the strong | |||
坚毅 | fortitude | 瘦弱 | thin and weak | |||
正义 | righteous | 可恶 | hateful | |||
正直 | upstanding | 爱唠叨 | nagging | |||
消防员 | firemen | 工作危险 | dangerous work | 喊口号 | shout slogans | |
行动迅速 | act quickly | 行动缓慢 | slow movement | |||
灭火救人 | put out the fire and rescue people | 不办实事 | don’t get things done | |||
令人尊敬 | respectable | 唯利是图 | seek nothing but profits | |||
敬业 | dedicated | 凶残 | merciless | |||
勇敢 | brave | 无责任心 | without the sense of responsibility | |||
大学教师 | professors | 有学问 | scholarly | 狡猾 | crafty | |
知识渊博 | knowledgeable | 凶残 | cruel | |||
教书育人 | imparting knowledge and educating people | 游手好闲 | idle | |||
心理咨询师 | psychotherapists | 善于观察 | good at observing | 没人性 | inhuman | |
懂人心 | empathetic | 缺少关爱 | lack of love and care | |||
有耐心 | have much patience | 极端 | go to extremes | |||
温和 | gentle | 没有同情心 | without compassion | |||
帮助他人 | helping others | 凶狠 | fierce | |||
空姐 | air hostesses | 礼仪好 | good manners | 威严 | dignified | |
微笑服务 | service with a smile | 残暴 | brute | |||
有气质 | graceful | 颓废 | decadence | |||
漂亮 | pretty | 白发苍苍 | grey-haired | |||
美丽大方 | beautiful and gracious | 心理扭曲 | psychological distortion | |||
举止优雅 | elegant manner | 蛮横 | rude and unreasonable | |||
瑜伽教练 | yoga instructors | 柔韧性强 | great flexibility | 精神涣散 | spiritless | |
体形优美 | beautiful shape | 大腹便便 | big-bellied | |||
身材好 | good figure | 没有上进心 | no desire to advance | |||
健美 | well-built | 邋遢 | slovenly | |||
健康 | athletic | 脏 | dirty | |||
HW-LC | 老年人 | elderly | 需要照顾 | need to be looked after | 体形优美 | beautiful shape |
需要关爱 | need love and care | 柔韧性强 | great flexibility | |||
爱唠叨 | nagging | 身材好 | good figure | |||
白发苍苍 | grey-haired | 残暴 | brutal | |||
行动缓慢 | slow movement | 行动迅速 | act quickly | |||
慈祥 | kindly | 挥霍 | spend extravagantly | |||
农民 | farmers | 辛苦 | laborious work | 知识渊博 | knowledgeable | |
种田 | farming | 私生活混乱 | promiscuous | |||
勤劳 | industrious | 奢侈 | spend extravagantly | |||
憨厚老实 | simple and honest | 懒惰 | lazy | |||
收入低 | low-income | 官腔官调 | bureaucratic | |||
家庭主妇 | housewives | 做饭带孩子 | cooking and taking care of children | 留学 | study abroad | |
顾家 | value family | 保家卫国 | protect our homes and defend our country | |||
做家务 | keep house | 贪污腐败 | corrupt | |||
贤惠 | virtuous | 傲慢 | arrogant | |||
勤劳 | industrious | 形式主义 | formalism | |||
农民工 | migrant workers | 辛苦 | laborious work | 有知识 | educated | |
劳累 | overworked | 工作清闲 | work at leisure | |||
生活艰辛 | life is tough | 工作稳定 | stable job | |||
弱势群体 | vulnerable group | 官腔 | official jargon | |||
留守儿童 | left-behind children | 渴望陪伴 | eager to be accompanied by parents | 见识广 | well-informed | |
缺少关爱 | lack of love and care | 有钱 | wealthy | |||
孤独 | lonely | 美丽大方 | beautiful and gracious | |||
可怜 | pitiful | 外语好 | good foreign language | |||
清洁工 | cleaning workers | 辛苦 | laborious work | 有气质 | graceful | |
早起 | early rising | 有学问 | scholarly | |||
朴实 | simple | 收入高 | high income | |||
可敬 | worthy of respect | 铁饭碗 | has a stable lifelong job | |||
勤劳 | industrious | 漂亮 | pretty | |||
工资低 | low salary | 待遇好 | well-paid | |||
LW-HC | 商人 | businessmen | 重利益 | regard interests as highly important | 渴望陪伴 | eager to accompany |
有钱 | wealthy | 贫穷 | poor | |||
精明 | shrewd | 憨厚老实 | simple and honest | |||
唯利是图 | seek nothing but profits | 做家务 | keep house | |||
海归 | overseas returnees | 留学 | study abroad | 监狱 | prison | |
外语好 | good foreign language | 萎靡 | flattened | |||
见识广 | well-informed | 种田 | farming | |||
有知识 | educated | 工资低 | low salary | |||
公务员 | government workers | 工作稳定 | stable job | 绯闻多 | need more concern | |
铁饭碗 | has a stable lifelong job | 需要照顾 | need to be looked after | |||
工作清闲 | work at leisure | 堕落 | decadent | |||
待遇好 | well-paid | 工作危险 | dangerous work | |||
政府官员 | government officials | 官腔 | official jargon | 有耐心 | have much patience | |
形式主义 | formalism | 敬业 | dedicated | |||
官腔官调 | bureaucratic | 孤独 | lonely | |||
喊口号 | shout slogans | 弱势群体 | vulnerable group | |||
不办实事 | don’t get things done | 朴实 | simple | |||
贪污腐败 | corrupt | 做饭带孩子 | cooking and taking care of children | |||
大腹便便 | big-bellied | 骨瘦如柴 | skinny | |||
演艺明星 | stars in showbiz | 收入高 | high income | 无所事事 | slothful | |
绯闻多 | more scandal | 啃老 | depend on parents for living | |||
光鲜亮丽 | glamorous | 衣衫褴褛 | shabbily dressed | |||
漂亮 | pretty | 收入低 | low-income | |||
私生活混乱 | promiscuous | 灭火救人 | put out the fire and rescue people | |||
好看 | good-looking | 形容枯槁 | haggard | |||
富人 | rich | 有钱 | wealthy | 生活艰辛 | life is tough | |
奢侈 | extravagant | 需要关爱 | need love and care | |||
傲慢 | arrogant | 劳累 | overworked | |||
狡猾 | crafty | 可怜 | pitiful | |||
挥霍 | spend extravagantly | 辛苦 | laborious work | |||
LW-LC | 罪犯 | criminals | 监狱 | prison | 慈祥 | kindly |
可恶 | hateful | 令人尊敬 | respectable | |||
凶狠 | fierce | 正义 | righteous | |||
残暴 | violent and cruel | 教书育人 | imparting knowledge and educating people | |||
无业游民 | unemployed | 游手好闲 | idle | 辛苦 | laborious work | |
无所事事 | slothful | 早起 | early bird | |||
无责任心 | irresponsible | 好看 | good-looking | |||
啃老 | depend on parents for living | 勤劳 | industrious | |||
没有上进心 | no desire to advance | 勇敢 | courageous | |||
乞丐 | beggars | 脏 | dirty | 健美 | well-built | |
衣衫褴褛 | shabbily dressed | 光鲜亮丽 | glamorous | |||
邋遢 | slovenly | 有钱 | wealthy | |||
懒惰 | lazy | 漂亮 | pretty | |||
贫穷 | poor | 勤劳 | industrious | |||
吸毒者 | drug addicts | 颓废 | decadence | 健康 | fitness | |
堕落 | decadent | 辛苦 | laborious work | |||
萎靡 | flattened | 勤劳 | industrious | |||
精神涣散 | spiritless | 善于观察 | good at observing | |||
骨瘦如柴 | skinny | 坚毅 | fortitude | |||
形容枯槁 | haggard | 精明 | shrewd | |||
瘦弱 | thin and weak | 顾家 | value family | |||
恐怖分子 | terrorists | 极端 | go to extremes | 帮助他人 | helping others | |
残暴 | brutal | 微笑服务 | service with a smile | |||
凶残 | cruel | 正直 | upstanding | |||
心理扭曲 | psychological distortion | 举止优雅 | elegant manner | |||
没人性 | inhuman | 礼仪好 | good manners | |||
城管 | urban management officers | 欺软怕硬 | bully the weak and fear the strong | 温和 | gentle | |
蛮横 | rude and unreasonable | 懂人心 | empathetic | |||
凶残 | merciless | 贤惠 | virtuous | |||
没有同情心 | without compassion | 可敬 | worthy of respect |
References
- Macrae, C.N.; Bodenhausen, G.V. Social cognition: Thinking categorically about others. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2000, 51, 93–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ellemers, N. Gender stereotypes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2018, 69, 275–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Blair, I.V.; Banaji, M.R. Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 70, 1142–1163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Devine, P.G. Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 56, 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macrae, C.N.; Milne, A.B.; Bodenhausen, G.V. Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 66, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oakhill, J.; Garnham, A.; Reynolds, D. Immediate activation of stereotypical gender information. Mem. Cognit. 2005, 33, 972–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amodio, D.M.; Cikara, M. The Social Neuroscience of Prejudice. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quadflieg, S.; Macrae, C.N. Stereotypes and stereotyping: What’s the brain got to do with it? Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 22, 215–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amodio, D.M. The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 670–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blair, I.V. The Malleability of Automatic Stereotypes and Prejudice. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2002, 6, 242–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fiske, S.T.; Cuddy, A.J.C.; Glick, P.; Xu, J. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 82, 878–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cuddy, A.J.C.; Fiske, S.T.; Glick, P. The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 92, 631–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fiske, S.T.; Cuddy, A.J.C.; Glick, P. Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fiske, S.T. Intergroup biases: A focus on stereotype content. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015, 3, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fiske, S.T. Stereotype content: Warmth and competence endure. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 27, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cuddy, A.J.C.; Fiske, S.T.; Kwan, V.S.Y.; Glick, P.; Demoulin, S.; Leyens, J.P.; Bond, M.H.; Croizet, J.C.; Ellemers, N.; Sleebos, E. Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 48, 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, S.J.; Bai, X.; Fiske, S.T. Admired rich or resented rich? How two cultures vary in envy. J. Cross Cultur. Psychol. 2018, 49, 1114–1143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bye, H.H.; Herrebrøden, H.; Hjetland, G.J.; Røyset, G.Ø.; Westby, L.L. Stereotypes of Norwegian social groups. Scand. J. Psychol. 2014, 55, 469–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Grigoryev, D.; Fiske, S.T.; Batkhina, A. Mapping ethnic stereotypes and their antecedents in Russia: The stereotype content model. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cuddy, A.J.C.; Fiske, S.T.; Glick, P. Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 40, 61–149. [Google Scholar]
- Russell, A.M.T.; Fiske, S.T. It’s all relative: Competition and status drive interpersonal perception. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 38, 1193–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simon, J.C.; Styczynski, N.; Gutsell, J.N. Social perceptions of warmth and competence influence behavioral intentions and neural processing. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2020, 20, 265–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Abele, A.E.; Yzerbyt, V. Body posture and interpersonal perception in a dyadic interaction: A Big Two Analysis. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. Dehumanizing the lowest of the low neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 847–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. Social groups that elicit disgust are differentially processed in mPFC. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2007, 2, 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. Social neuroscience evidence for dehumanised perception. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 20, 192–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haslam, N.; Loughnan, S. Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014, 65, 399–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krendl, A.C. An fMRI investigation of the effects of culture on evaluations of stigmatized individuals. NeuroImage 2016, 124, 336–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cikara, M.; Farnsworth, R.A.; Harris, L.T.; Fiske, S.T. On the wrong side of the trolley track: Neural correlates of relative social valuation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2010, 5, 404–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dricu, M.; Bührer, S.; Hesse, F.; Eder, C.; Posada, A.; Aue, T. Warmth and competence predict overoptimistic beliefs for out-group but not in-group members. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e207670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dricu, M.; Schüpbach, L.; Bristle, M.; Wiest, R.; Moser, D.A.; Aue, T. Group membership dictates the neural correlates of social optimism biases. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moser, D.A.; Dricu, M.; Wiest, R.; Schüpbach, L.; Aue, T. Social optimism biases are associated with cortical thickness. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2020, 15, 745–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kawakami, K.; Young, H.; Dovidio, J.F. Automatic stereotyping: Category, trait, and behavioral activations. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2002, 28, 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macrae, C.N.; Bodenhausen, G.V.; Milne, A.B. The dissection of selection in person perception: Inhibitory processes in social stereotyping. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 69, 397–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pendry, L.F.; Macrae, C.N. What the disinterested perceiver overlooks: Goal-directed social categorization. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1996, 22, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sinclair, L.; Kunda, Z. Reactions to a black professional: Motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 885–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kidder, C.K.; White, K.R.; Hinojos, M.R.; Sandoval, M.; Crites, S.L. Sequential stereotype priming: A meta-analysis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 22, 199–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, K.R.; Danek, R.H.; Herring, D.R.; Taylor, J.H.; Crites, S.L. Taking priming to task: Variations in stereotype priming effects across participant task. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 49, 29–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, P.; Yang, Y.; Zhao, L. The activation of stereotypes: Behavioral and ERPs evidence. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2010, 42, 607–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, K.R.; Crites, S.L.; Taylor, J.H.; Corral, G. Wait, what? Assessing stereotype incongruities using the N400 ERP component. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2009, 4, 191–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wang, P.; Yang, Y.; Tan, C.; Chen, Q.; Van Cantfort, T.E. Gender stereotype activation versus lexical semantic activation: An ERP study. J. Gen. Psychol. 2017, 144, 283–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, Y.; Wang, P.; Yin, Z.; Chen, Q.; Feng, X. The pattern and neural correlates of unintentional stereotype activation. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2015, 47, 488–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, X.; Li, Q.; Sun, S.; Zuo, B. The time course from gender categorization to gender stereotype activation. Soc. Neurosci. 2018, 13, 52–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plaza, M.; Boiché, J.; Brunel, L.; Ruchaud, F. Sport = male but not all sports: Investigating the gender stereotypes of sport activities at the explicit and implicit levels. Sex Roles 2017, 76, 202–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsamadi, D.; Falbén, J.K.; Persson, L.M.; Golubickis, M.; Caughey, S.; Sahin, B.; Macrae, C.N. Stereotype-based priming without stereotype activation: A tale of two priming tasks. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2020, 73, 1939–1948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Müller, F.; Rothermund, K. What does it take to activate stereotypes? Simple primes don’t seem enough: A replication of stereotype activation (Banaji & Hardin, 1996; Blair & Banaji, 1996). Soc. Psychol. 2014, 45, 187–193. [Google Scholar]
- Banaji, M.R.; Hardin, C.D. Automatic stereotyping. Psychol. Sci. 1996, 7, 136–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kimura, A.; Wada, Y.; Goto, S.; Tsuzuki, D.; Cai, D.; Oka, T.; Dan, I. Implicit gender-based food stereotypes. Semantic priming experiments on young Japanese. Appetite 2009, 52, 521–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macrae, C.N.; Cloutier, J. A matter of design: Priming context and person perception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 45, 1012–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Macrae, C.N.; Martin, D. A boy primed Sue: Feature-based processing and person construal. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 37, 793–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macrae, C.N.; Mitchell, J.P.; Pendry, L.F. What’s in a forename? Cue familiarity and stereotypical thinking. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 38, 186–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gawronski, B.; Deutsch, R.; Mbirkou, S.; Seibt, B.; Strack, F. When “just say no” is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 370–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kawakami, K.; Dovidio, J.F. The reliability of implicit stereotyping. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 27, 212–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Todd, A.R.; Thiem, K.C.; Neel, R. Does seeing faces of young black boys facilitate the identification of threatening stimuli? Psychol. Sci. 2016, 27, 384–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ito, T.A.; Tomelleri, S. Seeing is not stereotyping: The functional independence of categorization and stereotype activation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017, 12, 758–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wittenbrink, B.; Judd, C.M.; Park, B. Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 72, 262–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittenbrink, B.; Judd, C.M.; Park, B. Evaluative versus conceptual judgments in automatic stereotyping and prejudice. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 37, 244–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Amodio, D.M.; Harmon-Jones, E.; Devine, P.G.; Curtin, J.J.; Hartley, S.L.; Covert, A.E. Neural signals for the detection of unintentional race bias. Psychol. Sci. 2004, 15, 88–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dickter, C.L.; Kittel, J.A. The effect of stereotypical primes on the neural processing of racially ambiguous faces. Soc. Neurosci. 2012, 7, 622–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Govorun, O.; Payne, B.K. Ego—Depletion and prejudice: Separating automatic and controlled components. Soc. Cogn. 2006, 24, 111–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jones, C.R.; Fazio, R.H. Person categorization and automatic racial stereotyping effects on weapon identification. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 36, 1073–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kawakami, K.; Dion, K.L.; Dovidio, J.F. Implicit stereotyping and prejudice and the primed Stroop task. Swiss J. Psychol. 1999, 58, 241–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kubota, J.T.; Ito, T.A. The role of expression and race in weapons identification. Emotion 2014, 14, 1115–1124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Payne, B.K. Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 81, 181–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Payne, B.K. Conceptualizing control in social cognition: How executive functioning modulates the expression of automatic stereotyping. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 89, 488–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stewart, B.D.; Payne, B.K. Bringing automatic stereotyping under control: Implementation intentions as efficient means of thought control. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 34, 1332–1345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hehman, E.; Volpert, H.I.; Simons, R.F. The N400 as an index of racial stereotype accessibility. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 544–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Freeman, J.B.; Ambady, N. A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 118, 247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kawakami, K.; Amodio, D.M.; Hugenberg, K. Intergroup perception and cognition: An integrative framework for understanding the causes and consequences of social categorization. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2017; Volume 55, pp. 1–80. [Google Scholar]
- Krieglmeyer, R.; Sherman, J.W. Disentangling stereotype activation and stereotype application in the stereotype misperception task. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 103, 205–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jia, L.; Dickter, C.L.; Luo, J.; Xiao, X.; Yang, Q.; Lei, M.; Qiu, J.; Zhang, Q. Different brain mechanisms between stereotype activation and application: Evidence from an ERP study. Int. J. Psychol. 2012, 47, 58–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amodio, D.M.; Bartholow, B.D.; Ito, T.A. Tracking the dynamics of the social brain: ERP approaches for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 385–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ito, T.A.; Kubota, J.T. Bioelectrical echoes from a career at the cutting edge: John Cacioppo’s legacy and the use of ERPs in social psychology. Soc. Neurosci. 2019, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, P.; Yang, Y.; Tan, C.; Zhao, X.; Liu, Y.; Lin, C. Stereotype activation is unintentional: Behavioural and event-related potenials evidence. Int. J. Psychol. 2016, 51, 156–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pesciarelli, F.; Scorolli, C.; Cacciari, C. Neural correlates of the implicit processing of grammatical and stereotypical gender violations: A masked and unmasked priming study. Biol. Psychol. 2019, 146, 107714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luck, S.J.; Hillyard, S.A. Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology 1994, 31, 291–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dickter, C.L.; Bartholow, B.D. Racial ingroup and outgroup attention biases revealed by event-related brain potentials. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2007, 2, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ito, T.A.; Urland, G.R. Race and gender on the brain: Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 616–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ito, T.A.; Urland, G.R. The influence of processing objectives on the perception of faces: An ERP study of race and gender perception. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2005, 5, 21–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willadsen-Jensen, E.C.; Ito, T.A. Ambiguity and the timecourse of racial perception. Soc. Cogn. 2006, 24, 580–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kutas, M.; Federmeier, K.D. Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2011, 62, 621–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bentin, S.; McCarthy, G.; Wood, C.C. Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1985, 60, 343–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, L.; Ma, Q.; Song, Z.; Shi, Y.; Wang, Y.; Pfotenhauer, L. N400 and the activation of prejudice against rural migrant workers in China. Brain Res. 2011, 1375, 103–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Osterhout, L.; Bersick, M.; McLaughlin, J. Brain potentials reflect violations of gender stereotypes. Mem. Cognit. 1997, 25, 273–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hammer, A.; Jansma, B.M.; Lamers, M.; Münte, T.F. Pronominal reference in sentences about persons or things: An electrophysiological approach. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 227–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Xu, X.; Jiang, X.; Zhou, X. Processing biological gender and number information during Chinese pronoun resolution: ERP evidence for functional differentiation. Brain Cogn. 2013, 81, 223–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, Y.; Xu, Q.; Zhu, T.; Zheng, X.; Dong, X.; Chen, Q. The behavioral patterns of stereotype activation among four different warmth-competence social groups based on Stereotype Content Model. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2019, 51, 1143–1156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ji, Z.; Chen, Q.; Fan, X.; Xu, Q.; Yang, Y. Stereotypes of Social Groups in Mainland China in Terms of Warmth and Competence: Evidence from a Large Undergraduate Sample. 2020. Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/dbh3v/ (accessed on 26 October 2020).
- Katz, D.; Braly, K. Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1933, 28, 280–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratcliff, R. Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 114, 510–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bartholow, B.D. On the role of conflict and control in social cognition: Event-related brain potential investigations. Psychophysiology 2010, 47, 201–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wentura, D.; Degner, J. A practical guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 95–116. [Google Scholar]
- Conrey, F.R.; Sherman, J.W.; Gawronski, B.; Hugenberg, K.; Groom, C.J. Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 89, 469–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hütter, M.; Klauer, K.C. Applying processing trees in social psychology. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 27, 116–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, D.J.; Hopwood, C.J.; Cesario, J.; Pleskac, T.J. Advancing research on cognitive processes in social and personality psychology: A hierarchical drift diffusion model primer. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2017, 8, 413–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gratton, G.; Coles, M.G.; Donchin, E. A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1983, 55, 468–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keil, A.; Debener, S.; Gratton, G.; Junghöfer, M.; Kappenman, E.S.; Luck, S.J.; Luu, P.; Miller, G.A.; Yee, C.M. Committee report: Publication guidelines and recommendations for studies using electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography. Psychophysiology 2014, 51, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carretié, L.; Martín-Loeches, M.; Hinojosa, J.A.; Mercado, F. Emotion and attention interaction studied through event-related potentials. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2001, 13, 1109–1128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eimer, M.; Holmes, A.; McGlone, F.P. The role of spatial attention in the processing of facial expression: An ERP study of rapid brain responses to six basic emotions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2003, 3, 97–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bamford, S.; Broyd, S.J.; Benikos, N.; Ward, R.; Wiersema, J.R.; Sonuga-Barke, E. The late positive potential: A neural marker of the regulation of emotion-based approach-avoidance actions? Biol. Psychol. 2015, 105, 115–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Peeters, G. From good and bad to can and must: Subjective necessity of acts associated with positively and negatively valued stimuli. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 32, 125–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klauer, K.C.; Teige-Mocigemba, S.; Spruyt, A. Contrast effects in spontaneous evaluations: A psychophysical account. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 96, 265–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Glaser, J.; Banaji, M.R. When fair is foul and foul is fair: Reverse priming in automatic evaluation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 669–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moskowitz, G.B.; Ignarri, C. Implicit volition and stereotype control. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 20, 97–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moskowitz, G.B.; Gollwitzer, P.M.; Wasel, W.; Schaal, B. Preconscious control of stereotype activation through chronic egalitarian goals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 167–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moskowitz, G.B. On the Control over Stereotype Activation and Stereotype Inhibition. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2010, 4, 140–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moskowitz, G.B.; Salomon, A.R.; Taylor, C.M. Preconsciously Controlling Stereotyping: Implicitly Activated Egalitarian Goals Prevent the Activation of Stereotypes. Soc. Cogn. 2000, 18, 151–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Y.; Zhang, D.; Luo, Y. How disgust facilitates avoidance: An ERP study on attention modulation by threats. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015, 10, 598–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, D.; Liu, Y.; Wang, L.; Ai, H.; Luo, Y. Mechanisms for attentional modulation by threatening emotions of fear, anger, and disgust. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2017, 17, 198–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fiske, S.T. Warmth and competence: Stereotype content issues for clinicians and researchers. Can. Psychol. 2012, 53, 14–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cuddy, A.J.C.; Glick, P.; Beninger, A. The dynamics of warmth and competence judgments, and their outcomes in organizations. Res. Organ. Behav. 2011, 31, 73–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schul, Y.; Mayo, R.; Burnstein, E. Encoding under trust and distrust: The spontaneous activation of incongruent cognitions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 86, 668–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mayo, R. Cognition is a matter of trust: Distrust tunes cognitive processes. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 26, 283–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Posten, A.; Mussweiler, T. When distrust frees your mind: The stereotype-reducing effects of distrust. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 105, 567–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Abele, A.E.; Ellemers, N.; Fiske, S.T.; Koch, A.; Yzerbyt, V. Navigating the social world: Toward an integrated framework for evaluating self, individuals, and groups. Psychol. Rev. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yzerbyt, V.; Corneille, O. Cognitive Process: Reality Constraints and Integrity Concerns in Social Perception. In On the Nature of Prejudice: Fifty Years after Allport; Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2008; pp. 173–191. [Google Scholar]
- Abele, A.E.; Wojciszke, B. Agency and communion from the perspective of self versus others. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 751–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Leach, C.W.; Ellemers, N.; Barreto, M. Group virtue: The importance of morality (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 234–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Koch, A.; Imhoff, R.; Dotsch, R.; Unkelbach, C.; Alves, H. The ABC of stereotypes about groups: Agency/socioeconomic success, conservative–progressive beliefs, and communion. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 110, 675–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Yang, Y.; White, K.R.G.; Fan, X.; Xu, Q.; Chen, Q.-W. Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 1001. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10121001
Yang Y, White KRG, Fan X, Xu Q, Chen Q-W. Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(12):1001. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10121001
Chicago/Turabian StyleYang, Yaping, Katherine R. G. White, Xinfang Fan, Qiang Xu, and Qing-Wei Chen. 2020. "Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample" Brain Sciences 10, no. 12: 1001. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10121001
APA StyleYang, Y., White, K. R. G., Fan, X., Xu, Q., & Chen, Q.-W. (2020). Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample. Brain Sciences, 10(12), 1001. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10121001