Next Article in Journal
On the Application of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience in Educational Environments
Previous Article in Journal
Hand and Foot Selection in Mental Body Rotations Involves Motor-Cognitive Interactions
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

Differential Predictors of Response to Early Start Denver Model vs. Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Child & Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Program, Modena University Hospital, Department of Biomedical, Metabolic and Neural Sciences, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, 41125 Modena, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(11), 1499; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12111499
Submission received: 15 September 2022 / Revised: 23 October 2022 / Accepted: 27 October 2022 / Published: 4 November 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Developmental Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
The effectiveness of early intensive interventions for Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is now well-established, but there continues to be great interindividual variability in treatment response. The purpose of this systematic review is to identify putative predictors of response to two different approaches in behavioral treatment: Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions (EIBI) and the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM). Both are based upon the foundations of Applied Behavioral Analysis (ABA), but the former is more structured and therapist-driven, while the latter is more naturalistic and child-driven. Four databases (EmBase, PubMed, Scopus and WebOfScience) were systematically screened, and an additional search was conducted in the reference lists of relevant articles. Studies were selected if participants were children with ASD aged 12–48 months at intake, receiving either EIBI or ESDM treatment. For each putative predictor, p-values from different studies were combined using Fisher’s method. Thirteen studies reporting on EIBI and eleven on ESDM met the inclusion criteria. A higher IQ at intake represents the strongest predictor of positive response to EIBI, while a set of social cognitive skills, including intention to communicate, receptive and expressive language, and attention to faces, most consistently predict response to ESDM. Although more research will be necessary to reach definitive conclusions, these findings begin to shed some light on patient characteristics that are predictive of preferential response to EIBI and ESDM, and may provide clinically useful information to begin personalizing treatment.

1. Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a heterogeneous disorder characterized by persistent deficits in social communication and interaction, by repetitive behaviors, and restricted interests or activities, and abnormal sensory processing. Moreover, these children often display co-morbid intellectual disability and language impairment [1]. Genetics strongly contribute to ASD, as supported by concordance in monozygotic twins being consistently higher than that observed in dizygotic twins [2]. A specific genetic aetiology is identifiable in up to 40% of individuals, although the majority of cases display complex gene–gene interactions involving multiple common and rare variants [3,4,5]. For many patients, also gene–environment interactions involving a genetic predisposition and prenatal–early-postnatal environmental influences are also plausible [6]. In addition to diagnosis, genetic variants can also contribute to explain interindividual variability in clinical phenotype, developmental trajectories, and responsiveness to behavioral or pharmacological treatment [7,8]. Hence, heterogeneity at the pathogenetic level translates into great clinical and treatment-related interindividual differences.
To date, there is no medical or biological treatment for core ASD symptoms, and interventions for ASD mainly fall within the psychoeducational, psychosocial, or behavioral frameworks [9,10]. Furthermore, there is no standard treatment for ASD. Community mental health programs, based on local guidelines, typically include a combination of interventions based on various approaches, such as speech therapy, sensory integration therapy, occupational therapy, neuropsychomotor treatment, and so on. However, these approaches are of limited efficacy and are not always evidence-based, so more structured and comprehensive interventions, such as those derived from Applied Behavioral Analysis (ABA), should be preferred [9].
Over the last 40 years, treatments based on ABA have become increasingly popular, especially after the publication of Lovaas’ promising results in 1987 [11]. In this now famous study, Lovaas showed that half of 19 children undergoing a manualized early intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI) [12], mainly based on discrete trial teaching (DTT), achieved normal cognitive and educational functioning as compared to children receiving less intensive behavioral intervention or other types of treatment. Since then, studies on EIBI have proliferated, confirming the effectiveness of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention, although no study has replicated results as favorable as those initially reported by Lovaas and colleagues [13,14,15,16,17].
Although effective in teaching new skills, research has shown that highly structured interventions such as DTT may have limitations, such as children having difficulties in generalizing learned skills in different contexts [18]. These limitations have led to the design and implementation of new approaches to behavioral intervention, still based on ABA, but less structured and with more naturalistic features: the Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral Interventions [19]. One type of intervention belonging to this family is the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM) [19], a comprehensive early intervention designed for children aged 12–48 months, whose efficacy and effectiveness have now been proven by several studies, including randomized controlled trials (RCT) and meta-analysis [20,21,22,23,24].
Although research on early and comprehensive behavioral interventions, such as EIBI and ESDM, has shown that children can achieve optimal outcomes, studies have found great interindividual variability in rate and extent of clinical improvement, with some children showing larger gains and others only small progress (e.g., [25,26,27,28]). This variability is not surprising, given the great heterogeneity in etiopathogenetic underpinnings, developmental trajectories, symptom patterns and severity present in the “Autisms”, term used by many investigators to comprehensively refer to ASD as a heterogeneous collection of rare disorders sharing the clinical features defined as “diagnostic criteria” in DSM-5 [29]. Within this framework, researchers have attempted to identify factors that may be associated with more favorable spontaneous developmental trajectories in children at risk or initially diagnosed with ASD. The most frequently reported factors are baseline cognitive abilities and severity of autism symptoms. For example, Weismer and Kover [30] found that ASD symptom severity and cognitive abilities at 30 months were significant predictors of language development at 66 months, in 129 children mostly receiving behavioral interventions at the time of the final assessment. Instead, fine motor skills, and not nonverbal cognitive abilities, were positive predictors of expressive language development in two independent samples of 86 and 181 children, assessed at age 3 and again at age 19 or 10.5, respectively [31]. Other factors, such as imitation and joint attention, were reported to be predictive of a favorable developmental trajectory, but not consistently in all studies.
In addition to interindividual variability, differences in treatment methodology could produce a more or less favorable outcome in different subgroups of autistic children. This notion spurred interest in searching for predictors of a positive response to specific forms of early intensive intervention. Two studies by Schreibman and Colleagues [32,33] appear to be especially interesting, as they suggest that the child characteristics associated with treatment outcome may be related to the style of treatment delivered. In their first study [32], the Authors identified two distinct behavioral profiles at baseline for responders and non-responders to a NDBI intervention, the Pivotal Response Training (PRT). Initially, the Authors predicted that children responding to PRT would show more toy play, less social avoidance and more verbal self-stimulatory behaviors, and this prediction was indeed proven correct [32]. In their subsequent study [33], the Authors selected six children with an incomplete “responder” profile, as three children lacked high toy play and three other children lacked low social avoidance, in the presence of the other two predictors. These six children received PRT first, and then DTT. PRT produced no significant response in children lacking only toy play, while children lacking only social avoidance displayed intermediate improvements between those of “responders” and “non responders” in the original study [32], pointing toward a greater role for toy play in predicting response to PRT. Importantly, the “PRT responder” profile did not predict response to DTT [33]. These two studies, for the first time, pointed to the existence of different sets of predictors of response to different forms of behavioral treatment, lending support to the possible personalization of early intervention in newly diagnosed ASD children.
Several studies have investigated factors that may be specifically associated with positive outcomes to EIBI and ESDM, yielding variable results. Higher intellectual functioning, measured as IQ or DQ at intake, was among the strongest predictors of response to EIBI in many [11,34,35,36,37], but not all, studies [13,16,23]. Starting treatment as early as possible seems to positively affect intervention outcome, and experts recommend that children should be referred to behavioral interventions as soon as ASD is diagnosed [10,38]. However, only some studies found that a younger age at intake predicted better outcome [36,39,40,41]. For example, Lovaas [11] did not find younger age at treatment intake to be associated with the best outcomes. Pretreatment autism severity and language skills have also been reported to predict treatment outcome, again with mixed results (positive for EIBI [36,42,43]; negative for EIBI [39,44,45]; positive for ESDM [27,46]; negative for ESDM [22,23,47]). Finally, other skills were found to be associated with positive outcomes, such as imitation [28,48] and joint attention [49,50], but these were not confirmed in other studies [22,45,51].
Though EIBI and ESDM seem to be equally effective in improving children outcome [23], their curriculum and teaching methods are indeed different. Therefore, it is plausible that some children are more likely to respond to one treatment approach than the other, based on their underlying neurobiology and genetics, which may express a set of clinically observable pre-treatment characteristics. Furthermore, the variable age range of children recruited in prior studies may have contributed to their discordant results, because greater deficits in a given function at an older age may reflect a more impaired underlying neurobiology, as compared to younger children, whose developmental trajectory is still at an earlier stage. This study has two main aims: (1) to systematically review all the available literature on predictors of response to two different types of behavioral interventions, notably Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention and Early Start Denver Model, in young children diagnosed with ASD and whose treatment starts by 48 months of age; (2) to combine evidence from different studies to define first- and second-line predictors of outcome for each intervention method based on the available evidence. Focusing on studies recruiting only young children should partly reduce inconsistencies and provide more helpful indications in the clinic. In fact, knowing which factors are most associated with a better response to treatment in this early stage of development, and whether these factors are treatment-specific, could help clinicians to prescribe the most effective intervention for each single child, at the time in life in which neuronal plasticity is at its maximum.

2. Methods

2.1. Design and Data Sources

Studies included in this systematic review were identified through a search performed on the following databases: EmBase, PubMed, Scopus and WebOfScience (WOS) [date of search: 9 September 2022].
Our search string was as follows: (autism OR autism spectrum disorder OR asd) AND (predictor OR predicting outcome OR outcome) AND (early intervention OR early start denver model OR esdm OR early intensive behavioral intervention OR eibi).

2.2. Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Only quantitative, empirical studies published in peer-reviewed journals were included. Studies were selected if participants were very young children under the age of four years at patient intake (i.e., 12–48 months); meeting DSM-5 criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder [1], or DSM-III/DSM-IV criteria for Autistic Disorder and/or Pervasive Developmental Disorder—Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS) [52,53], or ICD-10 criteria for Autistic Disorder [54]; receiving either Early Start Denver Model or Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention applied by a certified therapist. Studies were excluded if they focused on neurodevelopmental disorders of known genetic etiology (e.g., Fragile-X Syndrome, Rett Syndrome, Tuberous Sclerosis Complex); did not report pre-treatment child characteristics as predictors of ESDM/EIBI outcome; or applied parent-mediated interventions, whereby one parent was the main therapist.

2.3. Assessment and Measures

Overall, our initial search yielded 1601 articles, including 475 in WOS, 212 in PubMed, 666 in Scopus, and 248 in Embase. Articles were screened for eligibility based on title, abstract and, when appropriate, full text. We focused on children characteristics as potential predictors of treatment outcome, including anagraphical data and developmental measures, such as chronological age, cognitive abilities, language skills, and autism symptoms severity, recorded at the start of treatment.

2.4. Study Selection Process

After removing duplicates from the different databases, 1121 articles were identified; 1107 studies were excluded, because they did not meet our inclusion criteria (par. 2.2), leaving fifteen articles for this systematic review. Eight additional studies were found by searching the reference lists of selected articles, reviews and systematic reviews on the topic. Hence, a total 23 articles were ultimately selected: twelve on EIBI, ten on ESDM and one on both EIBI and ESDM. The selection process is illustrated in Figure 1. Complete lists of all articles extracted from each database are provided in Supplementary Table S1, distinguished by database source (sheets 1–4), and specifying the cause for exclusion (sheet 5).

2.5. Meta-Analytical Strategy for Combining p-Values

To quantitatively systematize the literature data, p-values from different studies assessing the same putative predictor were combined using Fisher’s method [55]. We chose these statistics because the association between each putative predictor and treatment outcome was tested using different statistical methods across multiple studies (t-tests, ANOVAs, Pearson’s correlation, regression analysis). In brief, Fisher’s method combines p-values from k independent tests of the same null hypothesis (H0), into one chi-squared (χ2) statistics with 2k degrees of freedom, providing a single combined p-value [55], as follows:
X 2 k 2 ~ 2 i = 1 k log ( p i )
To perform this meta-analytic procedure, p-values were recorded or extrapolated from each study. When more than one association between a putative predictor and an outcome variable was reported in the same study, the smallest p-value was chosen. When not explicitly reported, p-values were calculated from the available test statistics using GraphPad QuickCalcs Website [https://www.graphpad.com/quickcalcs/pvalue1.cfm (accessed on 4 October 2022)]. Fisher’s method was performed in R version 4.1.2 [56], using the “fisher” function of the “poolr” package [57].

3. Results

Overall, twenty-three articles were deemed eligible for inclusion in the systematic review: twelve on EIBI, nine on ESDM, and one on both EIBI and ESDM.

3.1. Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions

Thirteen publications reporting child’s predictors of EIBI outcome were selected in this section. These studies include six case-control trials [11,13,26,48,58,59], five single-group pre–post-treatment studies [15,48,59,60,61], and two randomized controlled trials (RCTs) [16,23]. One publication [62] is a two-year follow-up study of the same sample previously reported by Remington et al. [26]; therefore, these two publications will be counted and presented as a single study. Two publications [63,64] identified through database search were excluded because they merged the EIBI group with the comparison group in their results. These studies regarded the same sample as Zachor and Ben-Itzchak [58], which was included instead because the Authors analyzed and reported the experimental and the control groups separately. One study [37] was excluded, because it reports on the data collected from sixteen different individual publications, seven of which are included in the present review. Twenty-four articles on predictors of EIBI outcome, including three follow-up studies (twelve identified through database search and ten through systematic reviews and/or other reference lists) were excluded and will not be discussed because the age range of children at treatment start was over 48 months; however, these studies are listed for consultation in Supplementary Tables S2–S4).

3.1.1. Sample Characteristics and Patient Selection Criteria

Sample characteristics are summarized in Table 1. Overall, the studies included in this section comprised 382 children with Autism Spectrum Disorder aged 12–48 months at intake, including 38 (10%) females (three studies [26,58,65] did not report the gender of their experimental sample). Eight studies had a comparison group, comprising 220 children with ASD aged 12–42 months, including 33 (15%) females (but two studies [26,58] did not report the gender of their comparison group), and 58 typically developing children aged 18–59 months (gender not reported) [61]. In many studies, it was not possible to establish the age at which treatment actually began with any certainty, only the age at which the child was first referred or diagnosed (e.g., [13,26]).
Selected studies recruited patients diagnosed with ASD according to DSM-IV criteria, except for three studies, which used DSM-III [11], DSM-5 [23] and ICD-10 [15] criteria instead. Standardized instruments used to confirm the clinical diagnosis include the Autism Diagnostic Instrument-Revised (ADI-R) [66], in seven studies [13,15,26,48,58,59,65], the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) [67], in five studies [26,58,59,60,65], and the ADOS-2 [68], in one study [23]. Smith and colleagues [16] report that diagnosis for their participants was made by licensed psychologists independently of the study.
Main patient exclusion criteria in these studies were severe medical conditions [13,15,16,23,26,58,65]; genetic syndromes [23,59,60]; neurological disorders [48,59]; significant hearing [23,60], vision, physical or motor impairment, or children not yet walking [23]. Three studies [13,23,48] excluded children with an IQ < 35. Smith and Colleagues [16] did not include children with an IQ below 35 or above 75, while Lovaas [11] excluded children whose mental age was ≤11 months at a chronological age (CA) of 30 months. Four studies reported excluding children based on their CA at referral. Lovaas [11] excluded children whose CA was over 40 months if they were mute, or over 46 months if they were echolalic. Remington and Colleagues [26] included only children between 30 and 42 months old; Sallows and Graupner [48] between 24 and 42 months; Smith et al. [16] between 18 months and 42 months of age. One study [61] did not report any patient inclusion/exclusion criteria.

3.1.2. Treatment

Treatment characteristics are summarized in Table 2. All studies provided individualized EIBI. Six studies [11,13,15,23,48] employed the EIBI developed and manualized by Lovaas and Colleagues [12] at UCLA; three studies [58,60,61] provided EIBI according to Leaf and McEachin [69] and Maurice and Colleagues [70]; two studies [26,65] report to have provided ABA-based intervention using discrete trial teaching and other behavioral techniques. Finally, one study [59] reported providing a center-based ABA program.
Children with ASD in the comparison group received Eclectic Intervention [58,65], ESDM [23], low-intensity EIBI [11], parent-delivered EIBI [16,48], or treatment as usual (TAU) [13,26], also defined “community therapy” in some studies. Lovaas [11] also had a second comparison group, consisting of children studied by a different research group [71]. In one case, [15] the comparison group was composed of parent-commissioned EIBI (i.e., staff was hired and managed by parents themselves, as opposed to university-based EIBI where treatment personnel were provided by the University). Ten out of the 23 children included in Remington et al. [26] intervention group also received parent-commissioned EIBI, but they were considered part of the EIBI group, together with children receiving clinic-delivered EIBI.
The duration of EIBI varied considerably between studies, ranging from one to four years or more. Mean treatment duration was 22 months. Intensity also varied substantially, from a minimum of 12 h/week up to 40 h/week or more. On average, children received 28 h/week. Identifying the intensity and duration of treatment received by children in the comparison groups was more difficult, as the Authors did not always clearly report this information (e.g., [16,26,65]). Nevertheless, children in the comparison group received at least 19 h/week of treatment for approximately 19 months.

3.1.3. Measures

Outcome measures included cognitive abilities, autism symptoms severity, adaptive behaviors, language and communication abilities, social skills, and, in some cases, school placement, motor skills, imitation and joint attention. The measures used to assess IQ include the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID) [72] in eight studies [11,13,15,16,26,48,59,65], the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale [73,74] in five studies [11,16,26,59,65], the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Revised (WPPSI-R) [75] or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised (WISC-R) [76] in four studies [11,13,15,48], and the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) [77] in three studies [23,58,60]. Lovaas [11] also used the Cattell Infant Intelligence Scale [78], the Gesell Infant Development Scale [79], and the Vineland Social Maturity Scale [80] to assess the mental age of some children in their sample. To assess visual-reception skills, five studies [11,13,15,16,48] used the Merrill–Palmer Scale of Mental Tests [81]. Autism symptoms severity was assessed with ADI-R in seven studies [13,15,26,48,58,60,63] and with ADOS in five studies [23,58,59,60,65]. Remington et al. [26] also administered the Autism Screening Questionnaire (ASQ, [82]). Five studies [13,15,16,26,48] assessed receptive and expressive language using the Reynell Developmental Language Scales [83], while Rogers and Colleagues [23] administered the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Developmental Inventories [84]. Eight studies [13,15,16,23,26,48,58,59,60,65] assessed adaptive behaviors with the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS) [85,86]. In some studies, direct-observation tools were used to assess children’s skills, including the Early Learning Measures (ELM) [87], the Early Social Communication Scale (ESCS) [88], and the Early Skills Assessment Tool (ESAT) [89], respectively [16,26,61]. Ben-Itzchak et al. [59] also used developmental-behavioral scales to measure imitation, receptive and expressive language, and restricted and stereotyped behaviors. Remington et al. [26] assessed child behaviors with the Positive Social subscale of the Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form [90] and the parent-report version of the Developmental Behavior Checklist [91]. Finally, the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [92] was applied in two studies to assess children’s behaviors [16,48].

3.1.4. Predictors of EIBI Treatment Outcome

Pre-treatment characteristics associated with response to EIBI are listed in Table 3. It is important to point out that the results of Hayward et al. [15] and of Sallows and Graupner [48] refer to their entire sample, i.e., to the intervention and control group combined. However, while in Hayward et al. [15], the control group received EIBI commissioned by parents and delivered by trained therapists, in Sallows and Graupner [48], the comparison group received EIBI delivered directly by parents.
Cognitive abilities. Cognitive abilities at baseline represent the most-studied predictor of EIBI outcome in young children. Seven studies reported the association between pre-treatment IQ and EIBI treatment outcome, whereas four studies failed to find a correlation (Table 3). Lovaas [11] found that children with the most favorable outcome (i.e., children who achieved normal educational and intellectual functioning) had a higher IQ and mental age at pre-intervention. In subsequent studies, children with pretreatment IQ or DQ ≥ 70 showed significantly greater improvements in receptive language [59], as well as Communication, Daily living and Socialization VABS sub-domains scores [60]; performed better in ADOS scores both pre- and post-intervention (however, this result was also found in the comparison group) [65] and had a better outcome in terms of improved IQ [26,48], possibly predicting the maintenance of improvement at two-year follow-up [62]. In contrast to these seven positive results, four studies [13,16,23,61] found no association between pre-treatment IQ and EIBI intervention outcome, although one of these studies reported non-significantly higher pre-treatment cognitive scores in children categorized as High/Medium Responders, as compared to Low Responders [61]. Counterintuitively, in two studies, children with IQ < 70 at baseline showed a significantly greater improvement in imitation skills [59] and in MSEL scores, especially Fine Motor and Receptive Language score [60], compared to children with higher pretreatment IQ. Importantly, Hayward and Colleagues [15] found that pre-treatment visuo-spatial IQ correlated not only with post-treatment visuo-spatial IQ, but also with the magnitude of improvement in global IQ, language abilities (both receptive and expressive) and adaptive behaviors at the end of EIBI.
Chronological age at intake. Four studies explored the predictive role of age at treatment onset [11,15,26,61], but only one found younger chronological age at intake to be associated with better EIBI outcome. Specifically, MacDonald and Colleagues [61] found that children under 29 months of age were more likely to be classified as high responders and improved more than their older peers in terms of joint attention, cognitive abilities and play skills. However, the lack of a comparison group receiving another type of treatment does not allow to conclude with any certainty whether this result is specific to EIBI. In the other three studies, age at intake did not predict treatment outcome [11,15,26].
Severity of autism symptoms. Results are very mixed, as only one out of the three studies significantly supports a correlation between milder autism symptoms and better outcome and/or longer maintenance of improvement after EIBI (Table 3). In fact, Zachor and Ben-Itchak [58] report that children with milder severity symptoms (in both the EIBI and Eclectic group) showed greater gains in adaptive skills (i.e., VABS Daily Living, Communication and Socialization), cognitive and language abilities. Instead, Remington and Colleagues [26] found that children with the best outcome showed more severe, not milder autistic symptoms, as reported by their parents. However, in their 2-year follow-up study [62] the Authors found that children who maintained the positive effects of EIBI displayed a non-significant (p = 0.051) trend toward less severe symptoms of autism, as measured by the ADI-R, upon treatment start. Finally, Rogers and Colleagues [23] did not find any significant association between autism severity and EIBI outcome.
Language skills. In general, studies report that pretreatment language skills were primarily correlated with post-treatment language skills [15]. A broader improvement involving additional functions was described by two out of four studies, reporting a correlation between receptive language and EIBI outcome, with a third study displaying a non-significant trend in this direction (Table 3). Smith et al. [16] reported that language skills at entry were positively correlated with language skills and adaptive behaviors after two years of EIBI. Sallows and Graupner [48] found that receptive language predicted later IQ, social and language skills when considered together with other pre-treatment variables such as imitation, IQ, social interest and communication abilities. Cohen and Colleagues [13] reported that children with the most favorable outcome (i.e., children who scored on the average range on all outcome measures) showed a trend toward a slightly better receptive language at intake, although this finding was not significant. No association was found regarding expressive language [13]. Finally, Rogers and Colleagues [23] did not detect any significant association between pre-treatment language abilities and treatment outcome.
Communication skills. Two out of three studies found more developed communication skills associated with better outcome (Table 3). Remington et al. [26] found that children who benefited the most from treatment had higher communication skills at entry. Sallows and Graupner [48] found that communication abilities predicted later IQ, social and language skills, together with other pre-treatment variables (Table 3). However, Ben-Itzchak and Zachor [59] found no differences in the outcome of children who started treatment with higher vs. lower communication abilities, as measured by the ADOS. Unfortunately, these three studies each used a different tool to assess communication skills (Table 3), and this may have contributed to their discordant results.
Social skills. Three studies assessed social skills and found them associated with a better outcome after EIBI treatment (Table 3). Ben-Itzchak and Zachor [59] found that children with higher pre-treatment social skills showed greater improvement in receptive language and a trend toward slightly higher improvement in expressive language. Sallows and Graupner [48] found that social skills predicted post-treatment IQ, language and social skills, together with other variables, as outlined above. Finally, VABS Social Skills scores are part of a panel of variables predictive of EIBI response, measured as IQ change [26], whereas, in the same sample, ADI-R social skill scores predict persistent benefits two years after the end of treatment [62] (Table 3).
Adaptive behaviors. Four studies addressed adaptive behaviors, yielding mixed results (Table 3). Sallows and Graupner [48] found that VABS Daily Living Skills was one of several variables, such as imitation, receptive language and communication skills, that best predicted post-treatment IQ, language and social skills. In another study [26], children with the best outcome showed better pretreatment adaptive behaviors, as measured by the VABS, but also greater problem behaviors, as measured by the Developmental Behavior Checklist. Adaptive behaviors were not found to predict EIBI outcome in two other studies [13,15].
Imitation skills. Only one study, by Sallows and Graupner [48], investigated and confirmed that verbal and nonverbal imitation strongly predicted post-treatment IQ, social and language skills (Table 3).
Joint Attention. MacDonald and Colleagues [61] reported that High/Medium responders to EIBI had higher tendency to initiate joint attention, but this result did not reach statistical significance.

3.2. Early Start Denver Model

Eleven studies on ESDM reporting predictors of outcome were selected. Four articles focused on one-group pre–post-test studies [51,93,94,95] and one was an observational retrospective study [96]. Three studies were randomized controlled trials (RCTs) [22,23,97]. Three studies were case–control trials [98,99,100]. Latrèche and Colleagues [99] conducted both a cross-sectional and a longitudinal analysis, comparing children with ASD with typically developing children and children with ASD receiving ESDM vs. TAU, respectively. Eight additional studies were not included, since some children were older than 48 months at treatment start, but the sample characteristics, intervention strategies and outcome of these studies are summarized for consultation in Supplementary Tables S5–S7.

3.2.1. Sample Characteristics and Patient Selection Criteria

Overall, these eleven studies investigated predictors of ESDM outcome in 468 children, including 107 (22.8%) females, aged 12–48 months at intake based on our study-selection criteria. A summary of sample characteristics can be found in Table 4. Four studies [22,23,99,100] had a control group, consisting of 206 children with ASD, including 41 (19.9%) females, aged 12–48 months at intake. Latrèche and Colleagues [99] also included a second comparison group consisting of 16 typically developing children (females n = 4, 25.0%). One study [98] compared outcomes of younger children (18–48 months) with 28 older children with ASD aged 48–62 months at intake, both receiving ESDM.
Four studies included patients with ASD diagnosed according to DSM-5 criteria [23,95,96,97,98,99,100], whereas one study used DSM-IV criteria for Autistic Disorder or PDD-NOS [22]. All studies administered ADOS or ADOS-2 to confirm the diagnosis.
Main patient exclusion criteria in selected studies were severe medical conditions other than ASD [22,23,95,98,100], neurological disorders [51,95,96,100], genetic syndromes [51,95,96] and significant vision, hearing, motor, or physical impairment [22,23,51,96,98,100]. Two studies [22,23] excluded children with a Developmental Quotient (DQ) below 35 on MSEL; children born at a gestational age of less than 34 months; and children not yet walking. One study [97] reported no exclusion criteria based on child behaviors or cognitive abilities. Finally, three studies [93,94,99] did not specify exclusion/inclusion criteria.

3.2.2. Treatment

ESDM treatment characteristics are summarized in Table 5. All eleven studies implemented ESDM according to the Rogers and Dawson [19] manual. Seven studies [22,23,94,95,96,99,100] delivered individualized ESDM sessions, three [93,97,98] delivered group-setting ESDM, while Contaldo and Colleagues [51] provided two hours of individualized and two hours of group ESDM sessions per week. Children received, on average, 13 h/week of ESDM (range: 3–20) for an overall mean duration of 15 months (range: 10–24). Children in comparison groups received community therapy (CT) [22,99], also defined as “treatment as usual” (TAU) in some studies, or EIBI [23], for an average of 14 h/week (range: 3.4–20) for approximately 18 months (range 12–24). Vivanti et al. [98] provided ESDM to children in the older group with the same intensity (20 h/week) and for the same duration (12 months) as children in the younger group. Finally, children in the Wang and Colleagues [100] comparison group were on a waiting list to ESDM.

3.2.3. Measures

Outcome measures included mainly cognitive abilities, autism severity, adaptive behaviors, language and communication abilities, social skills, joint attention, and imitation. Cognitive abilities were assessed using the MSEL in all studies except three [51,96,100], which administered, respectively, the Bayley Scales of Infant Development-Third Edition (Bayley-III) [101] and the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Third Edition (WPPSI-III) [102], the Gesell Developmental Scale (GDS) [103] and the Griffith Mental Development Scales (GMDS) [104], which was also administered by Vivanti et al. [97]. One study [95] assessed children’s cognition, motor and adaptive skills through the Psychoeducational Profile-Third Edition (PEP—3) [105] ADOS-2 or ADOS were used by all studies to assess autism severity. One study [51] also employed the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) [106]. Adaptive behaviors were assessed with VABS-II in all studies except one [96], while Zitter and Colleagues [94] administered VABS-3 [107]. Contaldo et al. [51] administered the Italian version of the MB-CDI (“Il Primo Vocabolario del Bambino”) [108] to assess language skills, and the ESDM Curriculum Checklist [19], a direct-observation tool used to evaluate children’s skills in all developmental domains. One study [93] employed the Language Environment Analysis System (LENA), a wearable audio-recorder used to capture and quantify child vocalization and language-learning environment (LENA Research Foundation) [109]. Finally, Latrèche and Colleagues [99] used an eye-tracking paradigm to measure children’s attention to faces.

3.2.4. Predictors of ESDM Treatment Outcome

Pre-treatment characteristics associated with response to ESDM are listed in Table 6.
Cognitive abilities. Five studies found an association between cognitive abilities and several outcome measures [22,51,93,95,96], in contrast to two negative studies [23,94]. In particular, Contaldo and Colleagues [51] found that a higher developmental age at entry was associated with faster gains in “Socialization”, and “Cognition and Play” ESDM-checklist domains, as well as the rate of learning (operationalized as the number of objects acquired in one months by each child). Rogers and Colleagues [22] found that children with a higher DQ at baseline had lower autistic scores on the ADOS at the end of treatment. Two studies [93,95] found that DQ at baseline predicted DQ at the end of treatment. Surprisingly, one study [96] found that children with a DQ below 75 at baseline showed greater post-treatment improvement in cognitive and language scores compared to their peers, whose DQ was ≥75. Two studies [23,94] did not find any association between cognitive abilities and any outcome measure.
Chronological age at intake. Five studies addressed the possible association between age at treatment onset and final outcome. Pre-treatment chronological age was found to predict ESDM outcome in four of these five studies. Devescovi and Colleagues [96] found that entering ESDM before 27 months predicted greater improvements in autistic symptoms severity. Vivanti and Colleagues [98] compared younger children (18–48 months) with older children (48–62 months) receiving ESDM and found that younger children reached significantly larger gains on verbal DQ after one year of treatment, and that this result was moderated by initial language skills. Similarly, the same group also found that a younger age predicted verbal DQ, regardless of other factors [97]. Counterintuitively, Zitter et al. [94] found that age at intake was positively correlated with child-learning response; that is, older children responded more quickly to ESDM. Finally, no association between chronological age and any outcome measure was reported by Contaldo and Colleagues [51].
Severity of autism symptoms. Six studies investigated the possible association between ASD symptom severity and treatment outcome [22,23,51,94,95,100]. All but one [51] yielded negative results. In the only positive study, milder autism severity predicted greater gains in Socialization, Cognition, Play, and Motor ESDM-checklist domains, as well as in the rate of learning (i.e., number of learning objectives acquired by each child in one month) [51].
Language skills. Three out of four studies support receptive language and non-verbal communication as predictive of outcome after ESDM. Sulek and Colleagues [93] found that children vocalization ratio (i.e., a measure of speech-related sounds compared to non-speech sounds, such as vegetative sounds) was predictive of post-treatment DQ, together with pre-treatment DQ. Similarly, Godel and Colleagues [95] found that Expressive and Receptive Language predicted DQ and rate of DQ change at the end of treatment. Contaldo and Colleagues [51] found that receptive language, but not word production, was significantly associated with gains in Socialization, Cognition and Play, and Motor ESDM-checklist domains, as well as the rate of learning. Non-verbal communication, notably first communicative gestures repertoire and action with objects, were also associated with greater gains in the Communication ESDM-checklist domain [51]. Instead, Rogers and Colleagues [22] found that language abilities did not influence the effect of ESDM.
Attention to faces. One study [99] reports that higher levels of attention to faces, operationalized as the percentage of time spent staring at a face measured through an eye-tracking task, is predictive of children showing higher gains in overall DQ and verbal DQ after ESDM.
Stereotyped and repetitive behaviors. Three studies investigated whether this factor was associated with ESDM response. One of these [94] reported no significant results, while the other two found that lower repetitive behaviors at baseline predicted improvement in overall DQ [95] and cognitive verbal/preverbal [100] post-treatment.
Imitation, joint attention, play skills, and adaptive skills. Imitation [51], joint attention [22], and play skills [22] did not possess significant predictive power on ESDM outcome in single studies involving young children. Better adaptive skills were found to predict improvement in post-treatment DQ in only one study [95], while another was negative [94]. Similarly, studies enrolling older children reported mixed results (Supplementary Table S7).

4. Discussion

One of the ultimate aims of autism research is to allow for clinicians to define “which treatment for which child” beforehand, based on clinical predictors and objective biomarkers (genetics, brain imaging, electrophysiology, eye tracking, etc.). This aim not only regards pharmacological therapy [7,8], but also behavioral interventions which, although sharing some common elements, differ significantly in multiple aspects of their methodology. Structured behavioral approaches tend to favor a “teaching” relationship, and employ tasks which preferentially request and strengthen cognitive skills, making broader use of extrinsic motivators [9]; naturalistic approaches employ a “playground-like” relationship, employing activities that leave greater freedom of choice to the child and act as intrinsic motivators, while primarily requiring and strengthening social cognition (eye contact, theory of mind, joint attention, empathy, etc.). Predictably, not all children respond equally well to early intervention approaches, and yet it is at this time, early in life, that it would be most useful to provide targeted treatments, to maximally exploit neural plasticity. Research on pretreatment predictors of greater gains after behavioral interventions is still in its infancy. The available evidence trying to link preferential response to a specific type of treatment with a set of clinical/demographic characteristics is even more incomplete. Nonetheless, the evidence we have collected begins to point in some directions, which can possibly begin to orient clinicians and provide useful hints for future hypothesis-driven studies.
With this aim in mind, we began to identify first- and second-line predictors, as summarized in Table 7. First, we considered the number of studies addressing each pre-treatment variable in connection with post-treatment outcome. We then quantified the amount of available evidence in favor of each putative predictor, in terms of number of studies reporting a positive association between predictor and outcome, as well as cumulative p-value for each obtained predictor, combining all published statistical outcomes from multiple studies using Fisher’s method (Table 7 and Supplementary Table S8). First-line predictors are supported by more than 50% of the available studies for each treatment approach, with a cumulative p-values in the range of 10−10–10−11. Second-line predictors appear promising, as they also are supported by at least 50% of the available studies, but have been assessed in fewer articles and/or yield a cumulative p-value below the above-mentioned range. Other pre-treatment variables appear, at this stage, to be “Weak or non-predictors”, because they have been found to be associated with outcome in a minority of studies and/or with cumulative p-values < 10−5. Finally, special caution is required with variables assessed only in one or two studies, as insufficient evidence is currently available (Table 7).
Applying this stratification framework to studies regarding EIBI, the most studied and reliable factor associated with outcome in young autistic children is IQ/DQ at intake, since seven out of eleven studies support its predictive power, reaching an impressive combined p-value (Table 7). Interestingly, visuo-spatial IQ can especially be developed in EIBI responders [8]. Promising second-line predictors of better outcome after EIBI, requiring more studies to conclusively confirm and quantify their predictive power, include better receptive language abilities, communication skills, and social skills. Variables unlikely to be associated with EIBI outcome surprisingly include younger age at intake, and milder severity of autistic symptoms. Adaptive behaviors also yield very mixed results, which may reflect that this is a complex construct engaging multiple underlying skills. No conclusions can be drawn at this time about imitation and joint attention, which have each been the object of a single study to date.
For ESDM, the broader construct of pretreatment “social cognition” appears to predict a positive response in five out of six studies assessing communication (verbal and non-verbal) and attention to faces (Table 7). In particular, verbal (receptive and expressive language) and non-verbal (gestures) communication skills were collectively assessed in five studies, one negative [22] and four documenting greater improvements associated with better language skills at intake [51,98], higher expressive and receptive language DQ at the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL) [95], a broader repertoire of first communicative gestures [51], and greater intentional communication in the form of more speech-related vs. non-speech related vocalizations [93], albeit not necessarily full word production [51]. Meanwhile, another key feature in social cognition, i.e., attention to faces assessed by eye-tracking, was also predictive of better response to ESDM in another study [99]. Promising, but more mixed results, concerned age at intake, pretreatment IQ/DQ, and stereotyped/repetitive behaviors. Five studies investigated the predictive role of age at intake: three found that younger children made the biggest progress [96,97,98], one study found that older children achieved greater improvements [94], while one study was negative [51] (Table 6). Six studies investigated the predictive power of DQ at the beginning of treatment over response to ESDM, with four studies finding an association between greater post-treatment response and higher pre-treatment DQ [22,51,93,95], one study finding lower DQ predictive of greater post-treatment gains [96], and two studies reporting no association [23,94]. Interestingly, most studies tend to exclude a predictive role for the severity of autism symptoms prior to ESDM, which, despite being addressed by six studies, only reaches a cumulative p-value of 0.032 (Table 7). More research is needed, especially research focused on younger children, to draw firm conclusions on adaptive behaviors, imitation, joint attention, and play skills, each mostly not supported by single studies (Table 6), but with some positive results in research involving older children (Supplementary Table S7).
Predictors of positive response to EIBI and ESDM partially overlap, but also display some interesting differences. As expected, IQ/DQ was the most frequently reported variable associated with response to EIBI, but an association was also found in ESDM studies, although this was not as strong (Table 7). This difference is also present in studies involving older children, whereby IQ/DQ predicts better outcome to EIBI and ESDM in 11/12 (91.7%) and in 4/7 (57.1%) studies, respectively (Supplementary Tables S4 and S7). Conversely, response to ESDM was often associated with a set of variables falling within the realm of “social cognition”, including more speech-related sounds (i.e., greater intention to communicate), better receptive and expressive language, and greater attention to faces (Table 6 and Table 7). Some of these “social” variables also partly predict response to EIBI, but not quite as convincingly (Table 7). On the one hand, the overlap is not surprising, because both EIBI and ESDM propose tasks whose learning is influenced by child IQ/DQ, and both require interpersonal interactions between child and therapist. On the other hand, these results are collectively beginning to delineate an important difference: IQ/DQ and social cognition may represent preferential predictors of response to EIBI vs. ESDM, respectively, because these functions are the most required by each approach and may benefit the most from each approach. More specifically, children who have better cognitive functions, systemizing skills, and visuo-spatial IQ may benefit more from structured approaches that largely employ these functions, whereas children endowed with greater social motivation and with milder deficits in social cognition and communication may benefit more from approaches that use play, child’s initiative and fun interactions as a primary channel for stimulation. At the same time, EIBI and ESDM may catalyze global development and broader adaptive skills by primarily strengthening cognitive and social functions, respectively. Nonetheless, given the available evidence, this statement must be viewed more as a rationale hypothesis with some promising initial support than as a firm conclusion, which will require additional research.
There is some evidence that children may benefit more from starting interventions at an earlier age, especially for ESDM (Table 6). However, results are mixed and perhaps even disappointing for EIBI (Table 4 and Table 7). Some researchers [51,98] suggest that this might be due to the very narrow age-range of the children enrolled in most studies. However, even studies with a wider age-range have reported variable results, with only 10/19 (52.6%) and 1/6 (16.6%) studies finding that younger children achieve better results with EIBI and ESDM, respectively (see Supplementary Tables S4 and S7). Efficacy also in slightly older children suggests that the critical period of maximum plasticity, allowing for a satisfactory response to any type of early intensive intervention for some children could conceivably last longer than the narrow time-window adopted in this review (i.e., for some children, even starting treatment at 4–5 years of age may foster a positive response). At the same time, this lack of consistent benefits in children whose treatment was started at a very early age clearly shows that there are other variables that can override the effect of age on neuroplastic responses to environmental stimulation. One of these variables could conceivably be represented by rare [7] and common [110] genetic variants that negatively modulate dendritic spine formation and synaptic functions, including LTP and LTD. In parallel, similarly mixed results were obtained with autism severity in both EIBI and ESDM, investigated in three and six studies, respectively (Table 3 and Table 6). Milder symptoms at treatment onset were only associated with better outcome in 1/3 (33.3%) EIBI studies and in 1/6 (16.6%) ESDM study (Table 7). Studies recruiting older children yielded positive results in 4/9 (44.4%) for EIBI and 4/6 (66.7%) studies for ESDM (Supplementary Tables S4 and S7). This apparently greater predictive power for ESDM, if not a chance finding, may indicate that the severity of autistic symptoms is less relevant and predictive when treatment is started early, but may begin to matter more as treatment is started in children 4 years or older.
The methodological rigor of the studies included in this review was assessed through the Critical Review Form for Quantitative Studies [111], which highlighted several limitations, such as a small sample size yielding interesting trends, which often did not reach statistical significance [13,16], lack of a comparison group receiving another treatment, and lack of randomized assignment to intervention. Only two out of thirteen studies on EIBI and three out of nine studies on ESDM were fully RCTs. In some cases, the treatment protocol was not described in sufficient detail, including its intensity and duration. Studies did not always clearly report the time elapsed between the first assessment and the start of treatment, making it difficult to define the actual age of children at the beginning of the intervention. In some studies, the clinical and psychodiagnostic assessment was made a few months after the start of treatment [99]. Different instruments were used to measure the same variable, such as IQ and language skills, sometimes even within the same study, thus preventing a true comparison of the results. The use of objective measures such as gaze parameters obtained using eye-tracking technologies has been very limited, at least in young children [99]. Some important functions, such as imitation and joint attention, were investigated in very few studies. This is especially surprising, since it has been suggested that these skills may be predictors of positive response to ESDM, given its focus on social and communication skills [112]. This insufficient number of studies does not stem from a bias introduced by our inclusion criteria, because very few studies involving also older children have investigated these critical functions [28,45,112]. On the contrary, it would be advisable to shift away from broader constructs, such as IQ and language skills, and toward more proximal predictors of outcome, such as spontaneous imitation, vocalizations, and social interaction [47]. Furthermore, investigators often find what they are searching for: as an example, proportionally fewer ESDM studies investigated IQ/DQ as predictor of treatment outcome compared to EIBI studies (7/11 studies = 63.6% for ESDM, as compared to 11/13 studies = 84.6% of EIBI studies). Several studies have found that “overall pre-treatment functioning” or “initial learning rate” are associated with later outcome [113,114,115,116,117], and this may lead to circular reasoning: children who are more likely to learn because they are skillful will learn more and sooner from interventions [47]. However, many other factors and confounding variables may influence the initial rate of learning, for example, the degree of response to reinforcers used during treatment [116]. Moreover, early intensive interventions should act as catalyzers of functions which, at a given time, are observed to be underdeveloped in a child: placing this process into the framework of a mere “learning” paradigm may well be oversimplifying the complexities of motivation, emotion and relationship, which are at the core of autistic deficits. This point is often neglected in studies whose outcome measures are exclusively focused on cognitive functioning and DQ. Hopefully, future research will take these limitations into account, to reach broader and more definitive conclusions [24,47,118].

5. Limitations and Strengths

In many countries, the age at first diagnosis is beginning to significantly decrease, thanks to the more efficient and targeted health policies, increasing social engagement, growing political attention on autism, and larger resources being invested in ASD compared to the past. For this reason, we decided to focus the present review on young children, selecting studies whose sample was limited to the 12–48-month age range at the start of treatment. On the one hand, this stringent approach is a strength of our systematic review and metanalysis, because children do change with age and early infancy is when children should receive their first diagnosis and intensive intervention. Including studies also enrolling children who were older than 48 months at treatment onset can be expected to decrease the sensitivity and specificity of behavioral predictors of treatment response, because of heterogeneity in the timing of children’s developmental trajectory. Nonetheless, these studies are provided to interested readers in Supplementary Tables S2–S7. On the other hand, the exclusion of studies focused on parent-delivered interventions may be viewed as a limitation. We made this choice to avoid mixing parent- and therapist-delivered interventions, which are intrinsically different, and to prevent parental variables, such as parental stress, from introducing a bias in our assessment of treatment efficacy [119]. Furthermore, we only analyzed the intrinsic characteristics of the child as predictors of treatment outcome, under the assumption that treatments would be delivered by equally expert personnel using manualized approaches. Treatment-related factors, such as treatment intensity [37], influence response in real life and have not been considered here. Finally, the stratification strategy that we adopted has its own strengths and limitations. We attempted to “weigh” the evidence in favor of or against single predictors, by considering both the quantity of available studies and the effect size of the association between each predictor and treatment outcome. Given the limited number of articles selected in this review, the different possible strategies that are available to combine p-values, and the fact that our stratification criteria appear well-justified but also somewhat subjective, our conclusions should be viewed as preliminary rather than definitive.

6. Conclusions

A number of studies have now proven the effectiveness of comprehensive behavioral interventions such as EIBI and ESDM. Predictably, great individual variability has been observed in response to treatment, with some children showing considerably larger gains than others. The aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to begin identifying factors associated with positive response to early intervention and possibly treatment-specific predictors for EIBI and ESDM, which may help to maximize the clinical efficacy of intervention strategies. Our systematic review and combined p-values indicate that at a very young age, cognitive skills and developmental quotient appear to be most predictive of greater gains with EIBI. Instead, a set of variables pertaining to social cognition and communication appear to be most predictive of response to ESDM. More research focused on young children aged 12–48 mo and possibly devoid of the methodological limitations present in many studies published to date, will be necessary to draw more firm conclusions on these and on other promising variables that have been presented and discussed here (Table 7). Nonetheless, the results of this systematic review begin to shed some light on the factors associated with preferential response to EIBI and ESDM. Despite our many caveats and the need to always consider the broader clinical context of each patient, this information may provide clinicians with some useful clues when personalizing intervention strategies for young children newly diagnosed with ASD.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci12111499/s1, Table S1: Complete lists of all articles extracted from each database, distinguished by database source (sheets 1-4), and specifying the cause for exclusion (sheet 5); Table S2: Summary of EIBI studies excluded from our systematic review due to a patient recruitment age range beyond 48 months: sample characteristics; Table S3: Summary of EIBI studies excluded from our systematic review due to a patient recruitment age range beyond 48 months: interventions characteristics; Table S4: Summary of EIBI predictors of positive outcome in studies excluded due to a patient recruitment age range beyond 48 months; Table S5: Summary of ESDM studies excluded from our systematic review due to a patient recruitment age range beyond 48 months: sample characteristics; Table S6: Summary of ESDM studies excluded from our systematic review due to a patient recruitment age range beyond 48 months: intervention characteristics; Table S7: Summary of ESDM predictors of positive outcome in studies excluded due to a patient recruitment age range beyond 48 months; Table S8: Complete list of references, predictors, statistics used in each original article, single p-value either provided in each original article or derived from the published statistics (in red), combined using Fisher’s statistics to obtain the cumulative p-values presented in Table 7 of our published manuscript.

Funding

L.A. is funded by the Ph.D. Program in Neuroscience of the Department of Biomedical, Metabolic and Neural Sciences at Univ. of Modena and Reggio-Emilia (Italy).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Trial Registration

This review has been registered on PROSPERO with the ID: CRD42022321824.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (Ed.) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Huguet, G.; Benabou, M.; Bourgeron, T. The Genetics of Autism Spectrum Disorders. In A Time for Metabolism and Hormones; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2016; pp. 101–129. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bai, D.; Yip, B.H.K.; Windham, G.C.; Sourander, A.; Francis, R.; Yoffe, R.; Glasson, E.; Mahjani, B.; Suominen, A.; Leonard, H.; et al. Association of Genetic and Environmental Factors With Autism in a 5-Country Cohort. JAMA Psychiatry 2019, 76, 1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Schaefer, G.B.; Mendelsohn, N.J. Clinical Genetics Evaluation in Identifying the Etiology of Autism Spectrum Disorders: 2013 Guideline Revisions. Genet. Med. 2013, 15, 399–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Genovese, A.; Butler, M.G. Clinical Assessment, Genetics, and Treatment Approaches in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2020, 21, 4726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Fernandez, B.A.; Scherer, S.W. Syndromic Autism Spectrum Disorders: Moving from a Clinically Defined to a Molecularly Defined Approach. Clin. Res. 2017, 19, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Cucinotta, F.; Ricciardello, A.; Turriziani, L.; Calabrese, G.; Briguglio, M.; Boncoddo, M.; Bellomo, F.; Tomaiuolo, P.; Martines, S.; Bruschetta, M.; et al. FARP-1 Deletion Is Associated with Lack of Response to Autism Treatment by Early Start Denver Model in a Multiplex Family. Mol. Genet. Genom. Med. 2020, 8, e1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Vorstman, J.A.S.; Spooren, W.; Persico, A.M.; Collier, D.A.; Aigner, S.; Jagasia, R.; Glennon, J.C.; Buitelaar, J.K. Using Genetic Findings in Autism for the Development of New Pharmaceutical Compounds. Psychopharmacology 2014, 231, 1063–1078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Landa, R.J. Efficacy of Early Interventions for Infants and Young Children with, and at Risk for, Autism Spectrum Disorders. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2018, 30, 25–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lord, C.; Wagner, A.; Rogers, S.; Szatmari, P.; Aman, M.; Charman, T.; Dawson, G.; Durand, V.M.; Grossman, L.; Guthrie, D.; et al. Challenges in Evaluating Psychosocial Interventions for Autistic Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2005, 35, 695–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Lovaas, I. Behavioral Treatment and Normal Educational and Intellectual Functioning in Young Autistic Children. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1987, 55, 3–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Lovaas, O.I. Teaching Developmentally Disabled Children: The ME Book; Pro-Ed: Austin, TX, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cohen, H.; Amerine-Dickens, M.; Smith, T. Early Intensive Behavioral Treatment: Replication of the UCLA Model in a Community Setting. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2006, 27, S145–S155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Eikeseth, S.; Klintwall, L.; Jahr, E.; Karlsson, P. Outcome for Children with Autism Receiving Early and Intensive Behavioral Intervention in Mainstream Preschool and Kindergarten Settings. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2012, 6, 829–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Hayward, D.; Eikeseth, S.; Gale, C.; Morgan, S. Assessing Progress during Treatment for Young Children with Autism Receiving Intensive Behavioural Interventions. Autism 2009, 13, 613–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Smith, T.; Groen, A.D.; Wynn, J.W. Randomized Trial of Intensive Early Intervention for Children with Pervasive Developmental Disorder. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 2000, 105, 269–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Sheinkopf, S.J.; Siegel, B. Home-Based Behavioral Treatment of Young Children with Autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1998, 28, 15–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Schreibman, L.; Dawson, G.; Stahmer, A.C.; Landa, R.; Rogers, S.J.; McGee, G.G.; Kasari, C.; Ingersoll, B.; Kaiser, A.P.; Bruinsma, Y.; et al. Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral Interventions: Empirically Validated Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 45, 2411–2428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Rogers, S.J.; Dawson, G. Early Start Denver Model for Young Children with Autism; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  20. Dawson, G.; Rogers, S.; Munson, J.; Smith, M.; Winter, J.; Greenson, J.; Donaldson, A.; Varley, J. Randomized, Controlled Trial of an Intervention for Toddlers With Autism: The Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics 2010, 125, e17–e23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Fuller, E.A.; Kaiser, A.P. The Effects of Early Intervention on Social Communication Outcomes for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Meta-Analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020, 50, 1683–1700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Rogers, S.J.; Estes, A.; Lord, C.; Munson, J.; Rocha, M.; Winter, J.; Greenson, J.; Colombi, C.; Dawson, G.; Vismara, L.A.; et al. A Multisite Randomized Controlled Two-Phase Trial of the Early Start Denver Model Compared to Treatment as Usual. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2019, 58, 853–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Rogers, S.J.; Yoder, P.; Estes, A.; Warren, Z.; McEachin, J.; Munson, J.; Rocha, M.; Greenson, J.; Wallace, L.; Gardner, E.; et al. A Multisite Randomized Controlled Trial Comparing the Effects of Intervention Intensity and Intervention Style on Outcomes for Young Children With Autism. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2021, 60, 710–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Waddington, H.; van der Meer, L.; Sigafoos, J. Effectiveness of the Early Start Denver Model: A Systematic Review. Rev. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016, 3, 93–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Eldevik, S.; Eikeseth, S.; Jahr, E.; Smith, T. Effects of Low-Intensity Behavioral Treatment for Children with Autism and Mental Retardation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2006, 36, 211–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Remington, B.; Hastings, R.P.; Kovshoff, H.; degli Espinosa, F.; Jahr, E.; Brown, T.; Alsford, P.; Lemaic, M.; Ward, N. Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention: Outcomes for Children With Autism and Their Parents After Two Years. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 2007, 112, 418–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Sinai-Gavrilov, Y.; Gev, T.; Mor-Snir, I.; Vivanti, G.; Golan, O. Integrating the Early Start Denver Model into Israeli Community Autism Spectrum Disorder Preschools: Effectiveness and Treatment Response Predictors. Autism 2020, 24, 2081–2093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Vivanti, G.; Dissanayake, C.; Zierhut, C.; Rogers, S.J.; Victorian ASELCC Team. Brief Report: Predictors of Outcomes in the Early Start Denver Model Delivered in a Group Setting. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 1717–1724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Persico, A.M.; Cucinotta, F.; Ricciardello, A.; Turriziani, L. Autisms. In Neurodevelopmental Disorders; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2020; pp. 35–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ellis Weismer, S.; Kover, S.T. Preschool Language Variation, Growth, and Predictors in Children on the Autism Spectrum. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2015, 56, 1327–1337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Bal, V.H.; Fok, M.; Lord, C.; Smith, I.M.; Mirenda, P.; Szatmari, P.; Vaillancourt, T.; Volden, J.; Waddell, C.; Zwaigenbaum, L.; et al. Predictors of Longer-term Development of Expressive Language in Two Independent Longitudinal Cohorts of Language-delayed Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2020, 61, 826–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sherer, M.R.; Schreibman, L. Individual Behavioral Profiles and Predictors of Treatment Effectiveness for Children With Autism. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 73, 525–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Schreibman, L.; Stahmer, A.C.; Barlett, V.C.; Dufek, S. Brief Report: Toward Refinement of a Predictive Behavioral Profile for Treatment Outcome in Children with Autism. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2009, 3, 163–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Eikeseth, S.; Smith, T.; Jahr, E.; Eldevik, S. Intensive Behavioral Treatment at School for 4- to 7-Year-Old Children with Autism: A 1-Year Comparison Controlled Study. Behav. Modif. 2002, 26, 49–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Eikeseth, S.; Smith, T.; Jahr, E.; Eldevik, S. Outcome for Children with Autism Who Began Intensive Behavioral Treatment Between Ages 4 and 7: A Comparison Controlled Study. Behav. Modif. 2007, 31, 264–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Perry, A.; Cummings, A.; Geier, J.D.; Freeman, N.L.; Hughes, S.; Managhan, T.; Reitzel, J.-A.; Williams, J. Predictors of Outcome for Children Receiving Intensive Behavioral Intervention in a Large, Community-Based Program. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2011, 5, 592–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Klintwall, L.; Eldevik, S.; Eikeseth, S. Narrowing the Gap: Effects of Intervention on Developmental Trajectories in Autism. Autism 2015, 19, 53–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Zwaigenbaum, L.; Bauman, M.L.; Fein, D.; Pierce, K.; Buie, T.; Davis, P.A.; Newschaffer, C.; Robins, D.L.; Wetherby, A.; Choueiri, R.; et al. Early Screening of Autism Spectrum Disorder: Recommendations for Practice and Research. Pediatrics 2015, 136 (Suppl. 1), S41–S59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Harris, S.L.; Handleman, J.S. Age and IQ at Intake as Predictors of Placement for Young Children with Autism: A Four- to Six-Year Follow-Up. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2000, 30, 137–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Robain, F.; Franchini, M.; Kojovic, N.; Wood de Wilde, H.; Schaer, M. Predictors of Treatment Outcome in Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Observational Study in the Greater Geneva Area, Switzerland. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020, 50, 3815–3830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Smith, D.P.; Hayward, D.W.; Gale, C.M.; Eikeseth, S.; Klintwall, L. Treatment Gains from Early and Intensive Behavioral Intervention (EIBI) Are Maintained 10 Years Later. Behav. Modif. 2021, 45, 581–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Magiati, I.; Charman, T.; Howlin, P. A Two-Year Prospective Follow-up Study of Community-Based Early Intensive Behavioural Intervention and Specialist Nursery Provision for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2007, 48, 803–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Magiati, I.; Moss, J.; Charman, T.; Howlin, P. Patterns of Change in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Who Received Community Based Comprehensive Interventions in Their Pre-School Years: A Seven Year Follow-up Study. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2011, 5, 1016–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Flanagan, H.E.; Perry, A.; Freeman, N.L. Effectiveness of Large-Scale Community-Based Intensive Behavioral Intervention: A Waitlist Comparison Study Exploring Outcomes and Predictors. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2012, 6, 673–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lewon, A.B.; Ghezzi, P.M. An Evaluation of the Early Learning Measure as a Predictor of Outcomes in Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention. Behav. Interv. 2021, 36, 388–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Fulton, E.; Eapen, V.; Crnčec, R.; Walter, A.; Rogers, S. Reducing Maladaptive Behaviors in Preschool-Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Using the Early Start Denver Model. Front. Pediatr. 2014, 2, 40s. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Vivanti, G.; Prior, M.; Williams, K.; Dissanayake, C. Predictors of Outcomes in Autism Early Intervention: Why Don’t We Know More? Front. Pediatr. 2014, 2, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Sallows, G.O.; Graupner, T.D. Intensive Behavioral Treatment for Children With Autism: Four-Year Outcome and Predictors. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 2005, 110, 417–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Kasari, C.; Paparella, T.; Freeman, S.; Jahromi, L.B. Language Outcome in Autism: Randomized Comparison of Joint Attention and Play Interventions. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2008, 76, 125–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Kasari, C.; Gulsrud, A.; Freeman, S.; Paparella, T.; Hellemann, G. Longitudinal Follow-Up of Children With Autism Receiving Targeted Interventions on Joint Attention and Play. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2012, 51, 487–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Contaldo, A.; Colombi, C.; Pierotti, C.; Masoni, P.; Muratori, F. Outcomes and Moderators of Early Start Denver Model Intervention in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Delivered in a Mixed Individual and Group Setting. Autism 2020, 24, 718–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. American Psychiatric Association (Ed.) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder: DSM III, 3rd ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  53. American Psychiatric Association (Ed.) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV, 4th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  54. World Health Organization (WHO) (Ed.) The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders; World Health Organization: Genève, Switzerland, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  55. Fisher, R.A. Statistical Methods for Research Workers, 4th ed.; Oliver and Boyd: London, UK, 1932. [Google Scholar]
  56. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  57. Cinar, O.; Viechtbauer, W. The Poolr Package for Combining Independent and Dependent p Values. J. Stat. Softw. 2022, 101, 1–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Zachor, D.A.; Ben Itzchak, E. Treatment Approach, Autism Severity and Intervention Outcomes in Young Children. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2010, 4, 425–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Ben-Itzchak, E.; Zachor, D.A. The Effects of Intellectual Functioning and Autism Severity on Outcome of Early Behavioral Intervention for Children with Autism. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2007, 28, 287–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Ben-Itzchak, E.; Watson, L.R.; Zachor, D.A. Cognitive Ability Is Associated with Different Outcome Trajectories in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 2221–2229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. MacDonald, R.; Parry-Cruwys, D.; Dupere, S.; Ahearn, W. Assessing Progress and Outcome of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention for Toddlers with Autism. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2014, 35, 3632–3644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Kovshoff, H.; Hastings, R.P.; Remington, B. Two-Year Outcomes for Children With Autism After the Cessation of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention. Behav. Modif. 2011, 35, 427–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ben Itzchak, E.; Zachor, D.A. Change in Autism Classification with Early Intervention: Predictors and Outcomes. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2009, 3, 967–976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Ben Itzchak, E.; Zachor, D.A. Who Benefits from Early Intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorders? Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2011, 5, 345–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Zachor, D.A.; Ben-Itzchak, E.; Rabinovich, A.-L.; Lahat, E. Change in Autism Core Symptoms with Intervention. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2007, 1, 304–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Lord, C.; Rutter, M.; Le Couteur, A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A Revised Version of a Diagnostic Interview for Caregivers of Individuals with Possible Pervasive Developmental Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1994, 24, 659–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Lord, C.; Rutter, M.; DiLavore, P.C. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-WPS (ADOS-WPS); Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  68. Lord, C.; Luyster, R.J.; Gotham, K.; Guthrie, W. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) Manual (Part II): Toddler Module; Western Psychological Services: Torrance, CA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  69. Leaf, R.; McEachin, J. A Work in Progress: Behavior Management Strategies and a Curriculum for Intensive Behavioral Treatment of Autism; DRL: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  70. Maurice, C.; Green, G.; Luce, S.C. Behavioral Intervention for Young Children with Autism: A Manual for Parents and Professionals; Pro-Ed: Austin, TX, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  71. Freeman, B.J.; Ritvo, E.R.; Needleman, R.; Yokota, A. The Stability of Cognitive and Linguistic Parameters in Autism: A Five-Year Prospective Study. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 1985, 24, 459–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Bayley, N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 2nd ed.; Psychological Corp.: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  73. Thorndike, R.L. Manual for Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale; Houghton Mifflin.: Boston, MA, USA, 1972. [Google Scholar]
  74. Thorndike, R.L. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, 4th ed.; Riverside: Itasca, IL, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  75. Wechsler, D. WPPSI-R: Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised, Revised Edition; Psychological Corp.: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  76. Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised; Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1974. [Google Scholar]
  77. Mullen, E. Mullen Scales of Early Learning: AGS Edition; American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  78. Cattel, P. The Measurement of Intelligence of Infants and Young Children; Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1960. [Google Scholar]
  79. Gesell, A. Gesell Developmental Schedules; Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1949. [Google Scholar]
  80. Doll, E.A. The Measurement of Social Competence: A Manual for the Vineland Social Maturity Scale; Educational Test Bureau, Educational Publishers: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1953. [Google Scholar]
  81. Stutsman, R. Guide for Administering the Merrill-Palmer Scale of Mental Tests; Harcourt, Brace & World: New York, NY, USA, 1948. [Google Scholar]
  82. Berument, S.K.; Rutter, M.; Lord, C.; Pickles, A.; Bailey, A. Autism Screening Questionnaire: Diagnostic Validity. Br. J. Psychiatry 1999, 175, 444–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Edwards, S.; Fletcher, P.; Garman, M.; Hughes, A.; Letts, C.; Sinka, I. The Reynell Developmental Language Scales III: The University of Reading Edition, 3rd ed.; NFER Nelson: Windsor, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  84. Fenson, L.; Marchman, V.; Thal, D.; Dale, P.; Reznick, S.; Bates, E. MacArthur Bates Communicative Development Inventories MB-CDI: The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories, User’s Guide and Technical Manual, 2nd ed.; Brookes Publishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  85. Sparrow, S.S.; Bella, D.A.; Cicchetti, D.V. The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale; American Guidance Service: Circle River, MN, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  86. Sparrow, S.S.; Cicchetti, D.V.; Bella, D.A. Vineland-II Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd ed.; NCS Pearson: Eagan, MN, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  87. Smith, T.; Eikeseth, S.; Buch, G.; Lovaas, I. Early Learning Measure. Unpublished test. 1995. [Google Scholar]
  88. Mundy, P.; Hogan, A.; Doehring, P. A Preliminary Manual for the Abridged Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS); University of Miami: Coral Gables, FL, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  89. MacDonald, R.; Anderson, J.; Dube, W.V.; Geckeler, A.; Green, G.; Holcomb, W.; Mansfield, R.; Sanchez, J. Behavioral Assessment of Joint Attention: A Methodological Report. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2006, 27, 138–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Tasse, M. The Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form: Age and Gender Effects and Norms*1. Res. Dev. Disabil. 1996, 17, 59–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Einfeld, S.L.; Tonge, B.J. The Developmental Behavior Checklist: The Development and Validation of an Instrument to Assess Behavioral and Emotional Disturbance in Children and Adolescents with Mental Retardation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1995, 25, 81–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Achenbach, T.N. Child Behavior Checklist; University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry: Burlington, VT, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  93. Sulek, R.; Smith, J.; Bent, C.A.; Hudry, K.; Trembath, D.; Vivanti, G.; Dissanayake, C. The Utility of LENA as an Indicator of Developmental Outcomes for Young Children with Autism. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2022, 57, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Zitter, A.; Rinn, H.; Szapuova, Z.; Avila-Pons, V.M.; Coulter, K.L.; Stahmer, A.C.; Robins, D.L.; Vivanti, G. Does Treatment Fidelity of the Early Start Denver Model Impact Skill Acquisition in Young Children with Autism? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2021; Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Godel, M.; Robain, F.; Kojovic, N.; Franchini, M.; Wood de Wilde, H.; Schaer, M. Distinct Patterns of Cognitive Outcome in Young Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder Receiving the Early Start Denver Model. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 13, 835580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Devescovi, R.; Monasta, L.; Mancini, A.; Bin, M.; Vellante, V.; Carrozzi, M.; Colombi, C. Early Diagnosis and Early Start Denver Model Intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorders Delivered in an Italian Public Health System Service. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2016, 12, 1379–1384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. Vivanti, G.; Dissanayake, C.; Duncan, E.; Feary, J.; Capes, K.; Upson, S.; Bent, C.A.; Rogers, S.J.; Hudry, K.; Jones, C.; et al. Outcomes of Children Receiving Group-Early Start Denver Model in an Inclusive versus Autism-Specific Setting: A Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial. Autism 2019, 23, 1165–1175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Vivanti, G.; Dissanayake, C.; The Victorian ASELCC Team. Outcome for Children Receiving the Early Start Denver Model Before and After 48 Months. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016, 46, 2441–2449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Latrèche, K.; Kojovic, N.; Franchini, M.; Schaer, M. Attention to Face as a Predictor of Developmental Change and Treatment Outcome in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biomedicines 2021, 9, 942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Wang, S.-H.; Zhang, H.-T.; Zou, Y.-Y.; Cheng, S.-M.; Zou, X.-B.; Chen, K.-Y. Efficacy and Moderating Factors of the Early Start Denver Model in Chinese Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Longitudinal Study. World J. Pediatr. 2022; Onlined ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Bayley, N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development: Bayley-III, 3rd ed.; Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  102. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence Third Edition WPPSI III, 3rd ed.; Giunti O.S.: Florence, Italy, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  103. Gesell Institute of Child Development. Gesell Developmental Observation–Revised and Gesell Early Screener. Technical Report: Ages 3–6. 2012. Available online: https://nanopdf.com/download/gdo-r-gesell-institute_pdf (accessed on 26 October 2022).
  104. Griffiths, R. The Abilities of Young Children: A Comprehensive System of Mental Measurement for the First Eight Years of Life, Revised Edition; A.R.C.I.D. Test Agency Limited: Bukcs, UK, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  105. Schopler, E.; Lansing, M.D.; Reichler, R.J.; Marcus, L.M. Psychoeducational Profile: TEACCH Individualized Psychoeducational Assessment for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (PEP-3) 3; Pro-Ed: Austin, TX, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  106. Schopler, E.; Reichler, R.J.; Renner, B.R. The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS); Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  107. Sparrow, S.S.; Cicchetti, D.V.; Saulnier, C.A. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 3rd ed.; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  108. Caselli, M.C.; Pasqualetti, P.; Stefanini, S. Parole e Frasi Nel «Primo Vocabolario Del Bambino». Nuovi Dati Normativi Fra i 18 e 36 Mesi e Forma Breve Del Questionario [Words and Sentences in the First Vocabulary of the Child: New Normative Data from 18 to 36 Months and Short Form of the Questionnaire]; Franco Angeli: Milan, Italy, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  109. Lena Research Foundation. User Guide LENA Pro, 2015. Available online: https://docplayer.net/44645642-User-guide-lena-pro-lena-research-foundation.html (accessed on 8 June 2022).
  110. Lamb, Y.N.; McKay, N.S.; Thompson, C.S.; Hamm, J.P.; Waldie, K.E.; Kirk, I.J. Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor Val66Met Polymorphism, Human Memory, and Synaptic Neuroplasticity: BDNF, Human Memory, and Synaptic Plasticity. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2015, 6, 97–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Law, M.; Stewart, D.; Pollock, N.; Letts, L.; Bosch, J.; Westmorland, M. Critical Review Form–Quantitative Studies; McMaster University: Hamilton, ON, Canada, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  112. Laister, D.; Stammler, M.; Vivanti, G.; Holzinger, D. Social-Communicative Gestures at Baseline Predict Verbal and Nonverbal Gains for Children with Autism Receiving the Early Start Denver Model. Autism 2021, 25, 1640–1652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Smith, T.; Klorman, R.; Mruzek, D.W. Predicting Outcome of Community-Based Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention for Children with Autism. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2015, 43, 1271–1282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Goin-Kochel, R.P.; Myers, B.J.; Hendricks, D.R.; Carr, S.E.; Wiley, S.B. Early Responsiveness to Intensive Behavioural Intervention Predicts Outcomes Among Preschool Children with Autism. Int. J. Disabil. Dev. Educ. 2007, 54, 151–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Virues-Ortega, J.; Rodríguez, V.; Yu, C.T. Prediction of Treatment Outcomes and Longitudinal Analysis in Children with Autism Undergoing Intensive Behavioral Intervention. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2013, 13, 91–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  116. Weiss, M.J. Differential Rates of Skill Acquisition and Outcomes of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention for Autism. Behav. Interv. 1999, 14, 3–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Préfontaine, I.; Morizot, J.; Lanovaz, M.J.; Rivard, M. A Person-Centered Perspective on Differential Efficacy of Early Behavioral Intervention in Children with Autism: A Latent Profile Analysis. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2022, 97, 102017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Howlin, P.; Magiati, I.; Charman, T. Systematic Review of Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions for Children With Autism. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2009, 114, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Strauss, K.; Vicari, S.; Valeri, G.; D’Elia, L.; Arima, S.; Fava, L. Parent Inclusion in Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention: The Influence of Parental Stress, Parent Treatment Fidelity and Parent-Mediated Generalization of Behavior Targets on Child Outcomes. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 688–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Study flow chart. EIBI: Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention; ESDM: Early Start Denver Model.
Figure 1. Study flow chart. EIBI: Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention; ESDM: Early Start Denver Model.
Brainsci 12 01499 g001
Table 1. Summary of EIBI studies: sample characteristics.
Table 1. Summary of EIBI studies: sample characteristics.
StudyCasesControls
N (M:F)Age at Intake in Months (Mean)DiagnosisExclusion CriteriaControl InterventionN (M:F)Age at Intake in Months (Mean)Diagnosis
[59] Ben-Itzchak and Zachor, 200729 (25:4)20–32 (27)DSM-IV;
ADOS;
ADI-R
  • Genetic syndromes
  • Seizure disorder
------------
[60] Ben-Itzchak et al., 201446 (39:7)17–33 (25.5) DSM-IV; ADOS
  • Genetic syndromes
  • Hearing impairment
------------
[13] Cohen et al., 200621 (18:3)20–41 (30)ADI-R
  • IQ < 35
  • Severe medical conditions
TAU21 (17:4)20–41 (33)ASD
[15] Hayward et al., 200923 (19:4)24–42 (36)ICD-10;
ADI-R
  • Severe medical conditions
Parent-commissioned EIBI21 (15:6)24–42 (34)ASD
[11] Lovaas, 198719 (16:3)<46 (35)DSM-III
  • Age at intake > 40 mo. if non-verbal or >46 mo. if echolalic
  • MA ≤ 11 mo. at CA of 30 mo.
C1: low intensity EIBI; C2: noneC1: 19 (11:8);
C2: 21 (n.r.)
C1: <42 (41);
C2: <42 (n.r.)
C1: ASD;
C2:ASD
[61] MacDonald et al., 201483 (n.r.)17–48 (n.r.)DSM-IVn.r.None58 (n.r.)18–59TD
[26] Remington et al., 2007; [62] Kovshoff et al., 201123 (n.r.)30–42 (36)DSM-IV; ADI-R
  • Age at intake < 30 and >42 mo.
  • Severe medical conditions
TAU21 (n.r.)30–42 (38)ASD
[23] Rogers et al., 202145 (34:11)12–30 (23)DSM-5; ADOS-2
  • Severe medical/genetic conditions;
  • Significant vision, hearing, motor, or physical problems;
  • DQ < 35;
  • Children not yet walking
ESDM42 (32:10)12–30 (24)ASD
[48] Sallows and Graupner, 200513 (11:2)24–42 (35)DSM-IV; ADI-R
  • DQ < 35;
  • Age at intake < 24 and >42 mo.
  • Neurological disorders
P-EIBI10 (8:2)24–42 (37)ASD
[16] Smith et al., 200015 (12:3)18–42 (36)n.r.
  • Age at intake < 18 and >42 mo.
  • IQ < 35 and >75
  • Severe medical conditions
P-EIBI13 (11:2)18–42 (36)ASD
[58] Zachor and Ben-Itzchak, 201045 (n.r.)17–35 (25)DSM-IV;
ADOS;
ADI-R
  • Severe medical conditions
Eclectic33 (n.r.)15–33 (26)ASD
[65] Zachor et al., 200720 (19:1)22–34 (28)DSM-IV;
ADOS;
ADI-R
  • Severe medical conditions
Eclectic19 (18:1)23–33 (29)ASD
ADI-R: Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; ADOS: Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ASD: Autism Spectrum Disorder; CA: Chronological Age; DQ: Developmental Quotient; DSM: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; EIBI: Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention; ESDM: Early Start Denver Model; F: Females; ICD: International Classification of Diseases; IQ: Intellectual Quotient; M: Males; MA: Mental Age; n.r.: Not reported; P-EIBI: Parent-Delivered Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention; TAU: Treatment as Usual; TD: Typical Development.
Table 2. Summary of EIBI studies: intervention characteristics.
Table 2. Summary of EIBI studies: intervention characteristics.
StudyCountryStudy DesignIntervention TypeSettingIntensityDuration
[59] Ben-Itzchak and Zachor, 2007IsraelOne group pre-test–post-test ABAAutism-specific preschool programs35 h/week12 mo
[60] Ben-Itzchak et al., 2014IsraelOne group pre-test–post-test ABACentre-based20 h/week24 mo
[13] Cohen et al., 2006USACase–control trialUCLA EIBIHome-based35–40 h/week36 mo
[15] Hayward et al., 2009UKNon-concurrent multiple baseline designUCLA EIBIHome-based37 h/week12 mo
[11] Lovaas, 1987USACase–control trialUCLA EIBIHome/School≥40 h/week≥24 mo
[61] MacDonald et al., 2014USACase–control trialABAHome/School20–30 h/week12 mo
[26] Remington et al., 2007;
[62] Kovshoff et al., 2011
UKCase–control trial;
2-year follow-up
ABAHome-based18–34 h/week
(mean = 26)
12 mo
[23] Rogers et al., 2021USARCTUCLA EIBIHome/Childcare setting12 vs. 20 h/week12 mo
[48] Sallows and Graupner, 2005USACase–control trialUCLA EIBINot reported38 h/week (gradually decreasing when children entered school)48 mo
[16] Smith et al., 2000USARCTUCLA EIBIHome/Preschool24 h/week (gradually decreasing after the first year)24–36 mo (mean = 33)
[58] Zachor and Ben-Itzchak, 2010IsraelCase–control trialABAAutism-specific preschool programs20 h/week12 mo
[65] Zachor et al., 2007IsraelCase–control trialABAAutism-specific preschool programs35 h/week12 mo
ABA: Applied Behavior Analysis; EIBI: Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention; RCT: Randomized Controlled Trial; UCLA: University of California, Los Angeles.
Table 3. Predictors of better outcome after EIBI treatment.
Table 3. Predictors of better outcome after EIBI treatment.
StudyPredictors of Better OutcomeImproved Functions Correlated with PredictorsNon-Predictors
[65] Ben-Itzchak and Zachor, 2007
  • IQ—Higher
  • IQ—Lower
  • Better social skills
  • Receptive language and play skills
  • Imitation
  • Receptive language (n.s. trend also for expressive language)
  • Communication skills (ADOS)
[60] Ben-Itzchak et al., 2014
  • IQ—Higher
  • IQ—Lower
  • VABS Communication, Daily living skills and Socialization scores
  • MSEL scores, especially Fine Motor and Receptive Language
  • None reported
[13] Cohen et al., 2006
  • None reported
  • IQ
  • Language skills (n.s. trend for receptive language)
  • Adaptive behaviors
[15] Hayward et al., 2009
  • Higher visuo-spatial IQ
  • Total IQ
  • Expressive and receptive language
  • Adaptive behaviors
  • Chronological age
  • Adaptive behaviors
  • Receptive and expressive language
[11] Lovaas, 1987
  • Higher IQ/mental age
  • Intellectual and educational functioning
  • Chronological age
[61] MacDonald et al., 2014
  • Younger chronological age
  • Responding to joint attention
  • Initiating joint attention
  • Cognition
  • Play skills
  • Cognitive abilities
  • Joint Attention (n.s. trend)
[26] Remington et al., 2007
[62] Kovshoff et al., 2011
  • Higher IQ/ mental age;
  • VABS scores: higher for Adaptive behaviors, Communication, Social skills; lower for motor skills.
  • More behavioral problems
  • Greater severity of autism symptoms
  • ADI-R social skills
  • IQ
  • Persistent benefits from EIBI (follow-up two years after the end of treatment)
  • Chronological Age
  • Milder severity of autism symptoms (trend)
[23] Rogers et al., 2021
  • None reported
  • None reported
  • Autism symptom severity
  • MSEL DQ
[48] Sallows and Graupner, 2005
  • Imitation (verbal and non verbal)
  • Higher IQ
  • Better receptive language
  • ADI-R communication
  • ADI-R social skills
  • VABS daily living skills
  • IQ
  • Social skills
  • Language skills
  • None reported
[16] Smith et al., 2000
  • Language skills
  • Language skills
  • Adaptive behavior
  • IQ
[58] Zachor and Ben-Itzchak, 2010
  • Milder severity of autism symptoms
  • Adaptive skills (VABS Daily living, Communication and Socialization)
  • Cognitive level
  • Language abilities
  • None reported
[65] Zachor et al., 2007
  • Higher IQ
  • Lower ADOS scores
  • None reported
AD: Autistic Disorder; ADI-R: Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; ADOS: Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; IQ: Intellectual Quotient; MSEL: Mullen Scales of Early Learning; n.s.: non-significant; PDD-NOS: Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified; VABS: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale.
Table 4. Summary of ESDM studies: sample characteristics.
Table 4. Summary of ESDM studies: sample characteristics.
StudyCasesControls
N (M:F)Age at Intake in Months (Mean)DiagnosisExclusion CriteriaControl InterventionN (M:F)Age at Intake in Months (Mean)Diagnosis
[51] Contaldo et al., 201932 (26:6)18–39 (29)ADOS-2
  • Genetic syndromes
  • Neurological disorders
  • Significant vision, hearing, motor, or physical impairment.
------------
[96] Devescovi et al., 201621 (18:3)20–36 (27)DSM-5;
ADOS-2
  • Genetic syndromes
  • Neurological disorders
  • Significant vision, hearing, motor, or physical impairment.
------------
[95] Godel et al., 202255
(48:7)
15–42 (28.7)DSM-5
ADOS-2
  • Severe somatic, neurologic or genetic condition that could have affected the validity of behavioral measures (e.g., cerebral palsy, epilepsy, etc.)
------------
[99] Latrèche et al., 202151 (45:6)17–48 (34)ADOS-2n.r.C1 = CT;
C2 = None
C1: 30 (25:5)
C2: 16 (12:4)
C1: 17–48 (34);
C2: 17–48 (30)
C1: ASD
C2: None
[22] Rogers et al., 201955 (41:14)14–29 (21)DSM-IV;
ADOS-2
  • Severe medical/genetic conditions
  • DQ < 35
  • Gestational age < 35 wks
  • Children not yet walking
CT63 (51:12)14–29 (21)ASD
[23] Rogers et al., 202142 (32:10)12–30 (24)DSM-5;
ADOS-2
  • Severe medical/genetic conditions
  • DQ < 35
  • Significant vision, hearing, motor, or physical impairment
  • Children not yet walking
EIBI45 (34:11)12–30 (23)ASD
[93] Sulek et al., 202299 (70:29)14–47 (32)ADOS-2n.r.------------
[98] Vivanti et al., 201632 (26:6)18–48 (33)DSM-5;
ADOS
  • Severe medical/genetic conditions
  • Significant vision, hearing, motor, or physical impairment
ESDM28 (25:3)48–62 (49.5)ASD
[97] Vivanti et al., 201944 (27:17)15–32 (26)DSM-5;
ADOS-2
No exclusion criteria based on child behavior or cognition------------
[100] Wang et al., 202221
(17:4)
18–36 (21)DSM-5
ADOS
  • Neurodevelopmental disorders of known genetic etiology
  • Developmental disorder other than ASD
  • Cerebral palsy
  • Hearing disorder
None (Waitlist for ESDM)24 (18:6)18–36 (22)ASD
[94] Zitter et al., 202116 (11:5)20–39 (29)ADOS-2n.r.------------
ADOS: Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ASD: Autism Spectrum Disorder; CT: Community Therapy; DQ: Developmental Quotient; DSM: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; EIBI: Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention; ESDM: Early Start Denver Model; n.r.: not reported.
Table 5. Summary of ESDM studies: intervention characteristics.
Table 5. Summary of ESDM studies: intervention characteristics.
StudyCountryStudy DesignSettingIntensityDuration
[51] Contaldo et al., 2019ItalyOne group pretest-posttestCommunity-based (GS)4 h/week
(2 h GS and 2 h 1:1)
8–16 mo
(mean 12 mo)
[96] Devescovi et al., 2016ItalyRetrospective studyCommunity-based3 h/week11–19 mo
(mean 15 mo)
[95] Godel et al., 2022SwitzerlandOne group pretest-posttestCenter-based20 h/week24 mo
[99] Latrèche et al., 2021SwitzerlandCase–control trialn.r.20 h/week24 mo
[22] Rogers et al., 2019USARCTHome/Preschool/Daycare20 h/week24 mo
[23] Rogers et al., 2021USARCTHome/Daycare12 vs. 20 h/week12 mo
[93] Sulek et al., 2022AustraliaOne group pretest-posttestChildcare setting (GS)~15 h/week12 mo
[98] Vivanti et al., 2016AustraliaCase–control trialUniversity-based (GS)15–25 h/week12 mo
[97] Vivanti et al., 2019AustraliaRCTSchool-based (GS)15 h/week10 mo
[100] Wang et al., 2022ChinaCase–control trialHospital-based1 h/week6 mo
[94] Zitter et al., 2021USAOne group pretest-posttestClinic-based4 h/week12 mo
DQ: Developmental Quotient; GS: Group-setting ESDM; RCT: Randomized Controlled Trial. 3.2.4. Predictors of ESDM treatment outcome.
Table 6. Predictors of positive outcome after ESDM treatment.
Table 6. Predictors of positive outcome after ESDM treatment.
StudyPredictors of Better OutcomeImproved Functions Correlated with PredictorsNon-Predictors
[51] Contaldo et al., 2019
  • Receptive language
  • Higher DQ
  • Lower autism symptoms severity
  • First communicative gestures repertoire
  • Action with objects
  • Socialization, cognition, play and motor ESDM-checklist domains
  • Rate of learning
  • Communication ESDM-checklist domain
  • Age at intake
  • Imitation
  • Word production
[96] Devescovi et al., 2016
  • Younger age at intake
  • Lower DQ (<75) at entry
  • Greater improvement in severity of autism symptoms
  • Greater improvement in cognitive and language scores
  • None reported
[95] Godel et al., 2022
  • Higher MSEL Composite DQ at entry
  • Higher VABS-II Adaptive Behavior Composite and Communication score
  • Higher Expressive and Receptive Language MSEL DQ
  • Higher Visual Reception MSEL DQ
  • Higher Fine Motricity MSEL DQ
  • Lower stereotyped and repetitive behaviors (ADOS RRB)
  • Early developmental progress (i.e., rate of change) by 6 months of intervention
  • Rate of DQ change
  • Symptom severity (ADOS CSS)
[99] Latrèche et al., 2021
  • Attention to faces
  • MSEL DQ
  • Verbal DQ
  • None reported
[22] Rogers et al., 2019
  • Higher DQ at baseline
  • Lower ADOS scores
  • Joint Attention
  • Severity of autism symptoms
  • Play skills
  • Expressive and receptive language skills
[23] Rogers et al., 2021
  • None
  • Severity of autism symptoms
  • MSEL DQ
[93] Sulek et al., 2022
  • MSEL DQ
  • Speech-related vocalization ratio 1
  • MSEL DQ
  • None reported
[98] Vivanti et al., 2016
  • Younger age at intake
  • Initial language
  • Verbal DQ
  • None reported
[97] Vivanti et al., 2019
  • Younger age at intake
  • Verbal DQ
[100] Wang et al., 2022
  • Less stereotyped and repetitive behaviors (ADOS RRB)
  • Improvement in cognitive verbal/preverbal
  • Severity symptoms (ADOS Communication and ADOS Social)
  • Age at independent walking
[94] Zitter et al., 2021
  • Older age at intake
  • Learning response rate
  • Severity of autism symptoms
  • Stereotyped and repetitive behaviors
  • Adaptive behaviors
  • MSEL DQ
1 Speech-related sounds/non-speech sounds. DQ: Developmental Quotient; IQ: Intellectual Quotient; MSEL: Mullen Scales of Early Learning.
Table 7. Predictors of better response to EIBI and to ESDM, categorized based on number of published articles, percentage of positive studies, and combined p-value obtained using the Fisher’s method [55].
Table 7. Predictors of better response to EIBI and to ESDM, categorized based on number of published articles, percentage of positive studies, and combined p-value obtained using the Fisher’s method [55].
EIBIESDM
VariableN. (%) of Positive StudiesFisher’s Statistics *VariableN. (%) of Positive StudiesFisher’s Statistics *
First-line predictorsHigher IQ/DQ at intake7/11 (63.6%)χ2 = 83.968 (df = 20);
p = 8.24 × 10−10
Verbal and non-verbal intention to communicate, attention to faces 5/6 (83.3%)χ2 = 77.733 (df = 12);
p = 1.12 × 10−11
Second-line predictorsBetter receptive language abilities 2/4 (50%)χ2 = 38.399 (df = 8);
p = 6.35 × 10−10
Higher IQ or DQ at intake, action with objects 5/7 (71.4%)χ2 = 61.444 (df = 14);
p = 6.54 × 10−8
Greater social skills 3/3 (100%)χ2 = 23.799 (df = 6);
p = 5.69 × 10−4
Younger age at intake 3/5 (60%)χ2 = 25.633 (df = 8);
p = 0.0012
Communication skills2/3 (66.6%)χ2 = 17.710 (df = 6);
p = 0.007
Less stereotyped and repetitive behaviors 2/3 (66.7%)χ2 = 14.854 (df = 6);
p = 0.021
Weak or non-predictorsAdaptive behaviors 2/4 (50%)χ2 = 18.757 (df = 6);
p = 0.0046
Milder severity of autistic symptoms 1/6 (16%)χ2 = 22.565 (df = 12);
p = 0.032
Younger age at intake 1/4 (25%)χ2 = 24.048 (df = 4);
p = 7.81 × 10−5
Milder severity of autistic symptoms1/3 (33.3%)χ2 = 20.802 (df = 6);
p = 0.002
Insufficient evidenceImitation1/1---Adaptive behaviors1/2---
Joint Attention0/1---Imitation0/1---
Joint attention0/1---
Play skills0/1---
* d.f. = 2N; if d.f < 2N, no statistics could be retrieved from one or more original articles.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Asta, L.; Persico, A.M. Differential Predictors of Response to Early Start Denver Model vs. Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1499. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12111499

AMA Style

Asta L, Persico AM. Differential Predictors of Response to Early Start Denver Model vs. Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(11):1499. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12111499

Chicago/Turabian Style

Asta, Lisa, and Antonio M. Persico. 2022. "Differential Predictors of Response to Early Start Denver Model vs. Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" Brain Sciences 12, no. 11: 1499. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12111499

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop