Next Article in Journal
Functional Connectivity Signatures Underlying Simultaneous Language Translation in Interpreters and Non-Interpreters of Mandarin and English: An fNIRS Study
Previous Article in Journal
Differences in Cognitive Functioning in Two Birth Cohorts Born 20 Years Apart: Data from the Interdisciplinary Longitudinal Study of Ageing
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Parietal Gamma Band Oscillation Induced by Self-Hand Recognition

1
Graduate School of Comprehensive Rehabilitation, Osaka Prefecture University, Osaka 5838555, Japan
2
Rehabilitation Unit, Kyoto University Hospital, Kyoto University, Kyoto 6068507, Japan
3
Department of Comprehensive Rehabilitation, Osaka Prefecture University, Osaka 5838555, Japan
4
Department of Psychiatry, Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka University, Suita 5650871, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(2), 272; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020272
Submission received: 21 December 2021 / Revised: 7 February 2022 / Accepted: 13 February 2022 / Published: 16 February 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Behavioral Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
Physiological studies have shown that self-body images receive unique recognition processing in a wide range of brain areas, from the frontal lobe to the parietal-occipital cortex. Event-related potential (ERP) studies have shown that the self-referential effect on the image of a hand increases P300 components, but such studies do not evaluate brain oscillatory activity. In this study, we aimed to discover the self-specific brain electrophysiological activity in relation to hand images. ERPs on the fronto-parietal midline were elicited by a three-stimulus visual oddball task using hand images: the self-hand, another hand (most similar to the self-hand), and another hand (similar to the self-hand). We analyzed ERP waveform and brain oscillatory activity by simple averaging and time-frequency analysis. The simple averaging analysis found no significant differences between the responses for the three stimulus tasks in all time windows. However, time-frequency analysis showed that self-hand stimuli elicited high gamma ERS in 650–900 ms at the Cz electrode compared to other hand stimuli. Our results show that brain activity specific to the self-referential process to the self-hand image was reflected in the long latency gamma band activity in the mid-central region. This high gamma-band activity at the Cz electrode may be similar to the activity of the mirror neuron system, which is involved in hand motion.

1. Introduction

The mechanism of self-referential processing is one of the fundamental questions in psychology and neuroscience. The self-referential effects were defined by Rogers et al. [1]. Recently, the pathophysiology of self-referential processing has been studied in patients with autism [2,3], schizophrenia [4,5], and brain damage [6,7]. Body apraxia and body paraphernalia, which occur in people with brain damage, are disorders of body self-awareness [8,9], and means of understanding the mechanisms and developing rehabilitation methods are still under debate [10]. Additionally, it has been shown that self-awareness of the hand and visual factors may affect motor imagery and kinesthetic illusion; this is one of the rehabilitation methods to improve motor function of the upper limbs [11,12,13]. In recent years, there has been interest in body self-awareness mechanisms as self-referential effects, most of which have focused on face recognition processing. These physiological mechanisms were shown by applying electrophysiological methods, such as event-related potential (ERP) measured by electroencephalography (EEG) [14,15,16,17,18,19]. EEG measures brain electromagnetic activity with a high temporal resolution of milliseconds. This property is particularly important for investigating the dynamics of neural activity underlying cognitive processing [20]. However, few studies have provided information on the self-referencing effects of human limbs. In behavioral studies, Frassinetti et al. [21,22] identified a self-advantage effect that led to a faster and more accurate match-to-sample performance for self vs. other body images, such as hands and feet. Additionally, only two EEG studies using the task of discriminating one’s own hand from the hand of another showed the increased P300 and later component amplitude of the ERP in one’s own hand compared to another’s [23,24]. These EEG studies showed interesting temporal features of self-referential processing of brain activity toward hands and suggested that electrophysiological perspectives may be useful to examine the self-referential effects of hands.
There is general agreement that neural networks for the body’s self-awareness include a wide range of brain regions in the midline, the posterior parietal cortex, ventral temporal cortex, anterior insula, and the extrastriate body area, and previous studies applied the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) technique [25,26,27,28,29]. Although there have been studies on the referential effects of one’s own body parts using EEG with the region of interest as the median, there have been no studies using the brain oscillatory activities called event-related desynchronization (ERD) and synchronization (ERS). ERD/ERS are time-locked components to the event but not phase-locked and can reflect an induced oscillatory response, which cannot be extracted by a simple linear method, such as averaging [30,31]. The brain oscillatory activity associated with the self-referential activity is thought to be alpha-band power [32,33,34,35] and gamma-band power [36,37]. Hence, the time-frequency analysis seems to be appropriate to analyze task-related changes in oscillatory activity or induced response [38].
We hypothesized that the self-hand image elicits specific effects, such as increasing ERP amplitude or differences in induced ERD or ERS. Our main objective was the detection of brain activity specifically evoked by the recognition of one’s own hand. In this study, to minimize the increase in alpha-type error due to multiple comparisons, we recorded EEG from three sensor positions on the scalp at the midline (Fz, Cz, Pz) based on previous studies. In this paper, we examine the self-specific brain electrophysiological activity in response to hand images as self-referential stimuli. We analyzed ERP waveform and brain oscillatory activity elicited by visual images of one’s own or another’s hand using simple averaging and time-frequency analysis.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects

Ten university students (6 male, 4 female; 20–29 years old; average age: 21.3 ± 1.0 years old) gave their informed consent to participate in the research as volunteers. We confirmed by questionnaire that all of them were right-handed, had no medical history of neurological or psychiatric illness, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no visual disturbances, such as color blindness. All subjects had received more than 12 years of education and were free from any drugs or alcohol for at least 72 hours before the test. The explanation on research cooperation was given orally and in writing to all subjects, and they signed an informed consent form. This study was conducted with the approval of the Osaka Prefecture University Graduate School General Rehabilitation Studies Ethics Committee (approval number 2018-201, Osaka, Japan).

2.2. External Stimuli

As external stimuli, visual images of the subjects’ hands were individually captured using a digital camera before the experimental setting. An image of each subject’s hand was taken fixed in an intermediate angle position with a black background and around 550-lx illuminance, which was measured using a luminometer. We controlled for ethnic group, age, and gender in subjects. The similarity scores of the hands for each subjects were calculated using PC software (Robust Finder, Canon IT Solutions Co Ltd., Tokyo, Japan). The similarity score was calculated from the normalized correlation to the luminance values of the images. In the case of color images, the luminance values were converted to monochrome images for processing and extraction. Therefore, the difference in luminance values between the model and the target affected the correlation value. Luminance values were greatly affected by hand size and skin color. The image of a hand with the highest similarity score to subjects themselves was labeled as “other1”, and the second-highest as “other2”.

2.3. ERP Designs

In this study, we applied three oddball tasks consisting of three visual stimuli to elicit P300 in ERPs to make them reproducible and more reliably represent the differences between oneself and others (Figure 1). Each trial was presented randomly with a black screen as the interval for 400–500 ms. Subjects were instructed to fix their head, minimize eye blinking as much as possible, and push the button by the right hand as soon as possible when the left hand appeared. The total time was about 30 minutes.
All subjects completed the experiments in a practice phase and a test phase. The former phase was to familiarize the subjects with these tasks by having them complete 20 practice trials (15 trials of the right hand and 5 trials of the left hand). The test phase had three experiments (see Figure 1). First, the condition “self” consisted of hand images of oneself or others. The standard stimulus was another’s right hand presented 160 times, the target stimulus was another’s left hand presented 40 times, and the distractor stimulus was one’s own right hand presented 40 times, at random. The stimulation presentation time was 1000 ms, and the interval from the stimulation end to the next stimulation start was set to 400–500 ms. Second, the condition “other1” exchanged the distractor stimulus of the “self” hand to the “other1” hand. Third, the condition “other2” exchanged the distractor stimulus of the “other1” hand to the “other2” hand. Thus, in the three experiments, standard and target stimuli were always the same (another’s left hand and another’s right hand), but the distractor was different (self, other1, other2 right hand). The subjects were instructed to press the button for the target stimulus without giving any information about the visual stimuli of their own hands. The trial order of experiments was completely random. Each task required about 8 minutes.

2.4. ERP Recording

The EEG measurement was conducted in a shielded room, and we removed the power line noise by connecting any equipment that could generate alternating currents to the ground. The measured EEG was checked visually, and there was no power line noise. The subjects were in a comfortable position in the seat. The stimulus outputs were displayed using PC software (Stimulus Sequencer, Miyuki Giken, Tokyo, Japan), and the output images were displayed on a 17-inch PC monitor set 60 cm away from the subject’s eyes.
EEG was recorded at 3 sensor positions on the scalp (Fz, Cz, Pz) by using Ag-AgCl dish electrodes (7 mm). The references set the bilateral earlobe attachment sites. The impedance level at the electrodes was set at 10 kΩ or less at all sites. EEGs were recorded without a notch filter. The band-pass filter was from 0.5 to 120 Hz for EEG and EOG, and the sampling rate was 1 kHz.
Electrooculograms (EOGs) were recorded through bipolar leads from the left supra and inferior orbital margins to detect mixed artifacts accompanying blinking and eye movement. To record EEG data, we connected a biosignal recording device (Polymate AP1000, Miyuki Giken Inc., Tokyo, Japan) with a preamplifier (32 Ch electroencephalogram amplifier for Polymate, Miyuki Giken Inc.) with a personal computer (CF-F9 with OS 7, Panasonic, Osaka, Japan). Epochs with artifacts due to eye blink or muscle movements were detected and removed based on their typical signal characteristics and abnormal amplitude information. Only artifact-free epochs were retained for further analysis.

2.5. Signal Averaging

The EEG signals for the distractor stimulus from the stimulus presentation start (0 ms) to 1000 ms were averaged and analyzed. P300 components were depicted as the maximum positive potentials observed between 250 and 650 ms after stimulus presentation. Particularly, the amplitude values of typical P3b and early components (P3a) were calculated [39]. EEG analysis software (AP Monitor Version 5, NoruPro Light Systems, Inc. (Tokyo, Japan), Bio Signal Viewer System Version 4, NoruPro Light Systems, Inc. (Tokyo, Japan)) was used for averaging and analyzing ERP. ERP trials with EOG artifacts and bursts of electromyography (EMG) activity (mean EOG and EMG voltage exceeding ± 50 μV) were excluded from further analysis. The pre-stimulus baseline (−100 to 0 ms) was used to perform a baseline correction.

2.6. Time-Frequency Analysis

We used the Brain Electrical Source Analysis (BESA) Version 5.0 (BESA GmbH) software to visualize time-frequency representations of the EEG signals in individual subjects. Brain oscillatory activity changes during the perception of each task stimuli of standard, target, and distractor were transformed into the time-frequency domain by using complex demodulation (for detailed information on this methodology, see [40]). To compare each stimulus of each task data, the time and frequency windows for time-frequency analysis were between 0 and 120 Hz and between 0 ms and 1000 ms, respectively. The evoked averaged responses were subtracted from the time series of each trial before the main time-frequency transformation to minimize the contribution of phase-locked components to subsequent estimates of induced activity.

2.7. Statistical Analysis

All three electrode sites were selected for statistical analysis (Fz, Cz, Pz). First, we analyzed the 2-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to the mean amplitudes of ERP for distractor stimuli in the 250–350 ms, 350–500 ms, and 500–650 ms ranges based on research by Su et al. [24]. The factors were condition (self-hand, other1-hand, other2-hand) and electrode sites (Fz, Cz, Pz). The significance level was less than 5%. Using G power 3.1 software, we conducted post-hoc power analyses with an effect size of medium (0.25), an α of 0.05, and a non-sphericity correction ε of 0.7. The correlation among repeated measures for a 2-way ANOVA to the mean amplitudes in the 250–350 ms was 0.23, and the power of analysis was 0.54; for 350–500 ms, the correlation was 0.31, and the power of analysis was 0.59; and for 500–650 ms, the correlation was 0.34, and the power of analysis was 0.61. The power analysis values for these measures were found to be acceptable.
Second, we analyzed time-frequency data averaged for each subject. Statistical analyses were conducted using BESA Statistics 1.0 for permutation testing and cluster analysis. BESA Statistics uses parameter-free permutation testing on the basis of the Student’s t-test [41,42]. In this study, there were no predefined clusters, as BESA Statistics 1.0 automatically identifies clusters in time and frequency that are significantly different between 2 conditions. The null hypothesis of “the data under the experimental conditions comes from the same probability distribution” was rejected if at least one t-value was above the critical threshold for p < 0.05 determined by 1024 permutation. We compared all subjects’ brain oscillation activities by standard, target, and distractor stimuli within each condition (thus, the comparison was target vs. standard, distractor vs. target, and distractor vs. standard in each condition).

3. Results

3.1. Simple Averaging Analysis

In all subjects, a clear peak latency of P300 was observed for the target stimuli; however, for the distractor stimulus, it was not found in several subjects. ERP for distractor stimuli, calculated by simple averaging, resulted in large deflections peaking between approximately 250 and 350 ms. Therefore, a mean amplitude of 250–350 ms as a typical P3a component was calculated.
Figure 2 shows grand mean ERPs for each task distractor stimuli from all scalp EEG channels. In behavioral data, the detection error was lower than 1%, and all subjects pressed the button for target stimuli easily. In all subjects, peak latency of P300 for the target stimuli was clear, but it did not appear in several subjects for the distractor stimulus. Thus, two-way ANOVA was performed to obtain the mean amplitudes of 250–350, 350–500, and 500–650 ms. The results of two-way ANOVA on mean amplitudes of 250–350 ms showed that the interaction between condition and electrode sites was significant [r = 0.23, F (8, 81) = 4.9651, p < 0.01], the main effect of each condition was not significant [r = 0.15, F (8, 81) = 2.0367, p = 0.1371], and the main effect of electrode site was significant [r = 0.32, F (8, 81) = 10.0952, p < 0.01]. On the mean amplitudes of 350–500 ms, the interaction between condition and electrode sites was not significant [r = 0.07, F (8, 81) = 0.487, p = 0.7450], the main effect of each condition was not significant [r = 0.08, F (8, 81) = 0.5873, p = 0.5582], and the main effect of electrode site was significant [r = 0.19, F (8, 81) = 3.3212, p < 0.05]. Mean amplitudes of 500–650 ms showed that the interaction between condition and electrode sites was not significant [r = 0.08, F (8, 81) = 0.5852, p = 0.6743], the main effect of each condition was not significant [r = 0.08, F (8, 81) = 0.5753 p = 0.5648], and the main effect of electrode site was significant [r = 0.24, F (8, 81) = 5.0590, p < 0.01]. In summary, the main effects of electrode sites showed a significant difference in each time window; however, the condition showed no significant difference.

3.2. Time-Frequency Analysis

All electrodes in individual subjects showed a pattern of suppression of oscillatory activity in mu (8–15 Hz) beginning within 200 ms in most stimuli after the stimulus appeared. The result of the time-frequency analysis, the cluster-based permutation test, revealed a significant difference between the distractor stimulus of self-hand and the standard stimulus (p < 0.05). Especially, 60–80 Hz frequency (high gamma) band activity in the time range of 650–900 ms at the Cz electrode for distractor stimulus of self-hand was higher than the standard stimuli (Figure 3), whereas there were no significant differences between other1-hand and standard stimuli, and other2-hand and standard stimuli.

4. Discussion

In this study, we found that brain cortical oscillatory components for self-hand were significantly larger than for other’s hands in the 60–80 Hz frequency (high gamma) band activity at the Cz electrode in the time range of 650–900 ms. The mean ERP amplitudes obtained using the simple average method were not significantly different between the tasks. These results were consistent with our hypothesis and illustrate that the self-hand image induced a specific late component in the gamma band in the central region.
In our results of the simple averaging analysis, the positive component around 300 ms in Pz was higher than that of the other electrodes. Previous studies that measured ERPs to self-hand reported an increase in P300 at the parieto-occipital electrodes compared to other sites [23,24], and our results follow the previous studies. In contrast, there were no significant differences between all tasks at the ERP amplitude. A direct comparison with previous studies’ results is difficult because these studies used different designs. Our results might be caused by using the visual stimuli of the self-hand without discrimination and the high similarity of the visual stimuli between the self-hand and other hands. The discrimination task used by previous studies may orient attention to one’s own hand because the subjects have no image of other hands. The P300 component usually reflects the course of attention to a stimulus [39,43]. Thus, ERP amplitudes may be susceptible to attention orienteering by tasks and thus may be unsuitable for detecting self-specific responses. However, because the sample size was small, the result of no significant difference between the ERP amplitudes of the self-hand and the other hands should be interpreted with care.
Remarkably, this study revealed a significant difference between the distractor stimulus of self-hand and standard stimulus, and we found that high gamma ERS was induced in 650–900 ms at Cz on self-hand stimuli compared to other hands. ERD/ERS by time-frequency analysis are time-locked components to the event but not phase-locked and can reflect an induced oscillatory response, which cannot be extracted by a simple linear method, such as simple averaging [30,31]. Although there are very few studies measuring evoked oscillatory responses to self-relevant stimuli, Knyazev et al. mentioned the possibility that oscillatory activity specific to self-referencing does occur in the late time window [35]. Our result might have detected self-specific frequency components of brain oscillation activity that are offset as brain noises by simple averaging analysis. Gamma band activity in the neocortex may be generated by responses to sensory stimuli of various modalities and tasks [44,45]. Although the applied time-frequency analysis of hand images as self-referential stimuli has not been reported, several studies reported that the hand motion observation induced high gamma EEG changes [46,47]. Darvas et al. [46] showed the moving hand elicited high gamma activity (70–100 Hz) at the interval from 378 ms to 898 ms around the primary motor area. They suggested that high gamma activity in the observation of biological motion reflects the overall activity of the mirror neuron system. In fact, it has also been reported that high gamma activity at the cortical motor area increased around the hand movement onset and became the most pronounced at the end of the reaching movement [48,49]. Thus, there is a relationship between hand motion and high gamma activity, which can be elicited by observing hand motion. In other words, when motor imagery activity is triggered by visual information of the hand, high gamma band activity associated with hand movement may be observed. In fact, a study using magnetoencephalography to measure brain activity during a hand mental rotation task, which involves motor imagery, gamma band activity in the parieto-occipital lobe was observed [50,51]. Therefore, our results suggest that a visual image of the self-hand may enhance the high gamma activity related to the mirror neuron system, which is supposed to be involved in the hand motion from visual information more than the other hands.
Previous studies have reported that changes in the alpha band activity are mainly associated with self-referential activity [32,33,34,35] and similarly to self-face stimuli [52,53], although similar changes have also been reported when recognizing preferred faces [54,55]. These previous studies may suggest that they roughly reflect top-down processes of visual attention. In this study, top-down process attention was not paid to the self-hand compared with others, as shown by the ERP amplitude results, and therefore, it is possible that a change in the alpha band was not observed.
The fact that the gamma activity in the parietal region band was observed by the self-hand image may support the use of kinesthetic illusion as rehabilitation. It has been reported that kinesthetic illusions could generate motor imagery and might have some effects of restoring motor dysfunction caused by various diseases [56,57] and recovering muscle strength by improving the excitability of the corticospinal tract [58]. However, previous studies have suggested that the effect was different for the image of one’s hand and that of another hand [11,12,13]. Our results suggest the hypothesis that the self-hand is more effective in inducing kinesthetic illusions than other hands.
We should specify some limitations of our research. First, in this study, the sample size was small (10 subjects). Although the statistical power obtained from the two-way analysis of variance for ERP amplitudes calculated using G*power was not small, it may not be sufficient. A concept closely aligned to type II error is statistical power; thus, the result of no significant difference between the ERP amplitudes of the self-hand and the other hands should be interpreted with caution. Second, the spatial resolution of EEG localization was low due to the small number of electrodes used in the measurement. Although we found brain oscillatory responses specific to visual stimuli of the self-hand, the exact localization of this activity was unclear. Further study of where to apply dense electrodes to explore the localization of activity should be conducted. In particular, for the ERPs in the time interval of 150 to 300 ms, the activity at occipital sites could provide valuable information about visual stimuli processing.

5. Conclusions

The simple averaging analysis found no significant differences between the responses for three stimulus tasks in all time windows. However, time-frequency analysis showed that self-hand stimuli elicited high gamma ERS in 650–900 ms at the Cz electrode compared with other hand stimuli. The time-frequency analysis might have detected self-specific frequency components of brain oscillation activity offset as brain noises by simple averaging analysis. The visual image of the self-hand may enhance the mirror neuron system related to hand motion more than images of other hands. Our results may bring us some beneficial information for selecting images to facilitate motor function. In future work, we will need to use a dense electrode that provides additional and more exact information.

Author Contributions

All co-authors contributed to the article as follows: M.U. conducted all aspects of the work, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript; K.U., T.I. and C.S. administered literature review, analyzed the data, and edited the manuscript; M.H. analyzed the data and edited the manuscript; R.I. and Y.N. oversaw the study, managed every part of the research, and edited the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Our research complied with the guidelines for human studies and was conducted ethically in accordance with the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. This study was conducted with the approval of the Osaka Prefecture University Graduate School General Rehabilitation Studies Ethics Committee (approval number 2018-201, Tokyo, Japan). The explanation on research cooperation was given orally and in writing to all subjects, and they signed an informed consent form.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this article. Further enquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Rogers, T.B.; Kuiper, N.A.; Kirker, W.S. Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1977, 35, 677–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Williams, D.M.; Nicholson, T.; Grainger, C. The Self-Reference Effect on Perception: Undiminished in Adults with Autism and No Relation to Autism Traits. Autism Res. 2018, 11, 331–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Yamamoto, K.; Masumoto, K. Brief Report: Memory for Self-Performed Actions in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Why Does Memory of Self Decline in ASD? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2018, 48, 3216–3222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Fuentes-Claramonte, P.; Martin-Subero, M.; Salgado-Pineda, P.; Santo-Angles, A.; Argila-Plaza, I.; Salavert, J.; Arévalo, A.; Bosque, C.; Sarri, C.; Guerrero-Pedraza, A.; et al. Brain imaging correlates of self- and other-reflection in schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin. 2020, 25, 102134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Green, M.F.; Horan, W.P.; Lee, J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 16, 620–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Butti, N.; Montirosso, R.; Giusti, L.; Piccinini, L.; Borgatti, R.; Urgesi, C. Early Brain Damage Affects Body Schema and Person Perception Abilities in Children and Adolescents with Spastic Diplegia. Neural. Plast. 2019, 2019, 1678984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Candini, M.; Farinelli, M.; Ferri, F.; Avanzi, S.; Cevolani, D.; Gallese, V.; Northoff, G.; Frassinetti, F. Implicit and Explicit Routes to Recognize the Own Body: Evidence from Brain Damaged Patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Feinberg, T.E.; Venneri, A.; Simone, A.M.; Fan, Y.; Northoff, G. The neuroanatomy of asomatognosia and somatoparaphrenia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2010, 81, 276–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Vallar, G.; Ronchi, R. Somatoparaphrenia: A body delusion. A review of the neuropsychological literature. Exp. Brain Res. 2009, 192, 533–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Jenkinson, P.M.; Haggard, P.; Ferreira, N.C.; Fotopoulou, A. Body ownership and attention in the mirror: Insights from somatoparaphrenia and the rubber hand illusion. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 1453–1462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Aoyama, T.; Kaneko, F.; Hayami, T.; Shibata, E. The effects of kinesthetic illusory sensation induced by a visual stimulus on the corticomotor excitability of the leg muscles. Neurosci. Lett. 2012, 514, 106–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kaneko, F.; Yasojima, T.; Kizuka, T. Kinesthetic illusory feeling induced by a finger movement movie effects on corticomotor excitability. Neuroscience 2007, 149, 976–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Kaneko, F.; Blanchard, C.; Lebar, N.; Nazarian, B.; Kavounoudias, A.; Romaiguère, P. Brain Regions Associated to a Kinesthetic Illusion Evoked by Watching a Video of One’s Own Moving Hand. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0131970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bentin, S.; Allison, T.; Puce, A.; Perez, E.; McCarthy, G. Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1996, 8, 551–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Eimer, M. Effects of face inversion on the structural encoding and recognition of faces. Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2000, 10, 145–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Itier, R.J.; Taylor, M.J. N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal differences between object and face processing using ERPs. Cereb. Cortex. 2004, 14, 132–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Gunji, A.; Inagaki, M.; Inoue, Y.; Takeshima, Y.; Kaga, M. Event-related potentials of self-face recognition in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Brain Dev. 2009, 31, 139–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Sui, J.; Zhu, Y.; Han, S. Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: An event-related brain potential study. Neuroreport 2006, 17, 423–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Zhu, M.; Luo, J.; Zhao, N.; Hu, Y.; Yan, L.; Gao, X. The temporal primacy of self-related stimuli and negative stimuli: An ERP-based comparative study. Soc. Neurosci. 2016, 11, 507–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ishii, R.; Canuet, L.; Aoki, Y.; Hata, M.; Iwase, M.; Ikeda, S.; Nishida, K.; Ikeda, M. Healthy and Pathological Brain Aging: From the Perspective of Oscillations, Functional Connectivity, and Signal Complexity. Neuropsychobiology 2017, 75, 151–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Frassinetti, F.; Maini, M.; Romualdi, S.; Galante, E.; Avanzi, S. Is it mine? Hemispheric asymmetries in corporeal self-recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008, 20, 1507–1516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Frassinetti, F.; Maini, M.; Benassi, M.; Avanzi, S.; Cantagallo, A.; Farnè, A. Selective impairment of self-body-parts processing in right brain-damaged patients. Cortex 2010, 46, 322–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Sanabria, D.; Madrid, E.; Aranda, C.; Ruz, M. Attentional orienting to own and others’ hands. Exp. Brain Res. 2015, 233, 2347–2355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Su, Y.; Chen, A.; Yin, H.; Qiu, J.; Lv, J.; Wei, D.; Tian, F.; Tu, S.; Wang, T. Spatiotemporal cortical activation underlying self-referencial processing evoked by self-hand. Biol. Psychol. 2010, 85, 219–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Berlucchi, G.; Aglioti, S.M. The body in the brain revisited. Exp. Brain Res. 2010, 200, 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Giabbiconi, C.M.; Jurilj, V.; Gruber, T.; Vocks, S. Steady-state visually evoked potential correlates of human body perception. Exp. Brain Res. 2016, 234, 3133–3143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Meeren, H.K.; de Gelder, B.; Ahlfors, S.P.; Hämäläinen, M.S.; Hadjikhani, N. Different cortical dynamics in face and body perception: An MEG study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e71408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Myers, A.; Sowden, P.T. Your hand or mine? The extrastriate body area. Neuroimage 2008, 42, 1669–1677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Hodzic, A.; Muckli, L.; Singer, W.; Stirn, A. Cortical responses to self and others. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 951–962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bertrand, O.; Tallon-Baudry, C. Oscillatory gamma activity in humans: A possible role for object representation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2000, 38, 211–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Pfurtscheller, G. Event-related synchronization (ERS): An electrophysiological correlate of cortical areas at rest. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1992, 83, 62–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ben-Simon, E.; Podlipsky, I.; Arieli, A.; Zhdanov, A.; Hendler, T. Never resting brain: Simultaneous representation of two alpha related processes in humans. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e3984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Chen, J.L.; Ros, T.; Gruzelier, J.H. Dynamic changes of ICA-derived EEG functional connectivity in the resting state. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013, 34, 852–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Sadaghiani, S.; Scheeringa, R.; Lehongre, K.; Morillon, B.; Giraud, A.L.; D’Esposito, M.; Kleinschmidt, A.K. α-band phase synchrony is related to activity in the fronto-parietal adaptive control network. J. Neurosci. 2012, 32, 14305–14310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Knyazev, G.G. Extraversion and anterior vs. posterior DMN activity during self-referential thoughts. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 6, 348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Foster, B.L.; Dastjerdi, M.; Parvizi, J. Neural populations in human posteromedial cortex display opposing responses during memory and numerical processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 15514–15519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Mu, Y.; Han, S. Neural oscillations dissociate between self-related attentional orientation versus evaluation. Neuroimage 2013, 67, 247–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Ishii, R.; Canuet, L.; Herdman, A.; Gunji, A.; Iwase, M.; Takahashi, H.; Nakahachi, T.; Hirata, M.; Robinson, S.E.; Pantev, C.; et al. Cortical oscillatory power changes during auditory oddball task revealed by spatially filtered magnetoencephalography. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2009, 120, 497–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Polich, J. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 2128–2148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Hoechstetter, K.; Bornfleth, H.; Weckesser, D.; Ille, N.; Berg, P.; Scherg, M. BESA source coherence: A new method to study cortical oscillatory coupling. Brain Topogr. 2004, 16, 233–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Maris, E.; Oostenveld, R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 2007, 164, 177–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Maris, E. Statistical testing in electrophysiological studies. Psychophysiology 2012, 49, 549–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Romero, R.; Polich, J. P3(00) habituation from auditory and visual stimuli. Physiol Behav. 1996, 59, 517–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Iijima, M.; Mase, R.; Osawa, M.; Shimizu, S.; Uchiyama, S. Event-Related Synchronization and Desynchronization of High-Frequency Electroencephalographic Activity during a Visual Go/No-Go Paradigm. Neuropsychobiology 2015, 71, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Ikeda, S.; Mizuno-Matsumoto, Y.; Canuet, L.; Ishii, R.; Aoki, Y.; Hata, M.; Katsimichas, T.; Pascual-Marqui, R.D.; Hayashi, T.; Okamoto, E.; et al. Emotion Regulation of Neuroticism: Emotional Information Processing Related to Psychosomatic State Evaluated by Electroencephalography and Exact Low-Resolution Brain Electromagnetic Tomography. Neuropsychobiology 2015, 71, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Darvas, F.; Rao, R.P.; Murias, M. Localized high gamma motor oscillations respond to perceived biologic motion. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2013, 30, 299–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Smith, M.M.; Weaver, K.E.; Grabowski, T.J.; Rao, R.P.; Darvas, F. Non-invasive detection of high gamma band activity during motor imagery. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ball, T.; Demandt, E.; Mutschler, I.; Neitzel, E.; Mehring, C.; Vogt, K.; Aertsen, A.; Schulze-Bonhage, A. Movement related activity in the high gamma range of the human EEG. Neuroimage 2008, 41, 302–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Darvas, F.; Scherer, R.; Ojemann, J.G.; Rao, R.P.; Miller, K.J.; Sorensen, L.B. High gamma mapping using EEG. Neuroimage 2010, 49, 930–938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. de Lange, F.P.; Jensen, O.; Bauer, M.; Toni, I. Interactions between posterior gamma and frontal alpha/beta oscillations during imagined actions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2008, 2, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. van Wijk, B.C.; Litvak, V.; Friston, K.J.; Daffertshofer, A. Nonlinear coupling between occipital and motor cortex during motor imagery: A dynamic causal modeling study. Neuroimage 2013, 71, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Alzueta, E.; Melcón, M.; Jensen, O.; Capilla, A. The ‘Narcissus Effect’: Top-down alpha-beta band modulation of face-related brain areas during self-face processing. Neuroimage 2020, 213, 116754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Miyakoshi, M.; Kanayama, N.; Iidaka, T.; Ohira, H. EEG evidence of face-specific visual self-representation. Neuroimage 2010, 50, 1666–1675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Kang, J.H.; Kim, S.J.; Cho, Y.S.; Kim, S.P. Modulation of Alpha Oscillations in the Human EEG with Facial Preference. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0138153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Park, J.; Kim, H.; Sohn, J.W.; Choi, J.R.; Kim, S.P. EEG Beta Oscillations in the Temporoparietal Area Related to the Accuracy in Estimating Others’ Preference. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Cramer, S.C.; Orr, E.L.; Cohen, M.J.; Lacourse, M.G. Effects of motor imagery training after chronic, complete spinal cord injury. Exp. Brain Res. 2007, 177, 233–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Page, S.J.; Levine, P.; Leonard, A. Mental practice in chronic stroke: Results of a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Stroke 2007, 38, 1293–1297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Inada, T.; Kaneko, F.; Hayami, T. Effect of kinesthetic illusion induced by visual stimulation on muscular output function after short-term immobilization. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 2016, 27, 66–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Sample sequence in the visual oddball task.
Figure 1. Sample sequence in the visual oddball task.
Brainsci 12 00272 g001
Figure 2. Grand average ERPs from the 10 subjects for each condition.
Figure 2. Grand average ERPs from the 10 subjects for each condition.
Brainsci 12 00272 g002
Figure 3. Results of time-frequency analysis. 3 sensor positions on the scalp (Fz = Frontal zero, Cz = Central zero, Pz = Parietal zero) by the international 10–20 system EEG placement were used to record EEG. Paired t-test result of distractor–standard stimuli in all subjects and channels of time-frequency data and paired t-test result of distractor–standard stimuli only in the Cz channel. Significant increase in high gamma (60–80 Hz) band activity to self-hand was observed within 650–900 ms after stimulus onset in the Cz channel. In the time-frequency plots, the x-axis denotes the time relative to the stimulus onset (ms), and the y-axis denotes the frequency of oscillatory activity (Hz). The color bar shows the percentage of decrease (blue) and increases (red) in cortical power the 1000 ms post-stimuli.
Figure 3. Results of time-frequency analysis. 3 sensor positions on the scalp (Fz = Frontal zero, Cz = Central zero, Pz = Parietal zero) by the international 10–20 system EEG placement were used to record EEG. Paired t-test result of distractor–standard stimuli in all subjects and channels of time-frequency data and paired t-test result of distractor–standard stimuli only in the Cz channel. Significant increase in high gamma (60–80 Hz) band activity to self-hand was observed within 650–900 ms after stimulus onset in the Cz channel. In the time-frequency plots, the x-axis denotes the time relative to the stimulus onset (ms), and the y-axis denotes the frequency of oscillatory activity (Hz). The color bar shows the percentage of decrease (blue) and increases (red) in cortical power the 1000 ms post-stimuli.
Brainsci 12 00272 g003
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ueda, M.; Ueno, K.; Inamoto, T.; Shiroma, C.; Hata, M.; Ishii, R.; Naito, Y. Parietal Gamma Band Oscillation Induced by Self-Hand Recognition. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 272. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020272

AMA Style

Ueda M, Ueno K, Inamoto T, Shiroma C, Hata M, Ishii R, Naito Y. Parietal Gamma Band Oscillation Induced by Self-Hand Recognition. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(2):272. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020272

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ueda, Masaya, Keita Ueno, Takashi Inamoto, China Shiroma, Masahiro Hata, Ryouhei Ishii, and Yasuo Naito. 2022. "Parietal Gamma Band Oscillation Induced by Self-Hand Recognition" Brain Sciences 12, no. 2: 272. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020272

APA Style

Ueda, M., Ueno, K., Inamoto, T., Shiroma, C., Hata, M., Ishii, R., & Naito, Y. (2022). Parietal Gamma Band Oscillation Induced by Self-Hand Recognition. Brain Sciences, 12(2), 272. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020272

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop