Next Article in Journal
Resting-State Networks Associated with Behavioral and Self-Reported Measures of Persecutory Ideation in Psychosis
Previous Article in Journal
Prism Adaptation and Optokinetic Stimulation Comparison in the Rehabilitation of Unilateral Spatial Neglect
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Objective Knowledge Mediates the Relationship between the Use of Social Media and COVID-19-Related False Memories

1
Department of Psychology, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, 81100 Caserta, Italy
2
Department of Family Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC H3S1Z1, Canada
3
Inmates Ward, Department of Internal Medicine, Antonio Cardarelli Hospital, 80131 Naples, Italy
4
Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, Suor Orsola Benincasa University, 80135 Naples, Italy
5
Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, 80138 Naples, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Both authors contributed equally to this work.
Both authors contributed equally to this work.
Brain Sci. 2021, 11(11), 1489; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111489
Submission received: 12 October 2021 / Revised: 4 November 2021 / Accepted: 9 November 2021 / Published: 11 November 2021

Abstract

:
The exposure to relevant social and/or historical events can increase the generation of false memories (FMs). The Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic is a calamity challenging health, political, and journalistic bodies, with media generating confusion that has facilitated the spread of fake news. In this respect, our study aims at investigating the relationships between memories (true memories, TMs vs. FMs) for COVID-19-related news and different individual variables (i.e., use of traditional and social media, COVID-19 perceived and objective knowledge, fear of the disease, depression and anxiety symptoms, reasoning skills, and coping mechanisms). One hundred and seventy-one university students (131 females) were surveyed. Overall, our results suggested that depression and anxiety symptoms, reasoning skills, and coping mechanisms did not affect the formation of FMs. Conversely, the fear of loved ones contracting the infection was found to be negatively associated with FMs. This finding might be due to an empathy/prosociality-based positive bias boosting memory abilities, also explained by the young age of participants. Furthermore, objective knowledge (i) predicted an increase in TMs and decrease in FMs and (ii) significantly mediated the relationships between the use of social media and development of both TMs and FMs. In particular, higher levels of objective knowledge strengthened the formation of TMs and decreased the development of FMs following use of social media. These results may lead to reconsidering the idea of social media as the main source of fake news. This claim is further supported by either the lack of substantial differences between the use of traditional and social media among participants reporting FMs or the positive association between use of social media and levels of objective knowledge. The knowledge about the topic rather than the type of source would make a difference in the process of memory formation.

1. Introduction

The fabricated or distorted recollection of an event is defined as a false memory (FM). People may remember events that never happened or may remember them differently from how they actually occurred [1]. Intriguingly, the rate of FMs seems to be affected by specific individual variables, e.g., knowledge (alleged or actual) about the topic of interest, emotional arousal and valence, depressive and anxiety symptoms, personality factors, and cognitive abilities.
FMs appear to be related to a higher interest in, and engagement with, a certain topic [2,3], which underlines a greater exposure to news on the matter. In this respect, as recently observed, FMs were found to be associated with the spreading of fake news [4]. Some studies found that FMs are experienced more frequently by individuals exaggerating their perceived knowledge about the topic of interest (i.e., overclaiming). This finding is likely explained by the tendency to not admit that they do not remember a given piece of information due to social pressure [5]. Objective knowledge about a certain topic has instead been associated with a lower frequency of FMs [6,7]. Indeed, people would tend to form high-level cognitive reasoning schemes for topics on which they have high knowledge about; such a tendency may represent a protective factor against the development of memory biases [8]. This hypothesis is in accordance with the rationale of the emerging Monitoring Model Framework (MMF) [9,10]. According to the MMF, people would discriminate a true memory (TM) from a FM by evaluating their personal mental experiences based on two cognitive systems operating in parallel. The first system is based on heuristic judgments employed to determine whether the contents of an alleged memory correspond to the typical features (e.g., perceptual, temporal, and semantic) of a TM. For instance, TMs are usually more vivid and familiar than FMs, and they contain more spatial and temporal details compared with imagined or unreal experiences. Conversely, the second system is based on a more systematic approach according to which the contents of the alleged memory are carefully evaluated to verify their plausibility by comparing them with pre-existing knowledge and/or other (dis)confirmative evidence [10].
Emotions can also affect susceptibility to FMs. For instance, in the context of emotional arousal, some studies showed that individuals generally pay more attention to emotional than neutral events. As a consequence, emotional events are more likely to be encoded in memory. Once encoded, stress hormones in the brain consolidate emotional memories by making them more enduring [11,12]. This psychophysiological mechanism appears to significantly reduce people’s susceptibility to FMs [13]. However, it is important to underline that also emotional valence may play a role in the genesis of FMs. Indeed, it has been suggested that negative emotions lead to careful monitoring of the environment for encoding and processing relevant information to cope with unexpected scenarios. Conversely, people experiencing positive emotions seem to match to a less detail-oriented information-processing style, pending more relevant and/or rewarding stimuli within a benign and predictable environment. This results in the activation of re-constructive processes depending on existing knowledge and schematic heuristics [14]. In sum, people experiencing positive emotions may be more vulnerable to incorporate misleading information into memory than people experiencing negative emotions [12].
Studies exploring the relationship between FMs and levels of depression and anxiety provided mixed results. Some evidence suggested a negative association between depression and FMs and a positive association between anxiety and FMs [15,16]. Other investigations showed instead a positive relationship between depression and FMs formation [17].
Coping mechanisms, i.e., personality traits modulating the individual’s behavior in response to stressors, may also play a role in the development of FMs, although the available data are inconsistent. For instance, avoidance-coping strategies including massive stressor avoidance, in combination with denial and wishful thinking, were found to predict FMs construction [18]. Subsequent evidence has instead observed that negative coping strategies, such as avoidance and denial, were negatively associated with FMs; conversely, active coping strategies (e.g., positive reframing, planning) showed a positive association with the frequency of FMs [16]. More recently, some studies have reported that excessive use of help-seeking strategies (e.g., asking for advice, delegating to solve a problem, seeking social and moral support), in synergy with high-stress levels, would tend to facilitate FM formation [19,20].
Finally, high analytic skills, e.g., logical reasoning and critical thinking, may allow people to effectively monitor the sources of their memories, thus reducing the frequency of FMs [21]. In line with this claim, poor cognitive functioning has been associated with a higher likelihood of developing FMs [16]. However, contradictory findings are also available [22,23,24].
As widely acknowledged, the exposure to relevant social or historical events may enhance the tendency of generating FMs [25,26,27]. Clearly, the Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic is a major, unpredictable, and unprecedented calamity heavily challenging health, political, administrative, and journalistic authorities [28,29]. Although the psychological symptoms experienced by individuals during the quarantine have led to the development of severe attentional and memories difficulties per se [30,31], the probability to develop FMs has been increased also by the resulting generalized confusion in media communication, a fertile ground for the spreading of misinformation and fake news [3,32]. Fake news, as antecedents, and FMs as consequences, can affect the creditability of the healthcare system and reduce the adherence to public health guidelines [33], such as those relating to the vaccination plan [34,35]. Therefore, especially in the current historic period, it would have been relevant to investigate the relationship between individual variables, including those described above, and susceptibility to FMs after exposure to COVID-19 fake news. To the best of our knowledge, only one study addressed this issue [7].
The aim of this study was to investigate, in a sample of Italian university students, whether FM development is affected by individual/personological variables in terms of use of traditional and social media, COVID-19 perceived and objective knowledge, fear of the disease, depression and anxiety symptoms, reasoning skills, and coping mechanisms. As unfiltered, rough, or false information is often spread through social media [36,37], it is reasonable to hypothesize that a higher use of social media facilitates the generation of FMs, particularly in young individuals [38]. However, higher objective knowledge about COVID-19 could represent a significant mediator across this relationship, with more knowledgeable individuals being less susceptible to FMs following the use of social media.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

G*Power 3.1.9.4 was used to perform an a priori power analysis for determining the number of participants needed according to linear regression. At a nominal alpha level of 0.05, power (1 − β) set to 0.80, large effect size (f2 = 0.35), and a number of predictors ranging from 1 to 14, the required total sample size was between 25 and 66.
Convenience sampling method was used. Participants were recruited via web through email, social media posts, and word-of-mouth. A total of 205 Italian University students were surveyed, 200 of whom completed the experimental protocol in full (97.6%, 150 females). Participants were attending the degree courses in a variety of disciplines including medicine, psychology, engineering, and economics.

2.2. Procedure

Participants filled in a survey in the Italian language, which was constructed using the Google Forms platform. Respondents took about 25 min to complete the questionnaire. In the months of February and March 2021, the study rationale and methodological approach were defined. Data collection started on 16 April 2021 and ended on 8 May 2021. The survey consisted of several blocks.
First, participants were informed about the study aim, i.e., investigating knowledge and memories about COVID-19 and their association with certain individual variables. Then, participants were asked to provide demographic information (i.e., sex, age, and years of education) and whether they got the Coronavirus infection.
In the second block, participants were required to rate, on a 6-point Likert scale, the frequency of use of traditional media (e.g., newspaper articles, television) and social media (e.g., Facebook, Instagram, Twitter) for acquiring information about COVID-19 news and discussion (0 = “not at all”, 5 = “a lot”). Furthermore, they reported how afraid they were of contracting, and that their loved ones contracting, the virus (0 = “not at all”, 5 = “a lot”). Finally, perceived knowledge (PK) about COVID-19 was assessed by asking participants to judge, on the same 0–5 Likert scale, how competent they considered themselves to be about COVID-19 (in terms of studies, events, statistics, and political/administrative measures).
In the third block, COVID-19 objective knowledge (OK) was measured through an ad hoc devised questionnaire consisting of eight statements (4 true and 4 false) about COVID-19 (true, e.g., “The first clinical manifestations of the infection are generally fever, dry cough and muscle pain”, “Women are less affected by the infection compared with men”; false, e.g., “Quarantine is a precautionary isolation period lasting 40 days”, “There are many drugs that are effective in treating the infection”). The questionnaire was constructed by a pneumologist in cooperation with two psychologists experienced in psychometrics and research methodology. One point was given for each statement correctly recognized as true or false (maximum OK score = 8).
In the fourth block, levels of depression and anxiety were assessed by using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI [39,40]) and the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI [41]), respectively. The BDI is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 21 statements exploring overt behavioral manifestations and symptomatic areas of depression in the last weeks. The severity of symptoms is evaluated on a Likert scale ranging from 0 to 3, with a higher score indicating higher levels of depression [41]. The BAI is a self-report questionnaire devised for the quantitative assessment of anxiety symptoms that occurred during the past week. The inventory consists of 21 items describing the emotional, physiological, and cognitive symptoms of anxiety. Each item is rated on a 0–3 Likert scale, with a higher score reflecting severer anxiety symptoms [42].
In the fifth block, coping mechanisms were measured through the COPE-New Italian Version (COPE-NVI-25 [43,44,45]). This is a 25-item tool assessing the frequency of use of coping mechanisms in difficult or stressful situations. Items are organized in five subscales: avoidance strategies (5 items), transcendent orientation (4 items), positive attitude (6 items), social support (5 items), and problem solving (5 items). The response format for each item is based on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (“I do not usually do this”) to 6 (“I do it almost all the time”).
In the sixth block, participants completed the Italian version of the Cognitive Estimation Task (CET [46]). This task was devised to test reasoning and self-monitoring abilities. It is composed of 21 questions that are not immediately answerable; however, the response can be provided using previous knowledge. According to the absolute error scoring procedure, participants were awarded points, from 0 to 2, depending on the accuracy of the estimates provided: the higher the score, the poorer the performance. Bizarre responses are scored 0 or 1 (see Della Sala et al. [46] for additional details).
In the last block, eight COVID-19-related news were presented. Four were fake news (e.g., Flu vaccine news, “A recent study conducted by the Agostino Gemelli University Polyclinic in Rome has shown that 85% of patients who received flu vaccine were less likely to contract the COVID-19 for at least 4 months following the vaccination”) and four were true news (e.g., Surfaces news, “According to a recent Australian study published in the Virology Journal, a sample of SARS-CoV-2, in conditions of ideal humidity and temperature, survives on some surfaces such as glass, steel and banknotes for up to 28 days”, see Appendix A). Fake news were totally fabricated and constructed after a careful examination of possible fake news spread on the web and television. Following this approach, the reporting of news items to which the subjects could have actually been exposed was prevented. Conversely, true news were extrapolated from information provided by the websites of the World Health Organization (WHO) and the Italian Ministry of Health. Participants were asked whether they remembered (“I remember this”) or not (“I do not remember this”) each news item. If participants reported remembering the news, they were asked to specify the source memory, i.e., where/how they acquired the information, i.e., traditional or social media. Note that each news was presented together with an image since previous studies showed that photographic stimuli tended to increase the generation of FMs [47]. One point was assigned for each remembered true/fake news, resulting in a True Memory (TM) and a False Memory (FM) score, which both ranged from 0 to 4.
From the second to the sixth block, the order of presentation of blocks was randomized within the subjects. The eight news were also presented in random order. Informed consent was obtained from all the participants involved in the study immediately after the illustration of the study’s objectives. This study was performed according to the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli” (protocol code: 17/2021, date of approval by the Department of Psychology: 13 April 2021).

2.3. Statistical Analyses

First, we compared the response rates and the source type for each of the eight news item by means of one-way chi-squared test (χ2). The Phi coefficient (φ = χ 2 / N ) was used as a measure of the strength of the association between the two levels of each nominal variable and therefore to compute the χ2 effect size [48].
Then, Spearman’s correlation analysis (rrho) was used for exploratory purposes. More specifically, we correlated the TM and FM scores with the frequency of use of traditional and social media, fear of contracting—and fear that loved ones could contract—COVID-19, PK and OK scores, BDI and BAI scores, CET score, and the five COPE-NVI-25 sub-scores. The Cohen’s conventions [49] were used to interpret the effect size (weak, rrho < 0.30; moderate, rrho = 0.30–0.50; strong, rrho > 0.50).
Subsequently, we constructed fourteen simple linear regression models, where TM and FM scores entered individually into each model as dependent variables, whereas media usage, PK score, OK score, and all psychological variables were assumed as potential predictors. Hence, the variables that independently explained a sufficient proportion of variance of TM and FM scores were entered into a simultaneous multiple regression model in order to identify the most influential predictors of both TM and FM scores.
Finally, to investigate whether and how OK mediated the relationship between the frequencies of use of social and traditional media and the creation of TMs and FMs (i.e., TM and FM scores), we carried out two mediation analyses, entering TM score and FM scores as dependent variables, the frequencies of use of traditional and social media as predictors, and OK score as parallel mediator. In order to evaluate the significance of direct and indirect effects, bootstrapping procedure with 5000 samples with replacement from the full sample to construct bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals (hereafter 95% CI; LL = lower level of confidence interval, UL = upper level of confidence interval) was conducted by SPSS Macro PROCESS [50].
All statistical analyses were performed by IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 26.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Data Cleaning

Eight subjects were excluded due to a large number of missing values; moreover, 15 univariate outliers, i.e., subjects obtaining z-scores equal to, or greater than, 3 in absolute value were removed [51]. Descriptive statistics expressed as mean and standard deviation on a total of 177 participants (131 females; M age = 24.34, SD = 2.78, age range = 20–39; M years of schooling based on the degree awarded = 14.39, SD = 1.50, education range = 13–16) were computed for each variable under examination (see Table 1). Only 18 participants (8.8%) reported having caught the infection before participation in the study.

3.2. True and False Memories for COVID-19-Related News

Participants obtained a TM score significantly higher than the FM score (TM score, M = 1.89, SD = 0.97; FM score, M = 0.75, SD = 0.84; t(176) = 12.181, p < 0.001). They also reported a more frequent use of traditional media rather than social media for news and insights regarding COVID-19 (traditional media, M = 3.56, SD = 1.19; social media, M = 3.18, SD = 1.37; t(176) = 3.265, p = 0.001).
The response rates for each true and fake news and information about source memory are summarized in Table 2. Overall, participants did not show a clear memory for the true news, except for the Symptoms news which was correctly recalled by 84.2% of participants (n = 177, χ2(1) = 82.718, p < 0.001, φ = 0.68). As for the Surfaces news, about half of participants (53.7%) did not remember seeing/hearing it (n = 177, χ2(1) = 0.955, p = 0.33); conversely, the majority of participants did not remember the Immunization (n = 177, χ2(1) = 74.718, p < 0.001, φ = 0.65) and Monoclonal antibodies news (n = 177, χ2(1) = 5.429, p = 0.02, φ = 0.17). Participants who correctly recalled the true news indicated having seen/heard them mainly through traditional media (Immunization, n = 31, χ2(1) = 7.258, p = 0.007, φ = 0.48; Monoclonal antibodies, n = 73, χ2(1) = 23.027, p < 0.001, φ = 0.56; Symptoms, n = 149, χ2(1) = 23.362, p < 0.001, φ = 0.39), apart from the Surfaces news (n = 82, χ2(1) = 1.756, p = 0.18). As concerns the fake news, most of the participants did not remember seeing/hearing them (n = 177, Flu vaccine, χ2(1) = 50.989, p < 0.001, φ = 0.54; Serological tests, χ2(1) = 67.124, p < 0.001, φ = 0.61; Masks, χ2(1) = 135.734, p < 0.001, φ = 0.87; Pets, χ2(1) = 40.819, p < 0.001, φ = 0.48). Participants who recalled the fake news, i.e., who reported FMs, indicated traditional media as primary source (69.3%) only for the Flu vaccine news (n = 41, χ2(1) = 5.488, p = 0.02, φ = 0.36); for the remaining fake news, no difference was found between use of traditional and social media (Serological tests, n = 34, χ2(1) = 1.059, p = 0.30; Masks, n = 11, χ2(1) = 2.273, p = 0.13; Pets, n = 11, χ2(1) = 0.348, p = 0.55).

3.3. Correlation Analysis

TM score showed weak, but significant, positive correlations with use of social media (rrho = 0.28, p < 0.001), fear of contracting COVID-19 (rrho = 0.17, p = 0.02), and fear that loved ones contracting COVID-19 (rrho = 0.18, p = 0.02). Furthermore, TM score correlated moderately, and positively, with use of traditional media (rrho = 0.34, p < 0.001) and OK score (rrho = 0.30, p < 0.001). FM score showed, instead, a negative moderate association only with OK score (rrho = −0.35, p < 0.001).

3.4. Predictors of True and False Memory Scores

As concerns the TM score, simple regression analysis revealed that higher use of traditional (R2 = 0.13, B = 0.30, p < 0.001) and social media (R2 = 0.08, B = 0.20, p < 0.001), higher OK score (R2 = 0.08, B = 0.19, p < 0.001), greater fear of contracting (R2 = 0.03, B = 0.16, p = 0.02) and loved ones contracting COVID-19 (R2 = 0.03, B = 0.20, p = 0.03) predicted a significant increase in TM score. When these variables entered a simultaneous multiple regression analysis, only the use of traditional media (B = 0.24, p < 0.001), use of social media (B = 0.13, p = 0.009), and OK score (B = 0.13, p = 0.004) remained significant, with the regression model that was able to account for 24% of the variance in TM score (F(5, 171) = 11.031, p < 0.001, see Table 3).
As concerns the FM score, simple regression analysis revealed that a higher OK score (R2 = 0.15, B = −0.22, p < 0.001) and a greater fear that loved ones would contract COVID-19 (R2 = 0.02, B = −0.16, p = 0.05) predicted a significant decrease in FM score. In multiple regression analysis, the OK score remained the only significant predictor (B = −0.22, p < 0.001), with the regression model being able to account for 16% of the variance in FM score (F(2, 174) = 16.716, p < 0.001, see Table 3).

3.5. Mediation Analysis

Results of multiple regression analyses further justified the construction of two mediation models for testing the possible mediation effect of the OK score on the relationship between the frequencies of use of social and traditional media and TM and FM scores. Indeed, OK is the only predictor common to both TM and FM scores.
More use of social media was related to a higher OK score (B = 0.183, p = 0.025), but no significant relationship between the use of traditional media and OK score (B = 0.097, p = 0.299) was found. Furthermore, a higher OK score was associated with an increase in TM score (i.e., more true memories; B = 0.142, p = 0.002, Figure 1) and a decrease in FM score (i.e., less false memories; B = −0.244, p < 0.001, Figure 2).
The 95% bias-corrected CI based on 5000 bootstrap samples revealed that the indirect effects of the use of social media on both TM and FM scores through the OK score were significant (TM: Estimate effect: 0.026; 95% CI: 0.002, 0.057; FM: Estimate effect: −0.045; 95% CI: −0.092, −0.003).

4. Discussion

This study aimed at identifying the variables that most contributed to FMs for COVID-19-related fake news. Evidence provided by previous research was used to interpret our findings, although the main term of comparison for the discussion of our results was the study by Greene and Murphy [7], which was—to the best of our knowledge—the only contribution on this issue.
In line with Greene and Murphy, our participants recalled, on average, more true than fake news (1.89/4 vs. 0.75/4). Nevertheless, they showed poor memory also for the true news (Surfaces, Immunization, Monoclonal antibodies), apart from the Symptoms news (see Appendix A), which was correctly recalled by about 85% of the participants. This finding is likely due to the effectiveness of the awareness-raising campaign on the virus, which made information about the onset, the course, and the outcome of COVID-19 highly salient.
Compared with previous investigations [7,26,27], the novelty of our study lies in having separately computed the use of traditional and social media to get information. In this respect, a higher use of traditional than social media was reported. Interestingly, traditional media were judged as the main source of information for most of the true news, whereas no significant mismatch was found between traditional and social media in participants who recalled the fake news.
About the effect of individual variables, our study provides evidence not fully in agreement with Greene and Murphy’s study. Consistent with their findings, we found that fear of COVID-19 increased the TM score; however, we also found a negative association between fear that loved ones could contract COVID-19 and the number of FMs. These results further support the hypothesis that negative emotional valence is a powerful catalyzer of memory abilities, i.e., it can improve encoding and maintenance of information, thereby reducing reconstructive memory errors and thus the probability of generating FMs [7,12,14,52]. The selective effect that fear of loved ones contracting COVID had in reducing FMs might be explained by the empathy-mediated willingness to help [53]. In support of this hypothesis, converging evidence coming from neuroimaging studies suggesting that the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is strictly involved in state empathy and prosocial behaviors [53,54,55] as well as in encoding and consolidation of memory traces [56], with particular reference to fear memories [56,57,58,59]. This neural overlap underlies a cognitive interaction across social and memory abilities. The mPFC was found to support the ability to recall the most appropriate motor and/or emotional response to specific events in a particular place and time. The ventral portion of mPFC is strongly interconnected with the anterior insula, which is involved in pain perception [60] and with the later habenula that, in concert with the Ventral Tegmental Area (VTA) and other regions of the dopamine network, plays a pivotal role in activating learned and adaptive responses to stressful/painful and to rewarding/motivationally salient events [61,62,63,64,65].
It is important to underline that the selective effect of the fear of loved ones contracting COVID-19—rather than contracting it personally—might be additionally explained by the fact that our sample consisted of young adults; therefore, they were less concerned about themselves than their older loved ones, who were more at risk for severe COVID-19 illness [28]. In sum, we hypothesize that our finding is likely explained by a prosociality-induced positive bias: the fear that a loved one may become ill, suffer, or even die leads to more carefully selecting the information disseminated about the disease, to filter them more thoroughly, and to construct more robust and durable mnestic traces in order to functionally cope with a potential emergency situation. When the goal is protecting others, people may be more motivated and committed, thus producing fewer cognitive errors.
Previous research highlighted the role of cognitive ability in affecting FM formation, with a particular focus on analytic reasoning [21]. More specifically, it has been suggested that as cognitive skills increase, the probability of reporting FMs decreases [16]. Intriguingly, two studies conducted by Greene and Murphy’s research group, one on FMs for fake news among Brexit voters [27] and the other on FMs for COVID-19 fake news [7], showed that higher levels of analytical reasoning were associated with fewer FMs as well as fewer TMs. In other words, their participants were less likely to report a memory for any given news. Authors hypothesized that individuals with higher analytical skills may show a greater degree of suspicion towards all news. Indeed, they may require further evidence before reporting a memory for a piece of information that sounded familiar; this translates into a clear response bias [7,27]. Therefore, it has been suggested that higher analytic skills do not allow to efficiently discriminate between memories for true and fake news [27]. In addition, note that some studies reported no effect of cognitive abilities on FMs. For instance, in the study by Powers, Andriks, and Loftus [22], no relationship between susceptibility to FMs and cognitive/intellectual abilities, assessed through the Washington Pre-College Test (WPCT), was found. Still, Nichols and Loftus [24] found no correlation between FMs and the three-item version of the Cognitive Reflection Task (CRT), which was used to assess inhibition, analytical, and problem-solving capabilities. Interestingly, a seven-item version of the same task was used by Greene and Murphy in their study on COVID-19. Some methodological discrepancies (e.g., sample size, age of participants, assessment methods used) may explain the different patterns of results. Similar to Nichols and Loftus, we found no relationship between analytic skills and FM counts in a sample characterized by overlapping demographic features. Instead, compared with Greene and Murphy’s study, our participants were younger and only 18% of them demonstrated poor analytic skills (CET cut-off > 18, [46]). Conversely, participants tested by Greene and Murphy showed, on average, lower analytic abilities (M CRT score = 3.52, SD = 1.91, max CRT score = 7, [7]). Thus, dissimilar samples’ characteristics and assessment techniques may account for the difference in results. Future research should further explore the possible relationship between cognitive functioning and the construction of FMs, possibly using a standardized and ubiquitously accepted evaluation protocol.
Neither the contribution of anxiety and depressive symptoms nor the role of personality traits in FM formation were explored in Greene and Murphy’s study. Previous research investigating the relationship between anxiety and FMs provided mixed results. Some studies found a positive association between anxiety and FMs [18,66], whereas other studies found no relationship [67,68,69]. Our results provide no evidence for a significant association between anxiety and memory biases and add to the previous literature suggesting that individuals suffering from anxiety (e.g., social anxiety, specific phobias) did not show a higher rate of FMs [69].
Studies assessing the effect of depression on FMs formation also provided conflicting results. Some evidence suggested that individuals with depression generated fewer FMs [15]; other investigations showed instead a positive relationship between depression and FMs [17,52,70]. Unlike previous research, we found no association between depression and frequency of FMs. This result might be due to the small number of participants (n = 10) obtaining a BDI score higher than normative cutoff (BDI ≥ 27, [40]).
As for the role of personality traits, some studies reported a significant association between active [16] or negative coping strategies [16,18,19,20] and FM formation, while other ones detected no relationship between personality factors and FMs [24,71]. Our results add to this latter evidence. However, it is important to underline that a shorter inventory, as the COPE-NVI-25, might suffer from poorer reliability than longer assessment tools due to the lower scores’ variance. Further studies are needed to test this hypothesis.
In the current study, the effect of both PK and OK on COVID-19-related information was also assessed. We found that OK was significantly associated with both TM and FM scores, whereas no effect of PK was detected. This result is at odds with previous evidence showing that PK might increase TMs [7] or memory biases [5] in subjects who rated themselves as experts. However, our finding reinforces the assumption that perceiving oneself as an expert on a certain topic does not necessarily mean that one actually is. Indeed, PK is closely related to the frequency of exposure to, and thus familiarity with, the information, whereas OK implies greater expertise, i.e., high-level competence and skills acquired on a particular task or within a given domain [72]. In line with this claim, several studies in the field of health care [73,74,75] and marketing [72,76] have reported weak or no association between PK and OK. Although, according to our results, FMs are unaffected by PK, overclaiming and overconfidence remain phenomena to be kept under control, especially when they involve public health issues. To achieve this goal, it would be necessary to promote the “knowledge calibration”, i.e., the correspondence between self-assessed and actual knowledge [72]. It is important to note that the different methodological approaches employed to evaluate PK and OK (i.e., one item rated on a Likert scale vs. an eight-item true/false questionnaire, respectively) could account for the observed dissociation. As a result, additional studies are needed to clarity the relation between PK and memory biases.
Similar to Greene and Murphy, OK was found to be one of the best predictors of TM score and the only variable explaining a sufficient proportion of the FM score’s variance. In particular, OK increased the TM and decreased the FM score. Accordingly, a quantitative estimate of the knowledge about a certain topic is likely the most suitable measure of the ability to discriminate between true and fake news. Furthermore, this observation provides additional support to the MMF, which underlines the key role of expertise for critically and systematically evaluating the contents of an alleged memory [10].
Apart from OK, we found that both the frequency of use of traditional and social media predicted a significant increase in TM score. Furthermore, mediation analyses showed that OK mediated significantly the relationships between the use of social media and development of both TMs and FMs. Particularly, OK mediated positively the relationship between the use of social media and TMs, whereas the effect of OK was negative in mediating the relationship between the use of social media and FMs.
These findings suggest reconsidering the idea of social media as the main source of fake news, especially compared to traditional media that are usually considered more credible and objective [77]. Indeed, an increased use of social media was associated with a higher probability of reporting TMs as well as with a higher OK score. In turn, OK seems to play a crucial mediating role since it strengthens the formation of TMs and reduces the development of FMs following use of social media. Hence, the knowledge about the topic rather than the type of sources from which people choose to inform themselves would make the difference in the process of memories formation. This hypothesis is further supported by the lack of substantial differences between the use of traditional and social media among participants reporting FMs. The absence of an OK-driven mediation effect binding the relationships between traditional media and memory scores might be precisely justified by the tendency to consider information spread by traditional media as more realistic and truthful [77], thus bringing about a more passive approach without active recourse to one’s own knowledge.
The current study is not free from limitations. First, the sample size is fairly small, although the number of participants included in the study was far greater than the number estimated by the power analysis. Second, our sample included only university students. Thus, our results are biased by a limited external validity, even more so considering that differences in years of age and schooling can significantly affect the use of traditional and social media. Furthermore, our participants reported how often they checked social media, such as Facebook, Twitter, or Instagram, to inform themselves about the COVID-19 issue. However, by using social networks, it is possible to access either gossip pages, the “voice” of influencers/bloggers and public opinion, or the official pages of journalists, politicians, and experts in medical science. Still, it is possible to be redirected to official websites of newspapers/TV news or to the vision of partial/integral texts of scientific indexed papers recognized by the scientific community. Consequently, the mere estimation of the frequency of use of social media may not have been sufficient or still biased by the heterogeneity of accessible content. A further limitation might be traced to the fact that participants were not asked to specify how many people they knew that contracted COVID-19, which would have strengthened the role of the fear of the disease. Finally, although the fake news were constructed after a careful examination of news spread by media, it is difficult to rule out the possibility that participants had already generated FMs for similar episodes/news, so that our experiment would have uncovered pre-existing FMs rather than implanting them. However, we consider this scenario quite unlikely.

5. Conclusions

The main findings of this study, which has examined the relationship between individual variables and FMs for COVID-19-related fake news, can be summarized as follows. Depression, anxiety, and personality traits seem to not affect FM formation. Furthermore, FMs appear also unaffected by reasoning skills, although the sample’s characteristics (i.e., highly educated young adults) may have produced a collapse of the variance. Overall, these findings add to previous evidence from mixed and confounding literature.
The fear of loved ones contracting COVID-19, rather than the fear of contracting it personally reduced the FM count. This finding has been interpreted as the consequence of an empathy-mediated prosociality bias boosting memory ability. Furthermore, this is in line with the hypothesis that people experiencing negative emotions are less vulnerable to misleading information and less likely to form FMs. However, the young age of our participants should be taken into account, since it is to be expected that younger individuals are more fearful of their older loved ones contracting the virus than they are of themselves.
OK was found to be a significant mediator of the relationship between social media usage and FM construction. Individuals with greater knowledge about COVID-19 might be more able to discriminate between reliable and fictional information spread by social media; consequently, they might be less susceptible to memory biases. OK mediated also the relationship between social media and TMs, with higher OK being associated with more TMs. However, a higher frequency of use of social media explained, per se, the increase in TM score, with OK strengthening this relationship. In sum, our results suggest that social media should be cleared of the negative connotation that they are the main sources of disinformation; indeed, a lot depends on the knowledge of those who make use of social media. In this respect, political and health authorities, together with journalistic institutions, are encouraged to actively use their social channels but paying attention to disseminating clear, precise, and unambiguous information regarding the progress of the COVID-19 pandemic, especially since social media posts can reach people with limited medical knowledge, poor education, susceptibility to stereotypes or prejudices, or who favor heuristic reasoning schema.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.S., C.R.I., M.L.M. and R.P.; Methodology, C.S., C.R.I. and A.I.; Formal Analysis and Data Curation, C.S., C.R.I. and G.M.; Investigation and Resources, C.S., C.R.I., M.L.M. and R.P.; Literature Review, C.S., C.R.I., F.A. and N.G.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, C.S., C.R.I. and F.A.; Writing—Review and Editing, G.M., A.I., G.B., N.G., M.L.M. and R.P.; Visualization, C.S. and C.R.I.; Supervision, M.L.M. and R.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli” (protocol code: n. 17/2021, date: 13 April 2021) and performed in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding authors.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Appendix A.1. Fake News

Figure A1. Flu vaccine.
Figure A1. Flu vaccine.
Brainsci 11 01489 g0a1
A recent study conducted by the Agostino Gemelli University Polyclinic in Rome has shown that 85% of patients who received flu vaccine were less likely to contract COVID-19 for at least 4 months following the vaccination.
Figure A2. Serological tests.
Figure A2. Serological tests.
Brainsci 11 01489 g0a2
In October 2020, the World Health Organization (WHO) announced that serological tests can, in the current state of the technological development, replace molecular and antigenic tests, as serological tests are able to detect both antibodies against the virus and the presence of COVID-19 in the body.
Figure A3. Masks.
Figure A3. Masks.
Brainsci 11 01489 g0a3
A study by the University College Dublin showed that 15% of Irish children under 10 years old experienced a temporary oxygen shortage and bacterial flora alteration following prolonged use of the masks.
Figure A4. Pets.
Figure A4. Pets.
Brainsci 11 01489 g0a4
In Belgium, some families decided to put down their domestic animals since they believed that the animals had gotten COVID-19 and could therefore transmit it to them.

Appendix A.2. True News

Figure A5. Surfaces.
Figure A5. Surfaces.
Brainsci 11 01489 g0a5
According to a recent Australian study published in the Virology Journal, a sample of SARS-CoV-2, in conditions of ideal humidity and temperature, survives on some surfaces such as glass, steel and banknotes for up to 28 days.
Figure A6. Immunization.
Figure A6. Immunization.
Brainsci 11 01489 g0a6
Based on the data provided by the “Istituto Svizzero per gli Agenti Terapeutici”, the protection offered by vaccines administered so far in Italy is over 60% from seven days after the second dose.
Figure A7. Monoclonal antibodies.
Figure A7. Monoclonal antibodies.
Brainsci 11 01489 g0a7
In Italy, monoclonal antibodies are being experimented with, which are medications that have been used with discreet success in the United States to treat COVID-19 patients.
Figure A8. Symptoms.
Figure A8. Symptoms.
Brainsci 11 01489 g0a8
In some patients, the presence of residual symptoms after negativization has been found, such as fatigue, short breath, respiratory difficulties, and osteoarticular pain.

References

  1. Roediger, H.L.; McDermott, K.B. Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1995, 21, 803–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Austin, J.L.; Strange, D.M. Television produces more false recognition for news than newspapers. Psychol. Pop. Media Cult. 2012, 1, 167–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. O’Connell, A.; Greene, C.M. Not strange but not true: Self-reported interest in a topic increases false memory. Memory 2017, 25, 969–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Apuke, O.D.; Omar, B. Fake news and COVID-19: Modelling the predictors of fake news sharing among social media users. Telemat. Inform. 2021, 56, 101475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Mehta, R.; Hoegg, J.; Chakravarti, A. Knowing too much: Expertise-induced false recall effects in product comparison. J. Consum. Res. 2011, 38, 535–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Frenda, S.J.; Knowles, E.D.; Saletan, W.; Loftus, E.F. False memories of fabricated political events. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 49, 280–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Greene, C.M.; Murphy, G. Individual differences in susceptibility to false memories for COVID-19 fake news. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2020, 5, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Chi, M.T.H.; Glaser, R.; Farr, M.J. The Nature of Expertise; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  9. Johnson, M.K.; Hashtroudi, S.; Lindsay, D.S. Source monitoring. Psychol. Bull. 1993, 114, 3–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Mitchell, K.J.; Johnson, M.K. Source monitoring: Attributing mental experiences. In The Oxford Handbook of Memory; Tulving, E., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 179–195. [Google Scholar]
  11. McGaugh, J.L. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 27, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Kaplan, R.L.; Van Damme, I.; Levine, L.J.; Loftus, E.F. Emotion and false memory. Emot. Rev. 2015, 8, 8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. English, S.M.; Nielson, K.A. Reduction of the misinformation effect by arousal induced after learning. Cognition 2010, 117, 237–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Bless, H. The interplay of affect and cognition: The mediating role of general knowledge structures. In Feeling and Thinking: The Role of Affect in Social Cognition; Studies in emotion and social interaction; 2nd Series; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 201–222. [Google Scholar]
  15. Storbeck, J.; Clore, G.L. With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness, false memory: Mood and the false memory effect. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 785–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Zhu, B.; Chen, C.; Loftus, E.F.; Lin, C.; He, Q.; Chen, C.; Li, H.; Xue, G.; Lu, Z.; Dong, Q. Individual differences in false memory from misinformation: Cognitive factors. Memory 2010, 18, 543–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Howe, M.L.; Malone, C. Mood-congruent true and false memory: Effects of depression. Memory 2011, 19, 192–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Gudjonsson, G.H. The relationship of intelligence and memory to interrogative suggestibility: The importance of range effects. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1988, 27, 185–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Babore, A.; Lombardi, L.; Viceconti, M.L.; Pignataro, S.; Marino, V.; Crudele, M.; Candelori, C.; Bramanti, S.M.; Trumello, C. Psychological effects of the COVID-2019 pandemic: Perceived stress and coping strategies among healthcare professionals. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 293, 113366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Rettie, H.; Daniels, J. Coping and tolerance of uncertainty: Predictors and mediators of mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Am. Psychol. 2021, 76, 427–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Pennycook, G.; Rand, D.G. Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition 2019, 188, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Powers, P.A.; Andriks, J.L.; Loftus, E.F. Eyewitness accounts of females and males. J. Appl. Psychol. 1979, 64, 339–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Salthouse, T.A.; Siedlecki, K.L. An individual difference analysis of false recognition. Am. J. Psychol. 2007, 120, 429–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Nichols, R.M.; Loftus, E.F. Who is susceptible in three false memory tasks? Memory 2019, 27, 962–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Greenberg, D.L. President Bush’s false [flashbulb] memory of 9/11/01. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2004, 18, 363–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Murphy, G.; Loftus, E.F.; Grady, R.H.; Levine, L.J.; Greene, C.M. False memories for fake news during ireland’s abortion referendum. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 30, 1449–1459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Greene, C.M.; Nash, R.A.; Murphy, G. Misremembering brexit: Partisan bias and individual predictors of false memories for fake news stories among Brexit voters. Memory 2021, 29, 587–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Ilardi, A.; Chieffi, S.; Iavarone, A.; Ilardi, C.R. SARS-CoV-2 in Italy: Population density correlates with morbidity and mortality. Jpn. J. Infect. Dis. 2020, 74, 61–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ilardi, A.; Chieffi, S.; Ilardi, C.R. Predictive role of population density and use of public transport for major outcomes of SARS-CoV-2 infection in the Italian population: An ecological study. J. Res. Health Sci. 2021, 21, e00518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Maggi, G.; Baldassarre, I.; Barbaro, A.; Cavallo, N.D.; Cropano, M.; Nappo, R.; Santangelo, G. Mental health status of Italian elderly subjects during and after quarantine for the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Psychogeriatrics 2021, 21, 540–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Santangelo, G.; Baldassarre, I.; Barbaro, A.; Cavallo, N.D.; Cropano, M.; Maggi, G.; Nappo, R.; Trojano, L.; Raimo, S. Subjective cognitive failures and their psychological correlates in a large Italian sample during quarantine/self-isolation for COVID-19. Neurol. Sci. 2021, 42, 2625–2635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Pennycook, G.; McPhetres, J.; Zhang, Y.; Lu, J.G.; Rand, D.G. Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: Experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 31, 770–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. O’Connor, C.; Murphy, M. Going viral: Doctors must tackle fake news in the COVID-19 pandemic. BMJ 2020, 369, 1587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Barello, S.; Nania, T.; Dellafiore, F.; Graffigna, G.; Caruso, R. ‘Vaccine hesitancy’ among university students in Italy during the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2020, 35, 781–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Troiano, G.; Nardi, A. Vaccine hesitancy in the era of COVID-19. Public Health 2021, 194, 245–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lazer, D.M.J.; Baum, M.A.; Benkler, Y.; Berinsky, A.J.; Greenhill, K.M.; Menczer, F.; Metzger, M.J.; Nyhan, B.; Pennycook, G.; Rothschild, D.; et al. The science of fake news. Science 2018, 359, 1094–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Wilson, S.L.; Wiysonge, C. Social media and vaccine hesitancy. BMJ Global Health 2020, 5, e004206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Auxier, B.; Anderson, M. Social Media Use in 2021; Pew Research Center: Washington, DC, USA, 2021; pp. 1–19. [Google Scholar]
  39. Beck, A.T.; Ward, C.H.; Mendelson, M.; Mock, J.; Erbaugh, J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1961, 4, 561–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Garofalo, E.; Iavarone, A.; Chieffi, S.; Carpinelli Mazzi, M.; Gamboz, N.; Ambra, F.I.; Sannino, M.; Galeone, F.; Esposito, S.; Ronga, B.; et al. Italian version of the Starkstein Apathy Scale (SAS-I) and a shortened version (SAS-6) to assess “pure apathy” symptoms: Normative study on 392 individuals. Neurol. Sci. 2021, 42, 1065–1072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Beck, A.T.; Steer, R.A. Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI). In Überblick über Reliabilitäts-und Validitätsbefunde von klinischen und außerklinischen Selbst-und Fremdbeurteilungsverfahren; Ratzek, M., von Hauenschild, L., Eds.; Public Health Editions, 1988; Volume 7. [Google Scholar]
  42. Beck, A.T.; Epstein, N.; Brown, G.; Steer, R.A. An Inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1988, 56, 893–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Carver, C.S. You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: Consider the brief COPE. Int. J. Behav. Med. 1997, 4, 92–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Sica, C.; Magni, C.; Ghisi, M.; Altoè, G.; Sighinolfi, C.; Chiri, L.R.; Franceschini, S. Coping orientation to problems experienced-nuova versione italiana (COPE-NVI): Uno strumento per la misura degli stili di coping. Psicoter. Cogn. E Comport. 2008, 14, 27–53. [Google Scholar]
  45. Foà, C.; Tonarelli, A.; Caricati, L.; Fruggeri, L. COPE-NVI-25: Validazione italiana della versione ridotta della Coping Orientation to the Problems Experienced (COPE-NVI). Psicol. Della Salut. 2015, 2, 123–140. [Google Scholar]
  46. Della Sala, S.; MacPherson, S.E.; Phillips, L.H.; Sacco, L.; Spinnler, H. How many camels are there in Italy? Cognitive estimates standardised on the Italian population. Neurol. Sci. 2003, 24, 10–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Strange, D.; Garry, M.; Bernstein, D.M.; Lindsay, D.S. Photographs cause false memories for the news. Acta Psychol. 2011, 136, 90–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Ilardi, C.R.; Garofalo, E.; Chieffi, S.; Gamboz, N.; La Marra, M.; Iavarone, A. Daily exposure to digital displays may affect the clock-drawing test: From psychometrics to serendipity. Neurol. Sci. 2020, 41, 3683–3690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Preacher, K.J.; Hayes, A.F. Contemporary approaches to assessing mediation in communication research. In The SAGE Sourcebook of Advanced Data Analysis Methods for Communication Research; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2008; pp. 13–54. [Google Scholar]
  51. Ilardi, C.R.; Chieffi, S.; Scuotto, C.; Gamboz, N.; Galeone, F.; Sannino, M.; Garofalo, E.; La Marra, M.; Ronga, B.; Iavarone, A. The frontal assessment battery 20 years later: Normative data for a shortened version (FAB15). Neurol. Sci. 2021. online ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Bookbinder, S.H.; Brainerd, C.J. Emotion and false memory: The context-content paradox. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 142, 1315–1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Gaesser, B. Constructing memory, imagination, and empathy: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Front. Psychol. 2013, 3, 576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Mathur, V.A.; Harada, T.; Lipke, T.; Chiao, J.Y. Neural basis of extraordinary empathy and altruistic motivation. Neuroimage 2010, 51, 1468–1475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Masten, C.L.; Morelli, S.A.; Eisenberger, N.I. An fMRI investigation of empathy for “social pain” and subsequent prosocial behavior. Neuroimage 2011, 55, 381–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Euston, D.R.; Gruber, A.J.; McNaughton, B.L. The role of medial prefrontal cortex in memory and decision making. Neuron 2012, 76, 1057–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Corcoran, K.A.; Quirk, G.J. Recalling safety: Cooperative functions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus in extinction. CNS Spectr. 2007, 12, 200–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Quinn, J.J.; Ma, Q.D.; Tinsley, M.R.; Koch, C.; Fanselow, M.S. Inverse temporal contributions of the dorsal hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to the expression of long-term fear memories. Learn. Mem. 2008, 15, 368–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Izquierdo, I.; Furini, C.R.G.; Myskiw, J.C. Fear memory. Physiol. Rev. 2016, 96, 695–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Jasmin, L.; Granato, A.; Ohara, P.T. Rostral agranular insular cortex and pain areas of the central nervous system: A tract-tracing study in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 2004, 468, 425–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Villano, I.; Ilardi, C.R.; Arena, S.; Scuotto, C.; Gleijeses, M.; Messina, G.; Messina, A.; Monda, V.; Monda, M.; Iavarone, A.; et al. Obese subjects without eating disorders experience binge episodes also independently of emotional eating and personality traits among university students of southern Italy. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Hikosaka, O. The habenula: From stress evasion to value-based decision-making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 503–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. La Marra, M.; Caviglia, G.; Perrella, R. Using smartphones when eating increases caloric intake in young people: An overview of the literature. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 587886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Itoi, K.; Sugimoto, N. The brainstem noradrenergic systems in stress, anxiety and depression. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2010, 22, 355–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Kranz, G.S.; Kasper, S.; Lanzenberger, R. Reward and the serotonergic system. Neuroscience 2010, 166, 1023–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Zoellner, L.A.; Foa, E.B.; Brigidi, B.D.; Przeworski, A. Are trauma victims susceptible to “false memories”? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2000, 109, 517–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Rapee, R.M.; McCallum, S.L.; Melville, L.F.; Ravenscroft, H.; Rodney, J.M. Memory bias in social phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 1994, 32, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Thorpe, S.J.; Salkovskis, P.M. Recall and recognition memory for spider information. J. Anxiety Disord. 2000, 14, 359–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Wenzel, A.; Jostad, C.; Brendle, J.R.; Ferraro, F.R.; Lystad, C.M. An investigation of false memories in anxious and fearful individuals. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 2004, 32, 257–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Joormann, J.; Teachman, B.A.; Gotlib, I.H. Sadder and less accurate? False memory for negative material in depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2009, 118, 412–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  71. Calvillo, D.P.; Rutchick, A.M. Political knowledge reduces hindsight memory distortion in election judgements. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2014, 26, 213–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Alba, J.W.; Hutchinson, J.W. Knowledge calibration: What consumers know and what they think they know. J. Consum. Res. 2000, 27, 123–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Awad, S.S.; Liscum, K.R.; Aoki, N.; Awad, S.H.; Berger, D.H. Does the subjective evaluation of medical student surgical knowledge correlate with written and oral exam performance? J. Surg. Res. 2002, 104, 36–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Rock, E.M.; Ireland, M.; Resnick, M.D.; McNeely, C.A. A rose by any other name? Objective knowledge, perceived knowledge, and adolescent male condom use. Pediatrics 2005, 115, 667–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Kobos, E.; Imiela, J.; Kryczka, T.; Szewczyk, A.; Knoff, B. Actual and perceived knowledge of type 1 diabetes mellitus among school nurses. Nurse Educ. Today 2020, 87, 104304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Han, T.I. Objective knowledge, subjective knowledge, and prior experience of organic cotton apparel. Fash. Text. 2019, 6, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Johnson, T.J.; Kaye, B.K. Reasons to believe: Influence of credibility on motivations for using social networks. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 50, 544–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Scheme of the mediation effect of objective knowledge in the relationship between social media and true memories (* p < 0.05).
Figure 1. Scheme of the mediation effect of objective knowledge in the relationship between social media and true memories (* p < 0.05).
Brainsci 11 01489 g001
Figure 2. Scheme of the mediation effect of objective knowledge in the relationship between social media and false memories (* p < 0.05).
Figure 2. Scheme of the mediation effect of objective knowledge in the relationship between social media and false memories (* p < 0.05).
Brainsci 11 01489 g002
Table 1. Descriptive statistics.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics.
VariablesMeanSDRange
TM score1.890.970–4
FM score0.750.840–3
Use of traditional media3.561.190–5
Use of social media3.181.370–5
PK score3.190.861–5
OK score5.201.451–8
Fear of contracting COVID-193.231.071–5
Fear of others contracting COVID-194.420.771–5
BDI score12.157.690–37
BAI score15.8111.650–44
CET score20.788.993–62
COPE—Avoidance Strategies9.483.484–21
COPE—Transcendent Orientation5.683.354–19
COPE—Positive Attitude24.735.1111–36
COPE—Social Support18.955.875–30
COPE—Problem Solving21.054.677–30
TM: True Memory; FM: False Memory; PK: Perceived Knowledge; OK: Objective Knowledge; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; BAI: Beck Anxiety Inventory; CET: Cognitive Estimation Task; COPE: COPE-New Italian Version-25.
Table 2. Response rates for true and fake news and source types for the recalled news.
Table 2. Response rates for true and fake news and source types for the recalled news.
Responses
Frequency (%)
Source Memory
Frequency (%)
News
True news
SurfacesImmunizationMonoclonal antibodiesSymptoms
I do not remember this 95 (53.7)146 (82.5)104 (58.8)28 (15.8)
I remember this 82 (46.3)31 (17.5)73 (41.2)149 (84.2)
Traditional media47 (57.3)23 (74.2)57 (78.08)104 (69.8)
Social media35 (42.7)8 (25.8)16 (21.9)45 (30.2)
Fake news
Flu vaccineSerological testsMasksPets
I do not remember this 136 (76.8)143 (80.8)166 (93.8)131 (74.0)
I remember this 41 (23.2)34 (19.2)11 (6.2)46 (26.0)
Traditional media28 (69.3)20 (58.8)8 (72.7)21 (45.6)
Social media13 (31.7)14 (41.2)3 (27.3)25 (54.3)
Table 3. Results of simple and multiple regression analyses.
Table 3. Results of simple and multiple regression analyses.
PredictorsSimple Regression AnalysisMultiple Regression Analysis
BtpR2BtpR2
True Memories
Use of traditional media0.3025.230<0.0010.1350.2434.253<0.0010.244
Use of social media0.2043.964<0.0010.0820.1312.6460.009
PK score0.1261.4900.1380.013
OK score0.1883.868<0.0010.0790.1352.9400.004
Fear of contracting COVID-190.1592.3570.0200.0310.1461.9370.064
Fear of others contracting COVID-190.2042.1490.0300.026–0.017–0.1570.875
BDI score−0.007−0.7650.4460.003
BAI score−0.004−0.6600.5100.002
CET score−0.009−1.1290.2610.007
COPE—Avoidance Strategies0.0130.6050.5460.002
COPE—Transcendent Orientation−0.021−0.9490.3440.005
COPE—Positive Attitude−0.018−1.2890.1990.009
COPE—Social Support0.0030.2650.7920.000
COPE—Problem Solving−0.014−0.9110.3630.005
False Memories
Use of traditional media0.0701.3250.1870.010 0.161
Use of social media0.0300.6490.5170.002
PK score0.0720.9800.3290.005
OK score−0.225−5.580<0.0010.151−0.217−5.364<0.001
Fear of contracting COVID-19−0.088−1.4990.1360.013
Fear of others contracting COVID-19−0.164−2.0060.0460.022−0.111−1.4510.149
BDI score0.0010.1380.8900.000
BAI score0.0050.9230.3570.005
CET score0.0081.1140.2670.007
COPE—Avoidance Strategies0.0070.3830.7020.001
COPE—Transcendent Orientation−0.003−0.1740.8620.000
COPE—Positive Attitude0.0070.5630.5740.002
COPE—Social Support−0.014−1.3210.1880.010
COPE—Problem Solving−0.006−0.4560.6490.001
PK: Perceived Knowledge; OK: Objective Knowledge; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; BAI: Beck Anxiety Inventory; CET: Cognitive Estimation Task; COPE: COPE-New Italian Version-25.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Scuotto, C.; Ilardi, C.R.; Avallone, F.; Maggi, G.; Ilardi, A.; Borrelli, G.; Gamboz, N.; La Marra, M.; Perrella, R. Objective Knowledge Mediates the Relationship between the Use of Social Media and COVID-19-Related False Memories. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1489. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111489

AMA Style

Scuotto C, Ilardi CR, Avallone F, Maggi G, Ilardi A, Borrelli G, Gamboz N, La Marra M, Perrella R. Objective Knowledge Mediates the Relationship between the Use of Social Media and COVID-19-Related False Memories. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(11):1489. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111489

Chicago/Turabian Style

Scuotto, Chiara, Ciro Rosario Ilardi, Francesco Avallone, Gianpaolo Maggi, Alfonso Ilardi, Giovanni Borrelli, Nadia Gamboz, Marco La Marra, and Raffaella Perrella. 2021. "Objective Knowledge Mediates the Relationship between the Use of Social Media and COVID-19-Related False Memories" Brain Sciences 11, no. 11: 1489. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111489

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop