The Role of Expectation and Beliefs on the Effects of Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation and Variability in Experimental Data
1.2. Placebo and Nocebo Effects
2. The Need of Blinding in Sham Protocols
2.1. Sham Stimulation: Definition and Open Issues
2.2. Blinding Success and Failure in NIBS Studies
3. The Role of Expectation in Shaping the Effects of NIBS
3.1. The Role of Contextual Factors in Shaping Outcome
3.2. Placebo and Nocebo Effects of TMS
3.3. Placebo and Nocebo Effects of tDCS
4. Toward a Systematic Assessment of Participants’ Expectations in NIBS Studies
5. Concluding Remarks
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Barker, A.T.; Jalinous, R.; Freeston, I.L. Non-invasive magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex. Lancet 1985, 325, 1106–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barker, A.T.; Freeston, I.L.; Jalinous, R.; Jarratt, J.A. Magnetic stimulation of the human brain and peripheral nervous system: An introduction and the results of an initial clinical evaluation. Neurosurgery 1987, 20, 100–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hallett, M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the human brain. Nature 2000, 406, 147–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pascual-Leone, A.; Walsh, V.; Rothwell, J. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience–virtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2000, 10, 232–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walsh, V.; Cowey, A. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2000, 1, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sack, A.T. Transcranial magnetic stimulation, causal structure–function mapping and networks of functional relevance. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2006, 16, 593–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, Y.Z.; Edwards, M.J.; Rounis, E.; Bhatia, K.P.; Rothwell, J.C. Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron 2005, 45, 201–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nitsche, M.A.; Paulus, W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 2000, 527, 633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nitsche, M.A.; Paulus, W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 2001, 57, 1899–1901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Priori, A.; Berardelli, A.; Rona, S.; Accornero, N.; Manfredi, M. Polarization of the human motor cortex through the scalp. Neuroreport 1998, 9, 2257–2260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruffini, G.; Wendling, F.; Sanchez-Todo, R.; Santarnecchi, E. Targeting brain networks with multichannel transcranial current stimulation (tCS). Curr. Opin. Biomed. Eng. 2018, 8, 70–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bindman, L.J.; Lippold, O.C.J.; Redfearn, J.W.T. Long-lasting changes in the level of the electrical activity of the cerebral cortex produced by polarizing currents. Nature 1962, 196, 584–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bindman, L.J.; Lippold, O.C.J.; Redfearn, J.W.T. Relation between the size and form of potentials evoked by sensory stimulation and the background electrical activity in the cerebral cortex of the rat. J. Physiol. 1964, 171, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bindman, L.J.; Lippold, O.C.; Milne, A.R. Prolonged changes in excitability of pyramidal tract neurones in the cat: A post-synaptic mechanism. J. Physiol. 1979, 286, 457–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Creutzfeldt, O.D.; Fromm, G.H.; Kapp, H. Influence of transcortical dc currents on cortical neuronal activity. Exp. Neurol. 1962, 5, 436–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacobson, L.; Koslowsky, M.; Lavidor, M. tDCS polarity effects in motor and cognitive domains: A meta-analytical review. Exp. Brain Res. 2012, 216, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nitsche, M.A.; Paulus, W. Transcranial direct current stimulation–update 2011. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2011, 29, 463–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sandrini, M.; Umiltà, C.; Rusconi, E. The use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: A new synthesis of methodological issues. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 35, 516–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radman, T.; Su, Y.; An, J.H.; Parra, L.C.; Bikson, M. Spike timing amplifies the effect of electric fields on neurons: Implications for endogenous field effects. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 3030–3036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landi, D.; Rossini, P.M. Cerebral restorative plasticity from normal ageing to brain diseases: A “never ending story”. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2010, 28, 349–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lang, N.; Nitsche, M.A.; Dileone, M.; Mazzone, P.; Ares, J.D.A.; Diaz-Jara, L.; Paulus, W.; Di Lazzaro, V.; Oliviero, A. Transcranial direct current stimulation effects on I-wave activity in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 2011, 105, 2802–2810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ridding, M.C.; Ziemann, U. Determinants of the induction of cortical plasticity by non-invasive brain stimulation in healthy subjects. J. Physiol. 2010, 588, 2291–2304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miniussi, C.; Ruzzoli, M.; Walsh, V. The mechanism of transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognition. Cortex 2010, 46, 128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Silvanto, J.; Muggleton, N.; Walsh, V. State-dependency in brain stimulation studies of perception and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 447–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miniussi, C.; Harris, J.A.; Ruzzoli, M. Modelling non-invasive brain stimulation in cognitive neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 1702–1712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Epstein, C.M.; Rothwell, J.C. Therapeutic uses of rTMS. In Plasticity in the Human Nervous System; Investigation with Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation; Boniface, S., Ziemann, U., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2003; pp. 246–263. [Google Scholar]
- Duecker, F.; Sack, A.T. Pre-stimulus sham TMS facilitates target detection. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e57765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duecker, F.; de Graaf, T.A.; Jacobs, C.; Sack, A.T. Time-and task-dependent non-neural effects of real and sham TMS. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e73813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flanagan, S.D.; Beethe, A.Z.; Eagle, S.R.; Proessl, F.; Connaboy, C.; Dunn-Lewis, C.; Kraemer, W.J. Blinding success of sham-controlled motor cortex intermittent theta burst stimulation based on participant perceptions. Brain Stimul. 2019, 12, 1058–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turi, Z.; Csifcsák, G.; Boayue, N.M.; Aslaksen, P.; Antal, A.; Paulus, W.; Groot, J.; Hawkins, G.E.; Forstmann, B.; Opitz, A.; et al. Blinding is compromised for transcranial direct current stimulation at 1 mA for 20 min in young healthy adults. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2019, 50, 3261–3268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Greinacher, R.; Buhôt, L.; Möller, L.; Learmonth, G. The time course of ineffective sham-blinding during low-intensity (1 mA) transcranial direct current stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2019, 50, 3380–3388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- O’Connell, N.E.; Cossar, J.; Marston, L.; Wand, B.M.; Bunce, D.; Moseley, L.; De Souza, L.H. Rethinking clinical trials of transcranial direct current stimulation: Participant and assessor blinding is inadequate at intensities of 2 mA. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e47514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brunoni, A.R.; Schestatsky, P.; Lotufo, P.A.; Benseñor, I.M.; Fregni, F. Comparison of blinding effectiveness between sham tDCS and placebo sertraline in a 6-week major depression randomized clinical trial. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2014, 125, 298–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bin Dawood, A.B.; Dickinson, A.; Aytemur, A.; Howarth, C.; Milne, E.; Jones, M. Investigating the effects of tDCS on visual orientation discrimination task performance: “The possible influence of placebo”. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2019, 4, 235–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hadi, F.; Soraya, S.; Bahramian, A. The enhanced placebo effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Prim. Care Companion CNS Disord. 2020, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Razza, L.B.; Moffa, A.H.; Moreno, M.L.; Carvalho, A.F.; Padberg, F.; Fregni, F.; Brunoni, A.R. A systematic review and meta-analysis on placebo response to repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for depression trials. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2018, 81, 105–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brunoni, A.R.; Lopes, M.; Kaptchuk, T.J.; Fregni, F. Placebo response of non-pharmacological and pharmacological trials in major depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e4824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dollfus, S.; Lecardeur, L.; Morello, R.; Etard, O. Placebo response in repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation trials of treatment of auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophr. Bull. 2016, 42, 301–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mansur, C.G.; Myczkowki, M.L.; Cabral, S.D.B.; Sartorelli, M.D.C.B.; Bellini, B.B.; Dias, A.; Bernik, M.A.; Marcolin, M.A. Placebo effect after prefrontal magnetic stimulation in the treatment of resistant obsessive-compulsive disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011, 14, 1389–1397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jiang, B.; He, D.; Guo, Z.; Mu, Q.; Zhang, L. Efficacy and placebo response of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for primary insomnia. Sleep Med. 2019, 63, 9–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bae, E.H.; Theodore, W.H.; Fregni, F.; Cantello, R.; Pascual-Leone, A.; Rotenberg, A. An estimate of placebo effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2011, 20, 355–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Okabe, S.; Ugawa, Y.; Kanazawa, I. 0.2-Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation has no add-on effects as compared to a realistic sham stimulation in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 2003, 18, 382–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garcin, B.; Mesrati, F.; Hubsch, C.; Mauras, T.; Iliescu, I.; Naccache, L.; Vidailhet, M.; Roze, E.; Degos, B. Impact of transcranial magnetic stimulation on functional movement disorders: Cortical modulation or a behavioral effect? Front. Neurol. 2017, 8, 338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- André-Obadia, N.; Magnin, M.; Garcia-Larrea, L. On the importance of placebo timing in rTMS studies for pain relief. Pain 2011, 152, 1233–1237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conforto, A.B.; Amaro, E.; Gonçalves, A.L.; Mercante, J.P.; Guendler, V.Z.; Ferreira, J.R.; Kirschner, C.C.; Peres, M.F. Randomized, proof-of-principle clinical trial of active transcranial magnetic stimulation in chronic migraine. Cephalalgia 2014, 34, 464–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Teepker, M.; Hötzel, J.; Timmesfeld, N.; Reis, J.; Mylius, V.; Haag, A.; Oertel, W.; Rosenow, F.; Schepelmann, K. Low-frequency rTMS of the vertex in the prophylactic treatment of migraine. Cephalalgia 2010, 30, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Granato, A.; Fantini, J.; Monti, F.; Furlanis, G.; Ilbeh, S.M.; Semenic, M.; Manganotti, P. Dramatic placebo effect of high frequency repetitive TMS in treatment of chronic migraine and medication overuse headache. J. Clin. Neurosci. 2019, 60, 96–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krummenacher, P.; Candia, V.; Folkers, G.; Schedlowski, M.; Schönbächler, G. Prefrontal cortex modulates placebo analgesia. Pain 2010, 148, 368–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zis, P.; Shafique, F.; Hadjivassiliou, M.; Blackburn, D.; Venneri, A.; Iliodromiti, S.; Mitsikostas, D.; Sarrigiannis, P.G. Safety, Tolerability, and Nocebo Phenomena During Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Placebo-Controlled Clinical Trials. Neuromodulation 2020, 23, 291–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rabipour, S.; Wu, A.D.; Davidson, P.S.; Iacoboni, M. Expectations may influence the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuropsychologia 2018, 119, 524–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rabipour, S.; Vidjen, P.S.; Remaud, A.; Davidson, P.S.; Tremblay, F. Examining the interactions between expectations and tDCS effects on motor and cognitive performance. Front. Neurosci. 2019, 12, 999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aslaksen, P.M.; Vasylenko, O.; Fagerlund, A.J. The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on experimentally induced heat pain. Exp. Brain Res. 2014, 232, 1865–1873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samartin-Veiga, N.; Pidal-Miranda, M.; González-Villar, A.J.; Bradley, C.; Garcia-Larrea, L.; O’Brien, A.T.; Carrillo-De-La-Peña, M.T. Transcranial direct current stimulation of three cortical targets is no more effective than placebo as treatment for fibromyalgia: A double-blind sham-controlled clinical trial. Pain 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, P.; Hooyman, A.; Schambra, H.M.; Lohse, K.R.; Schaefer, S.Y. Expectations from the general public about the efficacy of transcranial direct current stimulation for improving motor performance. Brain Stimul. 2021, 14, 500–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ray, M.K.; Sylvester, M.D.; Helton, A.; Pittman, B.R.; Wagstaff, L.E.; McRae, T.R.; Turan, B.; Fontaine, K.R.; Amthor, F.R.; Boggiano, M.M. The effect of expectation on transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to suppress food craving and eating in individuals with overweight and obesity. Appetite 2019, 136, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Elk, M.; Groenendijk, E.; Hoogeveen, S. Placebo Brain Stimulation Affects Subjective but Not Neurocognitive Measures of Error Processing. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2020, 4, 389–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Lange, F.P.; Heilbron, M.; Kok, P. How do expectations shape perception? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018, 22, 764–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Weinberg, R.; Gould, D.; Jackson, A. Expectations and performance: An empirical test of Bandura’s self-efficacy theory. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1979, 1, 320–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bollinger, J.; Rubens, M.T.; Zanto, T.P.; Gazzaley, A. Expectation-driven changes in cortical functional connectivity influence working memory and long-term memory performance. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 14399–14410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwarz, K.A.; Pfister, R.; Büchel, C. Rethinking explicit expectations: Connecting placebos, social cognition, and contextual perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016, 20, 469–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirsch, I. Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behavior. Am. Psychol. 1985, 40, 1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlino, E.; Piedimonte, A.; Benedetti, F. Nature of the placebo and nocebo effect in relation to functional neurologic disorders. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2016, 139, 597–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benedetti, F.; Pollo, A.; Lopiano, L.; Lanotte, M.; Vighetti, S.; Rainero, I. Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 4315–4323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fiorio, M.; Emadi Andani, M.; Marotta, A.; Classen, J.; Tinazzi, M. Placebo-induced changes in excitatory and inhibitory corticospinal circuits during motor performance. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 3993–4005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Weger, U.W.; Loughnan, S. Mobilizing unused resources: Using the placebo concept to enhance cognitive performance. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2013, 66, 23–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benedetti, F.; Lanotte, M.; Lopiano, L.; Colloca, L. When words are painful: Unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo effect. Neuroscience 2007, 147, 260–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colloca, L.; Miller, F.G. The nocebo effect and its relevance for clinical practice. Psychosom. Med. 2011, 73, 598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Colloca, L.; Tinazzi, M.; Recchia, S.; Le Pera, D.; Fiaschi, A.; Benedetti, F.; Valeriani, M. Learning potentiates neurophysiological and behavioral placebo analgesic responses. Pain 2008, 139, 306–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Benedetti, F.; Frisaldi, E.; Carlino, E.; Giudetti, L.; Pampallona, A.; Zibetti, M.; Lanotte, M.; Lopiano, L. Teaching neurons to respond to placebos. J. Physiol. 2016, 594, 5647–5660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Colloca, L.; Benedetti, F. How prior experience shapes placebo analgesia. Pain 2006, 124, 126–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colloca, L.; Petrovic, P.; Wager, T.D.; Ingvar, M.; Benedetti, F. How the number of learning trials affects placebo and nocebo responses. Pain 2010, 151, 430–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Colloca, L.; Benedetti, F. Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning. Pain 2009, 144, 28–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mazzoni, G.; Foan, L.; Hyland, M.E.; Kirsch, I. The effects of observation and gender on psychogenic symptoms. Health Psychol. 2010, 29, 181–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Clark, A. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci. 2013, 36, 181–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garofalo, S.; Timmermann, C.; Battaglia, S.; Maier, M.E.; Di Pellegrino, G. Mediofrontal negativity signals unexpected timing of salient outcomes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 29, 718–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alexander, W.H.; Brown, J.W. Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor. Nat. Neurosci. 2011, 14, 1338–1344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Battaglia, S.; Garofalo, S.; di Pellegrino, G.; Starita, F. Revaluing the role of vmPFC in the acquisition of Pavlovian threat conditioning in humans. J. Neurosci. 2020, 40, 8491–8500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holmes, J.; Nolte, T. “Surprise” and the Bayesian brain: Implications for psychotherapy theory and practice. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kok, P.; Brouwer, G.J.; Van Gerven, M.A.; de Lange, F. Prior expectations bias sensory representations in visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 16275–16284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sterzer, P.; Frith, C.; Petrovic, P. Believing is seeing: Expectations alter visual awareness. Curr. Biol. 2008, 18, R697–R698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stein, T.; Peelen, M.V. Content-specific expectations enhance stimulus detectability by increasing perceptual sensitivity. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2015, 144, 1089–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pinto, Y.; Van Gaal, S.; De Lange, F.P.; Lamme, V.A.F.; Seth, A.K. Expectations accelerate entry of visual stimuli into awareness. J. Vis. 2015, 15, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wyart, V.; Nobre, A.C.; Summerfield, C. Dissociable prior influences of signal probability and relevance on visual contrast sensitivity. Prac. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 3593–3598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Loo, C.K.; Taylor, J.L.; Gandevia, S.C.; McDarmont, B.N.; Mitchell, P.B.; Sachdev, P.S. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in controlled treatment studies: Are some “sham” forms active? Biol. Psychiatry 2000, 47, 325–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lisanby, S.H.; Gutman, D.; Luber, B.; Schroeder, C.; Sackeim, H.A. Sham TMS: Intracerebral measurement of the induced electrical field and the induction of motor-evoked potentials. Biol. Psychiatry 2001, 49, 460–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mennemeier, M.S.; Triggs, W.J.; Chelette, K.C.; Woods, A.J.; Kimbrell, T.A.; Dornhoffer, J.L. Sham transcranial magnetic stimulation using electrical stimulation of the scalp. Brain Stimul. 2009, 2, 168–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Opitz, A.; Legon, W.; Mueller, J.; Barbour, A.; Paulus, W.; Tyler, W.J. Is sham cTBS real cTBS? The effect on EEG dynamics. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 8, 1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bolton, D.A.; Staines, W.R. Transient inhibition of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex disrupts attention-based modulation of tactile stimuli at early stages of somatosensory processing. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 1928–1937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Duecker, F.; Sack, A.T. Rethinking the role of sham TMS. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lefaucheur, J.-P.; Antal, A.; Ayache, S.S.; Benninger, D.; Brunelin, J.; Cogiamanian, F.; Cotelli, M.; De Ridder, D.; Ferrucci, R.; Langguth, B.; et al. Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017, 128, 56–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fonteneau, C.; Mondino, M.; Arns, M.; Baeken, C.; Bikson, M.; Brunoni, A.R.; Burke, M.J.; Neuvonen, T.; Padberg, F.; Pascual-Leone, A.; et al. Sham tDCS: A hidden source of variability? Reflections for further blinded, controlled trials. Brain Stimul. 2019, 12, 668–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loo, C.K.; Husain, M.M.; McDonald, W.M.; Aaronson, S.; O’Reardon, J.P.; Alonzo, A.; Weickert, C.S.; Martin, D.M.; McClintock, S.M.; Mohan, A.; et al. International randomized-controlled trial of transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in depression. Brain Stimul. 2018, 11, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nikolin, S.; Martin, D.; Loo, C.K.; Boonstra, T.W. Effects of TDCS dosage on working memory in healthy participants. Brain Stimul. 2018, 11, 518–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwarzkopf, D.S.; Silvanto, J.; Rees, G. Stochastic resonance effects reveal the neural mechanisms of transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 3143–3147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Horvath, J.C.; Vogrin, S.J.; Carter, O.; Cook, M.J.; Forte, J.D. Effects of a common transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) protocol on motor evoked potentials found to be highly variable within individuals over 9 testing sessions. Exp. Brain Res. 2016, 234, 2629–2642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dissanayaka, T.D.; Zoghi, M.; Farrell, M.; Egan, G.F.; Jaberzadeh, S. Sham transcranial electrical stimulation and its effects on corticospinal excitability: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Rev. Neurosci. 2018, 29, 223–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tavakoli, A.V.; Yun, K. Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) mechanisms and protocols. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rossi, S.; Hallett, M.; Rossini, P.M.; Pascual-Leone, A.; The Safety of TMS Consensus Group. Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2009, 120, 2008–2039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Terney, D.; Chaieb, L.; Moliadze, V.; Antal, A.; Paulus, W. Increasing human brain excitability by transcranial high-frequency random noise stimulation. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 14147–14155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ambrus, G.G.; Paulus, W.; Antal, A. Cutaneous perception thresholds of electrical stimulation methods: Comparison of tDCS and tRNS. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2010, 121, 1908–1914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaptchuk, T.J.; Goldman, P.; Stone, D.A.; Stason, W.B. Do medical devices have enhanced placebo effects? J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2000, 53, 786–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burke, M.J.; Kaptchuk, T.J.; Pascual-Leone, A. Challenges of differential placebo effects in contemporary medicine: The example of brain stimulation. Ann. Neurol. 2019, 85, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morral, A.; Urrutia, G.; Bonfill, X. Placebo effect and therapeutic context: A challenge in clinical research. Med. Clín. (Engl. Ed.) 2017, 149, 26–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Testa, M.; Rossettini, G. Enhance placebo, avoid nocebo: How contextual factors affect physiotherapy outcomes. Man. Ther. 2016, 24, 65–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miller, F.G.; Kaptchuk, T.J. The power of context: Reconceptualizing the placebo effect. J. R. Soc. Med. 2008, 101, 222–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barbiani, D.; Benedetti, F. Placebo Effect. In The Wiley Encyclopedia of Health Psychology; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2020; pp. 127–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brody, H. The placebo response. J. Fam. Pract. 2000, 49, 649. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Benedetti, F.; Carlino, E.; Pollo, A. How placebos change the patient’s brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011, 36, 339–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Malhi, G.S.; Bell, E. Is the response to rTMS largely the result of non-specific effects? Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2021, 55, 445–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, N.J.; Gold, E.; Pascual-Leone, A.; Bracewell, R.M. Challenges of proper placebo control for non-invasive brain stimulation in clinical and experimental applications. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2013, 38, 2973–2977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Zwart, P.L.; Jeronimus, B.F.; de Jonge, P. Empirical evidence for definitions of episode, remission, recovery, relapse and recurrence in depression: A systematic review. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 2019, 28, 544–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meister, R.; Jansen, A.; Härter, M.; Nestoriuc, Y.; Kriston, L. Placebo and nocebo reactions in randomized trials of pharmacological treatments for persistent depressive disorder. A meta-regression analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 2017, 215, 288–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rief, W.; Nestoriuc, Y.; Weiss, S.; Welzel, E.; Barsky, A.J.; Hofmann, S.G. Meta-analysis of the placebo response in antidepressant trials. J. Affect. Disord. 2009, 118, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013; pp. 99–105. ISBN 978-0-89042-555-8. [Google Scholar]
- Aouizerate, B.; Guehl, D.; Cuny, E.; Rougier, A.; Bioulac, B.; Tignol, J.; Burbaud, P. Pathophysiology of obsessive–compulsive disorder: A necessary link between phenomenology, neuropsychology, imagery and physiology. Prog. Neurobiol. 2004, 72, 195–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Murray, D.; Stoessl, A.J. Mechanisms and therapeutic implications of the placebo effect in neurological and psychiatric conditions. Pharmacol. Ther. 2013, 140, 306–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gruzelier, J.H. EEG-neurofeedback for optimizing performance. I: A review of cognitive and affective outcome in healthy participants. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 44, 124–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kober, S.E.; Witte, M.; Grinschgl, S.; Neuper, C.; Wood, G. Placebo hampers ability to self-regulate brain activity: A double-blind sham-controlled neurofeedback study. Neuroimage 2018, 181, 797–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meyer, B.; Yuen, K.S.; Ertl, M.; Polomac, N.; Mulert, C.; Büchel, C.; Kalisch, R. Neural mechanisms of placebo anxiolysis. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 7365–7373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Knotkova, H.; Nitsche, M.A.; Cruciani, R.A. Putative physiological mechanisms underlying tDCS analgesic effects. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fertonani, A.; Rosini, S.; Cotelli, M.; Rossini, P.M.; Miniussi, C. Naming facilitation induced by transcranial direct current stimulation. Behav. Brain Res. 2010, 208, 311–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Borgomaneri, S.; Battaglia, S.; Garofalo, S.; Tortora, F.; Avenanti, A.; di Pellegrino, G. State-dependent TMS over prefrontal cortex disrupts fear-memory reconsolidation and prevents the return of fear. Curr. Biol. 2020, 30, 3672–3679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Borgomaneri, S.; Battaglia, S.; Sciamanna, G.; Tortora, F.; Laricchiuta, D. Memories are not written in stone: Rewriting fear memories by means of non-invasive brain stimulation and optogenetic manipulations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021, 127, 334–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Borgomaneri, S.; Battaglia, S.; Avenanti, A.; di Pellegrino, G. Don’t Hurt Me No More: State-dependent Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for the treatment of specific phobia. J. Affect. Disord. 2021, 286, 78–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jubb, J.; Bensing, J.M. The sweetest pill to swallow: How patient neurobiology can be harnessed to maximise placebo effects. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 2709–2720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nana, F.W.; Hilou, A.; Millogo, J.F.; Nacoulma, O.G. Neuromodulation of conditioned placebo/nocebo in heat pain: Anodal vs. cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Pain 2015, 156, 1342–1347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geuter, S.; Eippert, F.; Attar, C.H.; Büchel, C. Cortical and subcortical responses to high and low effective placebo treatments. Neuroimage 2013, 67, 227–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kong, J.; Gollub, R.L.; Rosman, I.; Webb, J.M.; Vangel, M.G.; Kirsch, I.; Kaptchuk, T.J. Brain activity associated with expectancy-enhanced placebo analgesia as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 381–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lui, F.; Colloca, L.; Duzzi, D.; Anchisi, D.; Benedetti, F.; Porro, C.A. Neural bases of conditioned placebo analgesia. Pain 2010, 151, 816–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Peciña, M.; Stohler, C.S.; Zubieta, J.Z. Neurobiology of placebo effects: Expectation or learning? Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013, 9, 1013–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nana, F.W.; Hilou, A.; Millogo, J.F.; Nacoulma, O.G. Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science 2004, 303, 1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, A.; El-Deredy, W.; Iannetti, G.D.; Lloyd, D.; Tracey, I.; Vogt, B.A.; Nadeau, V.; Jones, A.K. Placebo conditioning and placebo analgesia modulate a common brain network during pain anticipation and perception. Pain 2009, 145, 24–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Villa-Sánchez, B.; Emadi Andani, M.; Fiorio, M. The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the motor placebo effect. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2018, 48, 3410–3425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schicktanz, N.; Fastenrath, M.; Milnik, A.; Spalek, K.; Auschra, B.; Nyffeler, T.; Papassotiropoulos, A.; de Quervain, D.; Schwegler, K. Continuous theta burst stimulation over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex decreases medium load working memory performance in healthy humans. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0120640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shen, J.; Zhang, G.; Yao, L.; Zhao, X. Real-time fMRI training-induced changes in regional connectivity mediating verbal woring memory behavioral performance. Neuroscience 2015, 289, 144–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kaller, C.P.; Heinze, K.; Frenkel, A.; Läppchen, C.H.; Unterrainer, J.M.; Weiller, C.; Lange, R.; Rahm, B. Differential impact of continuous theta-burst stimulation over left and right DLPFC on planning. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013, 34, 36–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pecchinenda, A.; Ferlazzo, F.; Lavidor, M. Modulation of selective attention by polarity-specific tDCS effects. Neuropsychologia 2015, 68, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miller, E.K.; Cohen, J.D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 24, 167–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Article | Aim | Sample | Stimulation | Method & Outcomes | Main Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Duecker & Sack 2013 [27] | Explore the effects of TMS clicking sound and skin sensations on visual target detection. | 18 healthy volunteers. | Type: sham TMS Active: none Sham: identical to a real TMS coil with a magnetic shield reducing the effective magnetic field by 80% Intensity: 30% maximum SO Site: C3 and C4 Coil: figure-of-eight sham coil. | Within-subject design. Single pulse sham TMS to the right or left hemisphere at 300, 250, 200, 150, or 100 ms prior to target onset. As control, 80 trials without TMS. Outcome: RT at simple detection task. | Shorter RT when sham TMS preceded the target by 150, 200, and 250 ms; sham TMS ipsilateral to the target improved RT. |
Duecker et al. 2013 [28] | Investigate the time-dependency and task-dependency of the effects of TMS clicking sound and sensations. | 14 healthy volunteers. | Type: single-pulse TMS Active: 50% maximum SO Sham: identical to a real TMS coil at 30% maximum SO Coil: figure-of-eight Site: Vertex. | Within-subject design. Active or sham TMS during the tasks at 1 out of 9 TMS time windows (from −400 to +400 ms in steps of 100 ms) time-locked to stimulus onset, interleaved with no TMS trials. Outcomes: RT at a detection task and an angle judgment task. | Pre-stimulus TMS pulse increased the readiness to respond resulting in decreased reaction times. Post-stimulus TMS impaired task performance. This effect was specific for the detection task. No significant difference was found between active and sham TMS. |
Flanagan et al. 2019 [29] | Determine whether iTBS could be distinguished from sham stimulation. | 20 healthy volunteers (only women). | Type: iTBS Active: 600 pulses at 60%RMT Sham: sham coil < 0.3 T at 100% SO Coil: figure-of-eight coil Site: M1. | Crossover design. Two consecutive visits. Outcome: subjective reports on the type and the effects of stimulation (after 1 h from stimulation). | Prediction at chance level (55%) after active iTBS. Correct prediction after sham (74%). More accuracy at the second visit. |
Turi et al. 2019 [30] | Assess the effectiveness of blinding in sham (fade-in, short-stimulation, fade-out) and active tDCS protocols. | 192 healthy volunteers. | Type: anodal tDCS Active: 1 mA, 20 min Sham: 1 mA, 15 s, fade-in/out: 30 s Montage: anode over F3, cathode over right supraorbital region. | Between-subjects design. Sustained attention to response task during the stimulation (40 min duration); tDCS applied in the first 20 min. Outcome: assessment of blinding and discomfort. | Subjects could accurately guess they were receiving active tDCS when they actually did. More discomfort after active than sham tDCS. |
Greinacher et al. 2019 [31] | Assess the time course of sham tDCS-blinding. | 32 healthy volunteers. | Type: anodal tDCS Active: 1 mA, 10 min Sham: 1 mA, 20 s, fade-in/out: 30 s Montage: anode over C3, cathode over the right forehead. | Within-subjects design. Reaction time task before and during the stimulation. In blocks 2–4, sham-blinding probe questions were inserted every 30 s to assess blinding. | Difference in perception of itchiness between active and sham tDCS; participants correctly guessed above chance (78.1%) the session involving sham tDCS. |
O’Connell et al. 2012 [32] | Investigate the effectiveness of sham tDCS blinding and the effects of previous exposure to sham or active stimulation. | 96 healthy volunteers. | Type: anodal tDCS Active: 2 mA, 20 min, fade-in/out: 5 s Sham: switched off after 30 s Montage: anode over M1, cathode over the contralateral supraorbital region. | Crossover design; two separate sessions (active tDCS or sham). Word memory task before the stimulation. Judgement about the received stimulation (rating their confidence). | First session: 72% receiving active and 56% receiving sham, correctly judged the stimulation. Second session: 89% receiving active and 88% receiving sham guessed correctly. Confidence higher when they judged they had received active tDCS in the first session. |
Brunoni et al. 2014 [33] | Compare blinding integrity and associated factors for tDCS vs. placebo-pill. | 102 patients with major depression. | Type: tDCS Active: 2 mA, 30 min Sham: 2 mA, 30 s; fade-in 30 s; fade-out: 15 s Montage: anode over F3, cathode over F4. | Parallel design. Patients randomized to verum/placebo sertraline and active/sham tDCS. 10 sessions for the first 2 weeks, and 2 follow-up sessions every 2 weeks, for a total of 6 weeks. Outcome: assessment of blinding. | Both sertraline and tDCS mode were guessed above chance at week 6. Adverse effects and clinical response associated with correctly guessing. |
Article | Aim | Sample | Stimulation | Method & Outcomes | Main Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bin Dawood et al. 2019 [34] | Investigate the effects of occipital tDCS applied between two runs of orientation discrimination task (ODT). | Experiment 1: 66 healthy volunteers; experiment 2: 41 healthy volunteers. | Type: anodal and cathodal tDCS Active: 2 mA, 10 min Sham: 2 mA, 30 s, fade-in/out: none Montage: anode over Oz, cathode over left cheek and vice-versa. | Between-subjects design. Experiment 1: either anodal, cathodal, or sham tDCS. ODT administered at baseline and at the end of the stimulation. Experiment 2: baseline ODT, either no-tDCS with 2 min delay or 10 min delay between runs or receiving 10 min sham tDCS between the runs. | Experiment 1: Improvement in the second run of ODT compared to the first one regardless of the tDCS type. Experiment 2: only sham tDCS improved performance. |
Hadi et al. 2020 [35] | Single case study of a patient with OCD and generalized anxiety disorder. | A man with anxiety symptoms and compulsive checking. | Type: 10 Hz rTMS Active: 2000 total pulses Intensity: 110% RMT Sham: none Site: lDLPFC. | rTMS during a 10-day period while taking no medication. Two additional sessions: one 6 months and one 8 months later. | Remission of symptoms after the first 10 days treatment, but also after the single session 6 months and 8 months later. Dramatic remission of symptoms after every rTMS session (even with single session rTMS) probably indicating a placebo effect. |
Razza et al. 2018 [36] | Assess the magnitude of the placebo (sham) response to rTMS in major depressive disorder. | n.a. | n.a. | Meta-analysis of 61 studies. | Large placebo response directly associated with depression improvement of the active group, and inversely associated with higher levels of treatment-resistant depression. |
Brunoni et al. 2009 [37] | Assess placebo responses in pharmacological (antidepressant drugs) and non-pharmacological (rTMS device) depressive disorder trials. | n.a. | n.a. | Meta-analysis of 41 studies: 29 in rTMS arm and 12 in the escitalopram arm. | Large placebo response for both escitalopram and rTMS studies. Sham response associated with refractoriness and with the use of rTMS as an add-on therapy. |
Dollfus et al. 2016 [38] | Evaluate the placebo effect magnitude in rTMS treatment of auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia, considering the type of sham used. | n.a. | n.a. | Meta-analysis of 21 randomized, double-masked, sham-controlled studies. | Significant effect size of sham in 13 parallel design studies, but not in the 8 crossover studies. Highest effect size observed with the use of the 45° position sham coil. |
Mansur et al. 2011 [39] | Evaluate the efficacy of rTMS in patients with treatment-resistant OCD. | 27 OCD patients. | Type: 10 Hz rTMS Active: 60,000 total pulses, 40 trains, 5 s per train Intensity: 110% RMT; ISI: 25 s Sham: deactivated TMS coil Coil: figure-of-eight coil Site: rDLPFC. | Between-subjects design (active rTMS: 13 patients; sham rTMS: 14 patients). Outcome: clinical improvement, depression, anxiety, and cognitive tests applied at baseline, after rTMS and at follow-up. | Patients were not able to discern group allocation during or after rTMS treatment. Questionnaires and cognitive tests were not affected by the group (active rTMS vs sham). |
Jiang et al. 2019 [40] | Examine the efficacy and placebo response of rTMS in primary insomnia. | n.a. | n.a. | Meta-analysis of 9 clinical trials evaluating the efficacy of rTMS. | Active rTMS significantly improved insomnia symptoms for 10 days, 20 days, and 30 days. The proportion of sham rTMS response to the active rTMS response was 73.5%. |
Bae et al. 2011 [41] | Investigate the sham rTMS placebo effect in epilepsy, comparing different coil positions. | n.a. | n.a. | Meta-analysis of 3 placebo-controlled rTMS trials in epilepsy. Three treatment conditions were studied: placebo-rTMS, target-rTMS (coil positioned over a cortical seizure focus), and nontarget rTMS (the coil not positioned over a cortical seizure focus). | Median seizure frequency was low and essentially unchanged by placebo rTMS, neither in post-treatment nor in follow-up. |
Okabe et al. 2003 [42] | Investigate the efficacy of 0.2 Hz rTMS on Parkinson’s disease (PD) in comparison with sham. | 85 patients with PD. | Type: 0.2 Hz rTMS Active: 110% AMT Sham: electrical stimulation on the head (0.2 msec, 2 times the sensory threshold) and coil over Cz to produce a sound. Coil: circular Sites: M1 (coil over Cz) and occipital cortex (coil over the inion). | Between-subjects design. Participants were randomly assigned to M1, occipital, and sham stimulation. Outcomes: subjective improvement, UPDRS scores, HRSD scores, measured at baseline, 4 weeks, and 8 weeks after treatment. | No difference in UPDRS and HRSD between groups; significant difference in subjective improvement between M1 and occipital stimulation (with better performance in the former). |
Garcin et al. 2017 [43] | Investigate whether the positive effect of TMS in FMDs is due to cortical neuromodulation or to a cognitive-behavioral effect. | 33 patients with FMDs. | Type: 0.25 Hz rTMS or 0.25 Hz RMS Active: 120–150% RMT Sham: none Site: lateral or medial motor cortex contralateral to symptoms for rTMS; cervical or lumbar spinal roots homolateral to symptoms. | Crossover design. Patients randomized to receive RMS on day 1 and rTMS on day 2 or vice versa. Outcomes: clinical assessment using a rating scale specific for FMDs. Follow-up at 3 days, 3 months, 6 months, and 1 year. | The median percentage of improvement was 29.2% after the first session and 18.2% after the second session. Similar improvement after RMS and TMS. On the third day, 60% of the patients were much or very much improved; at 1 year, 56% of patients were much or very much improved. No difference between the scores at the follow-ups. |
Andrè-Obadia et al. 2011 [44] | Compare the analgesic effect of sham rTMS, either preceding or following active rTMS, in chronic pain. | 45 patients with chronic neuropathic pain resistant to drugs. | Type: 20 Hz hf-rTMS Active: 20 trains of 80 pulses ISI: 84 s Intensity: 90% RMT Sham: sham coil Coil: figure-of-eight Site: M1. | Crossover design. 2 sessions of active and sham rTMS separated by 2 weeks. Outcomes: pain scores (5 days before the first session, after the first session, and continuously for 2 weeks). | Placebo analgesia differed significantly when the sham rTMS session followed a successful or an unsuccessful active rTMS. Placebo sessions induced significant analgesia when they followed a successful rTMS, whereas they tended to worsen pain when following an unsuccessful rTMS. |
Conforto et al. 2014 [45] | Investigate the feasibility, safety, and efficacy of active rTMS in patients with chronic migraine without severe depression. | 14 patients with chronic migraine (all women). | Type: 10 Hz rTMS Active: 1600 total pulses, 32 trains, 5 s per train Intensity: 110% RMT ISI: 30 s Sham: coil perpendicularly to the vertex Coil: figure-of-eight coil Site: DLPFC. | Parallel design. Active or sham rTMS in 23 sessions within 8 weeks. Outcome: feasibility, proportion of adverse events, number of headache days in the past four weeks (at baseline, after four and eight weeks). | No significant differences in compliance with the sessions of treatment between the sham and active groups; decrease in number of headache days in the sham group. No significant decrease in number of headache days and more perceived pain in the active group. |
Teepker et al. 2010 [46] | Evaluate the therapeutic effects of low frequency rTMS in migraine. | 27 patients with migraine. | Type: 1 Hz rTMS Active: 2 trains of 500 pulses, ISI: 1 min Intensity: able to produce a visually detectable muscle contraction in at least 5 out of 10 trials Sham: deactivated coil Coil: circular for active, figure-of-eight for sham Site: Vertex. | Parallel design. Active or sham rTMS in 5 consecutive days. Outcomes: reduction of migraine attacks, number, and hours of days with headache, pain intensity, and analgesic intake for migraine. | After active rTMS, a significant decrease in migraine attacks was observed, however, this effect was not significantly different from sham group. The same was true for days with migraine and total hours of migraine. |
Granato et al. 2019 [47] | Investigate the effects hf-rTMS combined with suggestion to avoid medication overuse in patients suffering with chronic migraine and medication overuse headache. | 14 patients with chronic migraine. | Type: 20 Hz hf-rTMS Active: 10 trains of 2 s duration ISI: 30 s Intensity: 100% RMT Sham: sham stimulator able to induce the same skin vibratory sensation Coil: circular for active, figure-of-eight for sham Site: DLPFC. | Parallel design. 14 patients assigned to active hf-TMS and 14 to sham. 5 consecutive days per week, for two weeks. Outcomes: headache duration and intensity, symptomatic drug intake, recorded at baseline and 1, 2, and 3 months after the first stimulation. | All outcomes decreased in the two groups without significant differences. |
Krummenacher et al. 2010 [48] | Investigate the interaction of rTMS and expectations on pain perception. | 40 healthy volunteers. | Type: 1 Hz hf-rTMS Active: 2 trains of 15 min each Intensity: 100% RMT Sham: sham coil Coil: figure-of-eight Site: F3 and F4. | Between-subjects design. Analgesia-expectation group (TMS as a painkiller) and control group (no effect of TMS on pain). Of these, half assigned to active TMS and half to sham TMS. Heat-pain paradigm, low-frequency rTMS or sham TMS before expectation-induced placebo analgesia. | Placebo significantly increased pain threshold and pain tolerance. rTMS treatment did not affect pain perception but the disruption of DLPFC activity with TMS completely blocked expectation-induced placebo analgesia. Analgesia-expectation group reported more effective pain reduction than the control group. Participants in the active-TMS group perceived less analgesic effect than those in the sham group. |
Zis et al. 2020 [49] | Investigate the impact of nocebo phenomena during TMS clinical trials. | n.a. | n.a. | Meta-analysis of 93 placebo controlled randomized trials (depression: 28.0%, psychotic disorders: 19.4%, stroke: 12.9%, Parkinson’s disease: 7.5%, pain: 6.5%). | The pooled estimates of patients experiencing at least one adverse effect after active TMS and sham TMS was 29.3% and 13.6%. The odds of experiencing an adverse effect were 2.6 times higher in the active TMS group compared to sham. In depression, the nocebo adverse effects rate was 12.2%, while in depression the pooled estimates were 44.7% and 4.5%. |
Rabipour et al. 2018 [50] | Investigate the potential influence of expectations on tDCS intervention outcomes. | 90 healthy volunteers. | Type: anodal tDCS Active: 2 mA, 20 min Sham: 2 mA, 30 s, fade-in/out: 30 s for the single-blind round, 40 s for the double-blind round. Montage: anode over F3, cathode over supraorbital region. | Between-subjects design. High expectation priming (tDCS improves performance) or low expectation priming (tDCS has not known effects). Outcomes: expectations scores at baseline, after expectation priming and after tDCS; neuropsychological assessment; n-back task. Online task: working memory task. | Greater improvement in participants who received high compared to low expectation priming. Lowest performance after active tDCS and low expectation priming. Greater post-intervention improvement in executive function when receiving high compared to low expectation priming. |
Rabipour et al. 2019 [51] | Investigate whether expectations could influence behavioral outcome of tDCS intervention. | 121 healthy volunteers. | Type: anodal tDCS Active: 2 mA, 20 min Sham: fade-in/out: 30 s Montage: experiment 1 anode over M1 of the preferred hand, cathode over supraorbital region; experiment 2 anode over M1 of the non-preferred hand, cathode over supraorbital region. | Between-subjects design. High expectation priming (tDCS improves performance) or low expectations priming (tDCS has not known effects), and either active anodal or sham tDCS. Online task: finger fitness task. Outcomes: experiment 1: grooved pegboard test; experiment 2 grooved pegboard test, finger tapping test, and choice reaction time. | No significant effect in grooved pegboard test, in finger tapping test and choice reaction time in experiment 1 and 2. Participants primed to have high expectations significantly increased their expectation ratings compared to baseline, those who received low expectations priming significantly decreased their ratings. |
Aslaksen et al. 2014 [52] | Investigate the effects of short-term tDCS on pain perception. | 75 healthy volunteers. | Type: anodal tDCS Active: 2 mA, 7 min Sham: 2 mA, 30 s, fade-in/out: 20 s Montage: anode over C4 and cathode over the contralateral supraorbital area. | Between-subjects design. Three groups: active tDCS, sham tDCS, or no tDCS. Before, during and after tDCS painful stimuli were delivered (43, 45, 47 °C, for 20 s). Outcome: pain intensity, subjective stress, and blood pressure at baseline, after tDCS and in the post-test. | Pain decreased with active tDCS compared to no tDCS, no difference between active and sham at 45°. More effect of active tDCS compared to sham and no tDCS at 47°. More pain in the no tDCS group. Less subjective stress and lower blood pressure in the active tDCS compared to no tDCS group. |
Samartin-Veiga et al. 2021 [53] | Establish the optimal area of tDCS stimulation in a sham-controlled trial in fibromyalgia. | 130 healthy volunteers with fibromyalgia. | Type: anodal tDCS Active: 2 mA, 20 min, fade-in/out: 15 s Sham: fade-in/out: 15 s Montage: M1, DLPFC, OIC depending on the group. | Between-subjects design. Four groups: anodal tDCS over M1, DLPFC, OIC or sham. Outcome: pain intensity and improvement in other symptoms in fibromyalgia. 6 months follow-up. | Significant improvements across time for clinical pain and for fatigue, cognitive and sleep disturbances, and experimental pain, irrespective of the group. A significantly larger improvement after active tDCS, but not sham, in anxiety and depression. |
Wang et al.2021 [54] | Assess expectations about tDCS as enhancer of motor performance and explore the role of prior experience and knowledge of tDCS, sex, and age. | 379 healthy volunteers. | n.a. | Participants completed an online questionnaire through the Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) platform. | Expectations about tDCS for improving motor performance were higher than neutral. Prior knowledge had larger influence on expectancy scores in females compared to males. Prior knowledge had large effect on expectancy scores among younger adults compared to older adults. |
Ray et al. 2019 [55] | Evaluate the effect of tDCS on food craving and eating. | 74 adults with body mass index ≥ 25. | Type: tDCS Active: 2 mA, 20 min Sham: 2 mA, 1 min at the beginning and at the end of the session, fade-in/out: none Montage: anode over F4 and cathode over F3. | Four groups: told fake/got fake, told fake/got real, told real/got fake, and told real/got real. Outcome: food craving task (how much they liked each food), eating task (kcal consumed) and hunger assessment. | Participants who were told they were receiving real tDCS craved less and ate significantly less kcals (37.4%) than participants who were told they were receiving fake tDCS. In both measures, no effect of real tDCS over sham was found. |
Van Elk et al. 2020 [56] | Investigate how expectations about enhanced or impaired performance using tDCS affect feelings of agency and error processing. | 57 healthy volunteers. | Type: tDCS Active: none Sham: 1 mA, fade-in: 1 min Montage: electrodes positioned over Afz and CPz. | Within-subjects design. Placebo condition: instructions about tDCS positive effect; nocebo condition: instructions about tDCS negative effect; neutral condition: no tDCS. Outcome: EEG and Eriksen flanker task. Subjective feeling of agency after errors, perceived efficacy, and suggestibility. | Better performance perception in the placebo compared to the nocebo condition. Highest feelings of agency over the performance in the control condition, and lowest in the impairment condition. During the induction phase, expecting impaired vs. enhanced performance increased frontal theta power. |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Braga, M.; Barbiani, D.; Emadi Andani, M.; Villa-Sánchez, B.; Tinazzi, M.; Fiorio, M. The Role of Expectation and Beliefs on the Effects of Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1526. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111526
Braga M, Barbiani D, Emadi Andani M, Villa-Sánchez B, Tinazzi M, Fiorio M. The Role of Expectation and Beliefs on the Effects of Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(11):1526. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111526
Chicago/Turabian StyleBraga, Miriam, Diletta Barbiani, Mehran Emadi Andani, Bernardo Villa-Sánchez, Michele Tinazzi, and Mirta Fiorio. 2021. "The Role of Expectation and Beliefs on the Effects of Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation" Brain Sciences 11, no. 11: 1526. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111526
APA StyleBraga, M., Barbiani, D., Emadi Andani, M., Villa-Sánchez, B., Tinazzi, M., & Fiorio, M. (2021). The Role of Expectation and Beliefs on the Effects of Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation. Brain Sciences, 11(11), 1526. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111526