Next Article in Journal
Cognitive Considerations in Major Depression: Evaluating the Effects of Pharmacotherapy and ECT on Mood and Executive Control Deficits
Previous Article in Journal
Effect of Audiovisual Cross-Modal Conflict during Working Memory Tasks: A Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Altered Pain Processing Associated with Administration of Dopamine Agonist and Antagonist in Healthy Volunteers

by
Sarah L. Martin
1,2,*,
Anthony K. P. Jones
2,
Christopher A. Brown
2,3,
Christopher Kobylecki
4,
Grace A. Whitaker
5,
Wael El-Deredy
6 and
Monty A. Silverdale
4
1
Department of Psychology, Manchester Metropolitan University, Manchester M15 6GX, UK
2
The Human Pain Research Group, Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, The University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, UK
3
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool L69 3BX, UK
4
Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust, Department of Neurology, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, Salford M6 8HD, UK
5
Advanced Center for Electrical and Electronics Engineering, Federico Santa María Technical University, Valparaíso 1680, Chile
6
Centro de Investigación y Desarrollo en Ingeniería en Salud, Universidad de Valparaíso, Valparaiso 1680, Chile
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(3), 351; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12030351
Submission received: 2 February 2022 / Revised: 22 February 2022 / Accepted: 23 February 2022 / Published: 4 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Neuromuscular and Movement Disorders)

Abstract

:
Striatal dopamine dysfunction is associated with the altered top-down modulation of pain processing. The dopamine D2-like receptor family is a potential substrate for such effects due to its primary expression in the striatum, but evidence for this is currently lacking. Here, we investigated the effect of pharmacologically manipulating striatal dopamine D2 receptor activity on the anticipation and perception of acute pain stimuli in humans. Participants received visual cues that induced either certain or uncertain anticipation of two pain intensity levels delivered via a CO2 laser. Rating of the pain intensity and unpleasantness was recorded. Brain activity was recorded with EEG and analysed via source localisation to investigate neural activity during the anticipation and receipt of pain. Participants completed the experiment under three conditions, control (Sodium Chloride), D2 receptor agonist (Cabergoline), and D2 receptor antagonist (Amisulpride), in a repeated-measures, triple-crossover, double-blind study. The antagonist reduced an individuals’ ability to distinguish between low and high pain following uncertain anticipation. The EEG source localisation showed that the agonist and antagonist reduced neural activations in specific brain regions associated with the sensory integration of salient stimuli during the anticipation and receipt of pain. During anticipation, the agonist reduced activity in the right mid-temporal region and the right angular gyrus, whilst the antagonist reduced activity within the right postcentral, right mid-temporal, and right inferior parietal regions. In comparison to control, the antagonist reduced activity within the insula during the receipt of pain, a key structure involved in the integration of the sensory and affective aspects of pain. Pain sensitivity and unpleasantness were not changed by D2R modulation. Our results support the notion that D2 receptor neurotransmission has a role in the top-down modulation of pain.

1. Introduction

Impaired dopaminergic transmission has been associated with chronic pain conditions [1,2,3] such as Fibromyalgia [4,5], atypical facial pain [6], and pain in Parkinson’s disease [7,8]. However, dopamine is unlikely to have a direct role in coding the intensity of pain; the majority of dopaminergic neurons do not respond directly to aversive stimuli, with only a small percentage (5–15%) reportedly being activated during pain perception [9]. Instead, changes in the dopaminergic system have been associated with the chronification of pain [10,11,12].
Dopamine has a widespread modulatory role in a variety of cognitive and affective processes such as attention flexibility [13,14,15], reward processing [16], salience assignment [17,18,19,20,21], motivational states [22,23], and emotional processing [24,25,26]. In terms of pain processing, recent theories of dopamine have focussed on its potential top-down modulatory function by considering its role within predictive coding, in which dopamine is hypothesised to modulate the precision (reliability) and confidence of subjective predictions about pain [27,28], such as those occurring during the anticipation of a painful event [29].
On this basis, one possibility is that the analgesic qualities of dopamine are related to the top-down cognitive and affective modulation of pain rather than modulation of afferent nociception [30,31,32]. In support of this idea, dopamine appears to interact with systems involved with the endogenous (top-down) modulation of pain [33,34,35]. Propositions of dopamine function in modulating pain include processing stimulus salience (i.e., the extent to which pain stands out and captures attention) [17,36,37,38,39], the assignment of emotional valence [40], and the modulation of sensorimotor and motivation-related processes [41,42]. However, there is currently a lack of evidence in support of dopamine modulating the anticipation of pain.
A possible molecular substrate for top-down modulation of pain anticipation is the population of the dopamine D2-like receptor (D2R) family within the striatum, which has been associated with the modulation of pain [43,44]. The D2R family consists of the D2, D3, and D4 dopaminergic receptors. For instance, direct injection of the D2R agonists apomorphine or quinpirole into the striatum reduced pain-related behaviours in rodents, whilst a D1-like receptor (D1R) agonist did not [43,45]. In humans, PET imaging has shown that striatal D2R binding potential is inversely correlated with an individuals’ pain tolerance of the cold pressor test [44] and heat thermode [46], as well as their ability to suppress pain [44]. However, the role of the D2R in the top-down modulation of pain processing has yet to be directly investigated.
Top-down modulation has been researched via recording anticipation-evoked neural processing prior to the receipt of pain [29,47,48,49]. During the anticipation of a painful stimulus, the brain incorporates information from the environment and recalls prior experiences to construct a mental representation of the forthcoming pain. The brain also incorporates cognitive factors such as the current emotional state. During the anticipation period, these factors modulate the perception of the pain [50,51]. The brain regions which have been shown to be active during the anticipation of pain include the prefrontal cortex, the cingulate cortex, and the insula [52,53,54,55], all of which have connections with limbic regions, and together incorporate the subjective perception of pain [12,56].
D2R receptors are associated with non-nociceptive anticipatory processing within the prefrontal cortex [57], cingulate cortex [58,59], insula [60], and somatosensory thalamocortical connections [61]. The D2R’s role in nociceptive anticipation is yet to be reported. Therefore, this study uses EEG with source localisation and behavioural metrics (pain rating, pain unpleasantness and pain sensitivity) to compare the effects of a D2R agonist and antagonist to a control condition. We investigate the modulation of the D2R neural activations during the anticipation and the receipt of pain. The research was conducted in a healthy cohort with the aim to better understand how altered D2R processing may play a part in chronic pain conditions.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Ethical Approval

The study was approved by the University of Manchester Ethics Committee and by the UK Health Research Authority for the use of a National Health Service (NHS) site (Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust Hospital).

2.2. Drug Selection

We selected a D2R agonist, Cabergoline, and a D2R antagonist, Amisulpride, which both have a high affinity for D2Rs.
Cabergoline is a potent agonist of dopamine D2Rs, with high affinity of D2 (IC50 = 3.0 nM/L, Ki = 0.69 nM) and D3 (IC50 = 4.0 nM/L, Ki = 1.5 nM) receptors within the striatum [62,63,64], and typically used to increase dopamine in people with Parkinson’s. Cabergoline has a higher affinity for D2Rs than other agonists such as Pergolide and Bromocriptine [62]. Cabergoline has low affinity for D1, serotonergic (5-HT1D, 5-HT2A, 5-HT2B), and adrenergic receptors (α1 and α2) [62]. The affinity for these receptors is negligible compared to the high affinity at D2Rs and will therefore not be discussed in the interpretation of the results
Amisulpride is a D2R antagonist selective for D2 and D3 receptor subtypes and typically used as an anti-psychotic. In an in vivo rat model, Amisulpride was shown to inhibit D2R binding within the striatum and limbic system, with preferential action within the limbic system [65]. Amisulpride has a high affinity and selectivity for D2 (Ki = 2.8 nM) and D3 (Ki = 3.2 nM) [66] and moderate affinity to serotonergic receptors 5-HT2B (Ki = 13.0 nM) and 5-HT7 (Ki = 11.5 nM) (5-HT7A is the most commonly expressed variant of the 5-HT7 receptors) [67]. D2R/D3R affinity predominates over 5-HT7 receptor affinity in a ratio of ~ 3:1 [67]. The 5-HT2B receptors have not been directly associated with pain processing [68], [69], whereas the 5HT7A receptor is involved in pain processing—primarily at the spinal level [70,71,72]. Both receptors will be considered in the interpretation of our results.
Previous studies have shown that at low doses of D2R drugs, the binding of the presynaptic D2 auto-receptor is favoured and produces the opposite of the desired results (i.e., agonist decreases dopaminergic signalling and vice versa) [73,74] (see Appendix A, Figure A1). Therefore, a sufficiently high dose was selected to modulate the postsynaptic D2R and resulted in the agonist increasing dopaminergic D2R signalling and the antagonist reducing D2R signalling. The dosage for Cabergoline (1.25 mg) and Amisulpride (400 mg) was based on previous studies which demonstrated cognitive effects in the appropriate direction at the chosen dose with minimal side effects (Cabergoline: [75,76] and Amisulpride: [77]). Cabergoline has an elimination half-life of between 63 and 69 h [78], and peak plasma concentrations are observed between 2 and 3 h [78]. Amisulpride has an elimination half-life of 12-h after an oral doseand peak plasma concentrations are observed at approximately 3 h [79].

2.3. Participant Recruitment

A total of 29 healthy participants (16 females) were recruited for the study (mean age 22.4 years, SD 3.2 years). All subjects gave written informed consent. The exclusion criteria of the study were as follows: history of significant head injury or seizures, diagnosed or taking medication for any neurological or psychiatric condition, history of drug or alcohol dependence, use of psychotropic medication within the past 6 months, use of dopaminergic drugs within the past month or lifetime use exceeding 3 months, pregnant or breastfeeding or attempting to conceive, and suffering from chronic pain. The phase of menstrual cycle was not an exclusion criterion and was not recorded at experimental visits.
To limit the variability in baseline dopamine level between subjects, prior to recruitment, all participants completed the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS-11, [80]) subscale of ‘cognitive instability’. The degree of impulsivity has been shown via positron emission tomography (PET) and single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) imaging studies to be significantly correlated to dopaminergic transmission and receptor/transporter abundance within the striatum [81,82,83,84]. Participants were only recruited if their score was within 1.5 standard deviations of the mean reported from a large control sample (1577 healthy adults) [85]. It is important to note that no imaging was carried out within our participants to confirm our use of the BIS-11 to limit variability in baseline dopamine.
The study used a repeated-measures, double-blind, and triple-crossover design such that all participants were recruited to attend three visits and complete the experimental protocol following ingestion of the agonist, antagonist, and control substance. Two participants did not complete all three visits of the study due to withdrawing from the study, and one participant was removed due to an adverse reaction to Amisulpride. Therefore, 26 datasets were included for behavioural and EEG analyses (mean age 22.2 years, SD 3.7 years, 14 females).

2.4. Health Screening

Prior to the first experimental visit, the participants attended a health screening to deem them safe to take part in the study. A medical doctor assessed the participants’ heart rate, blood pressure, and temperature and recorded an electrocardiogram (ECG). Due to the potential risk of Amisulpride causing arrhythmias, all participants were assessed for QTc abnormalities in the ECG prior to inclusion in the study. No participant presented with any abnormalities resulting in the exclusion from the study. A letter to the participant’s general practitioner (GP) was also organised to inform them of the participant’s involvement in the study.

2.5. Prior to Experimental Visits

Participants were instructed to not consume alcohol for 24 h prior to the visit, to only drink their normal intake of coffee or tea on the morning of each visit, and to refrain from consuming other caffeinated drinks 2 h prior to each visit. Participants were also informed to not consume any psychoactive substances for the duration of the study or any over-the-counter medicine 48 h prior to each visit.

2.6. Experimental Visit Protocol

2.6.1. Drug Administration

One of three substances was administered orally to the participant at each experimental visit: Cabergoline (Agonist) (1.25 mg), Amisulpride (Antagonist) (400 mg), or Sodium Chloride (Control) (20 mg). All drugs hold a full product licence (EU). All participants were provided with the potential side effects of the drugs. Participants were instructed that the drugs may or may not modulate dopamine levels, and no further details were provided regarding the potential effect on pain experience. The administration of the drug was double-blinded, and each participant had each drug condition once over the three visits in a randomised order. The 3 visits were separated by at least 10 days. The administration of the drug was recorded as time 0 h, and experimental testing commenced at 3 h 30 min post drug administration.

2.6.2. Eye Blink Rate

Previous research has indicated a potential correlation between the level of striatal dopamine and the rate of blinks per minute [86,87]. The blink rates of the participants were recorded using frontal EEG electrodes during a resting state of 9 minutes at +2 h after taking the drug and prior to any experimental testing. The participants were not informed that their blink rate was being assessed to avoid affecting their spontaneous blink rate.

2.6.3. Pain Stimuli

A CO₂ laser [50 W Synrad 48-5 J-series (J-48-5(S)W) Wavelength: 10,600 nm] was used to deliver acute pain to the right dorsal forearm surface [53]. The CO2 laser activates the A-delta and C-fibres at conduction velocities of ~10 m/s and ~1.0 m/s, respectively, producing a localized ‘sharp pricking’ sensation and ‘diffuse’ and ‘burning’ after-sensation [88,89]. The acute and fast activation of the A-delta fibres allowed for a precise anticipation period. In addition, the CO2 laser has been used to investigate the anticipatory response in previous studies [51,90,91,92]. The CO₂ laser delivered a beam with a diameter of 15 mm and 150 ms duration. The laser intensity was measured in voltage (V) and ranged from 0.6 V to 2.6 V. For each test, the stimuli were delivered in an area measuring 4 × 5 cm and were delivered in a predetermined randomised path [51]. This was to avoid habituation, sensitization, or skin damage.

2.6.4. Psychophysics

Before starting the experimental protocol, psychophysics was used to calibrate the laser to the individual’s pain sensitivity. An ascending method of limits procedure was used, starting from 0.6 V, with 0.06 increments each time. The participant used an 11-point numerical rating scale (NRS) (0−10) to rate the intensity of the pain perceived for each laser stimuli (0 = no sensation, 4 = pain threshold, 7 = moderately painful, 10 = unbearably painful). The rating scale was introduced to the participant via these standardised descriptives to ensure that no explanation altered their interpretation of the scale. The procedure was repeated three times to allow participants to become accustomed to the laser. The average voltage to induce level 4 (low) and level 7 (moderate) pain were used to provide ‘low’ and ‘high’ pain stimuli, respectively, for the main laser experiment protocol.

2.6.5. Main Experiment

The participants received 120 laser stimuli at the two intensities (low and high) separated into four conditions: Certain Low (level 4), Certain High (level 7), Uncertain Low (level 4), and Uncertain High (level 7). To investigate the anticipation to a painful stimulus, a 3-s auditory countdown preceded the laser stimuli (see Figure 1). The first auditory cue was presented concurrently with a visual anticipatory cue to indicate the forthcoming laser stimuli. The participant was either shown the word: ‘Low’, ‘High’ (certain anticipation), or ‘Unknown’ (uncertain anticipation). The presentation of the word ‘Unknown’ indicated that the laser stimulus has an equal chance of being low or high intensity. This was to investigate the importance of the degree of certainty in the anticipation of the laser stimuli. The image was also used as a visual fixation cue to discourage eye movements. After each laser stimulus, the 0–10 NRS was shown on the screen, and the participant rated the intensity of the pain via a numerical keypad. The order of the stimuli was randomised and separated into three experimental blocks of 40 stimuli, with 2-min breaks in-between. Following each block, the participants rated the unpleasantness on average for each laser condition (Certain Low, Certain High, Uncertain Low, and Uncertain High). Unpleasantness was scored using an 11-point NRS, whereby 0 is not unpleasant and 10 is the most unpleasant sensation.

2.6.6. EEG Recording

A BrainVision MR EEG cap was used to record data from 63 scalp electrodes using a BrainVision-cap system [Standard BrainCap-MR with Multitrodes]. The arrangement of the electrodes was modelled on the extended 10–20 system. Recording parameters were set at Filter (DC to 70 Hz), Sampling rate (1000 Hz), and Gain (500). To reduce electrical interference, a 50 Hz notch filter was applied. Prior to starting the laser protocol, resting states were recorded with eyes open and closed for two minutes in all participants. This ensured that the experience prior to the experiment was identical.

2.7. Analysis Methods

2.7.1. Statistical Analysis of Behavioural Data

Statistical analyses of the behavioural measures were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics 22 software. Prior to using statistical tests, the data were assessed for normality using a combination of Q–Q plots, histograms, and the values of skew and kurtosis. To assess whether dopamine modulation evoked changes in sensitivity to the laser stimuli, the participants’ tolerance to the laser (level 7 voltage) was compared between each drug condition using a repeated measures one-way ANOVA. In addition, the pain and unpleasantness rating of the experimental laser stimuli were investigated for drug-related changes via a repeated measures three-way ANOVA with within subject factors of drug (control, agonist, and antagonist), certainty (certain and uncertain), and intensity (low and high).

2.7.2. EEG Analysis Method

EEG pre-processing was carried out using EEGLAB toolbox [93] in MATLAB version R2015a (The Mathworks Inc), whilst statistical analysis was carried out using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12) toolbox (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology, UCL, London, UK) running in MATLAB.

2.7.3. Blink Rate

For each participant and visit, the number of blinks per minute was calculated using the frontal EEG electrodes of the resting state recording. The data were downsampled to 200 Hz, and eye blinks were identified using Independent Component Analysis (ICA) within the EEGLAB toolbox. ICA is used to identify and remove ocular artefacts, by discarding the components that contain them. A Matlab script used those eye blink dominant components, identified by its topography and morphology, to count the number of blinks in a stretch of 9 min of resting EEG recorded before the start of the main study. The blinks per minute were reported as the blink rate and were assessed for differences between dopamine manipulation and blink rate via contrast analysis.

2.7.4. EEGLAB Pre-Processing

Pre-processing consisted of the removal and interpolation of bad channels, down-sampling to 500 Hz, a low-pass filter of 20 Hz, and re-reference to the common average. The four conditions were separated, −3500 ms to 2000 ms epochs were extracted centred on the delivery of the laser stimulus, and Linear detrend was applied. Independent Component Analysis (ICA) was carried out on all datasets using the SemiAutomatic Selection of Independent Components for Artifact correction (SASICA) toolbox to select components to remove via pre-determined thresholds. The thresholds were set to Autocorrelation (threshold = 0.35 r, lag = 20 ms), Focal (threshold = 3.5 z), Focal trial (threshold 5.5 z), Signal-to-noise (period of interest (POI) = [0 Inf], baseline (BL) [-Inf 0], threshold ratio = 0.5), and Adjust Selection enabled. The thresholds were sufficient to remove artefacts from the majority of the datasets; however, a number of datasets required further manual removal of eye-blink components were not identified by SASICA. 2.7.5. SPM EEG Analysis
The pre-processed datasets were converted to SPM-compatible files. Statistical analysis was carried out to investigate the anticipation-evoked potentials and the post-stimulus laser evoked potentials (LEPs) using scalp-level and source localisation analysis techniques available in the SPM toolbox.
SPM scripts for batch processing were used to analyse the EEG data at the scalp level and for source localisation. Two baselining methods were applied to the data analysis for the anticipation phase and were applied for scalp-level and source localisation analysis. Primary analysis applied distinct baselines (BLs) of 500 ms, occurring prior to each of the three auditory cues to analyse each of the three anticipation phases (early, mid, and late) [94]. The BLs and time window of interest (TWOI) were as follows: Early [BL: −3500 ms −3000 ms: TWOI: −2500 ms −2000 ms], Mid [BL: −2500 ms −2000 ms: TWOI: −1500 −1000 ms], and Late [BL: −1500 −1000 ms: TWOI: −500 0 ms] anticipation phases. The aim of baselining uniquely for each anticipation window was to reduce variability in the data as the anticipation phase progressed, such that the analysis of each phase of anticipation was unique to that phase and not subject to variability arising from neural activity occurring in the previous phase. The secondary analysis method applied a single baseline of 500 ms prior to the first auditory cue [BL: −3500 ms −3000 ms] that was common to every TWOI anticipation phase (early, mid, and late). This second analysis was conducted to enable comparison to previous studies [51,53] that used the same baselining method and to explore to what extent the results from the primary analysis were dependent on the baselining method used. All statistical analysis for the anticipation phase was adjusted for multiple comparisons.
Analysis for the post-stimulus phase TWOI [200 ms 600 ms] (centred on the LEP), was baseline corrected to −500 ms prior to the laser stimulus [BL: −500 ms 0 ms].

2.7.5. Source Localisation Analysis Parameters

SPM12 EEG and MATLAB scripts were used to estimate the sources of the anticipation- and laser-evoked potentials using Low-Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (LORETA). The forward model was created using an 8196 vertex template cortical mesh coregistered to the electrode positions of the standard 10–20 EEG system. A three-shell boundary element (BEM) EEG head model available in SPM12 was used to compute the forward model. The images were smoothed with a 12 mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM).
Source localisation results were reported as follows: To control for multiple comparisons, a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001 was used, and resulting clusters were considered significant at FWE (p < 0.05). Significant clusters were also restricted to >100 voxels in size and regions labelled using the Anatomical Automatic Labelling (AAL2) toolbox in SPM. To investigate potential changes in all brain structures, initial analyses were not restricted to regions of interest. Follow-up small volume correction (SVC) was applied using a 25 mm sphere to further investigate effects within regions of the pain matrix, specifically the Insula, Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC), Thalamus, and Amygdala, using coordinates from the Brede Database [95]. Significant results following SVC are highlighted in the results tables.

2.7.6. EEG Analysis Statistical Analysis

The anticipatory time windows of interest (TWOIs) were analysed using a repeated-measure three-way ANOVA, with within-subject factors of Drug (Control vs. Agonist vs. Antagonist), Certainty (Certain vs. Uncertain), and Expectation (Certain Low vs. Certain High). The analysis of the post-stimuli TWOI was conducted using a repeated measure three-way ANOVA, with within subject factors of Drug (Control vs. Agonist vs. Antagonist), Certainty (Certain vs. Uncertain), and Intensity (Low vs. High).

3. Results

3.1. Behavioural Results

To assess whether dopamine modulation evoked changes in pain sensitivity, the participants’ tolerance to the laser was compared between each drug condition. The mean ± SD laser voltage to induce high (level 7) pain was calculated for control (1.93 ± 0.43 V), agonist (1.98 ± 0.49 V), and antagonist (1.97 ± 0.41 V). There was no significant drug-induced effect on sensitivity to the nociception F(2, 50) = 0.465, p = 0.631, (Table 1).
In addition, the mean pain rating score for each drug condition was calculated. The pain rating scores were normally distributed, as assessed by Shapiro–Wilk’s test of normality (p > 0.05). The within-subject comparison of the three Drug Conditions (Control, Agonist, and Antagonist) reported no effect on the pain rating F(2, 50) = 0.60, p = 0.552. This outcome was expected, as the laser intensity was calibrated to the participants’ individual pain sensitivity on each visit to induce a score of seven on the pain NRS.
As expected, there was a significant effect of intensity [F(1, 25) = 262.98, p< 0.001], such that high-intensity laser stimuli were rated as more painful than low intensity. The effect of Certainty (Certain vs. Uncertain) on the pain rating did not show a significant result F(1, 25)= 3.33, p = 0.80; however, a two-way interaction between Certainty and Intensity was shown [F(2, 50) = 47.82, p < 0.001], such that pain ratings increased from Certain Low (2.95 ± 0.85) and Uncertain Low (3.36 ± 0.98), but decreased between Certain High (6.31 ± 0.60) to Uncertain High (5.73 ± 0.56) (i.e., Pain rating: Certain Low < Uncertain Low < Uncertain High < Certain High). Therefore, the ‘Uncertain’ cue results in a reduced ability to distinguish between low and high laser stimuli.
Interestingly, there was a three-way interaction between Drug, Certainty, and Intensity [F(2, 50) = 5.60, p = 0.006]. This indicates that the modulation of D2Rs affected the interaction between Certainty and Intensity. Follow-up three-way ANOVAs established how the modulation of D2R compared to control (control vs. agonist, and control vs. antagonist) affected the interaction between certainty and intensity. Firstly, the comparison of control and agonist revealed no three-way interaction between Drug, Certainty, and Intensity [F(1,25) = 2.658, p = 0.116]. However, the comparison of control versus the antagonist revealed a significant three-way interaction, [F(1, 25) = 9.535, p < 0.005]. The effect of the uncertain cue increasing Low pain rating, and decreasing High pain rating was larger in the antagonist condition compared to the control condition. Hence, the ability to distinguish between low and high stimuli following the ‘Unknown’ cue was reduced in the antagonist condition.
There was no significant difference in the mean unpleasantness rating for each drug condition (Control, Agonist, and Antagonist) [F(2, 30) = 2.766, p = 0.079] (Table 2). There was an effect of Intensity such that high-intensity stimuli were rated more unpleasant than low-intensity stimuli [F(1, 15) = 197.75, p = 0.000]. There was also an effect of Certainty (Certain vs. Uncertain) [F(1, 15) = 11.924, p = 0.004] on unpleasantness ratings such that the uncertain cue of “Unknown” caused a higher rating in unpleasantness compared to the Certain cues of “Low” and “High”. This effect was consistent across all three drug conditions.

3.2. EEG Results

3.2.1. Blink Rate Analysis

The blink rate assessed via EEG was shown to have a significant linear relationship between striatal dopamine activity (F(1,27) = 7.242, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.211; Figure 2). The agonist increased the eye-blink rate, whilst the antagonist reduced the eye-blink rate, in comparison to the control condition, which is consistent with previous reports [96,97,98,99].

3.2.2. Scalp-Level EEG Analysis

The electrode-level analysis of the anticipation TWOIs (early, mid, and late) reported no main effect of Drug, Expectation, or Certainty for both baselining methods.
In addition, the analysis of the post-laser time window of interest (TWOI) [200 to 600 ms] at scalp-level also reported no significant effects of drug or certainty. A main effect of intensity produced two clusters (p < 0.001, kE = 20,418, x: −13 mm y: −36 mm, 464 ms; and p < 0.000, kE = 3510, x: 55 mm y: −68 mm, 450 ms) with the T-contrast showing an enhanced activation during high pain in contrast to low pain (p < 0.001, kE = 17,732, x: −13 mm, y: −41 mm, 460 ms).

3.2.3. Source EEG Analysis

Anticipatory Time Window of Interest

The source localisation analysis identified significant differences between conditions within the anticipatory- and laser-evoked time windows using the pre-cue baselines. Firstly, during mid-anticipation, both the agonist and antagonist reported a lower degree of activity within several regions compared to the control condition (see Table 3). The agonist induced lower activity in the right mid-temporal region and the angular gyrus. The antagonist induced reduced activity within the right postcentral, mid-temporal, and inferior parietal regions (Figure 3).
The expectation of a higher pain (Certain High) during mid-anticipation (Table 3) increased activation within the left mid-temporal, mid-occipital, and insula regions compared to the expectation to lower pain (Certain Low).

Post-Laser Time Window of Interest

The higher laser intensity evoked higher activation within the post-laser TWOI [200 ms to 600 ms] (High > Low: p < 0.001, F = 8.44, kE = 98,792, x: 8, y: −14, z: −16). Interestingly, there was a main effect of the drug conditions within the post-stimulus TWOI (Table 3). The antagonist induced a reduced level of activation within the right insula in comparison to the control condition (see Figure 4). In comparison to the agonist condition, the antagonist showed a lower activation within the right hippocampus, the mid- and inferior temporal regions, the insula, and Heschl’s Gyrus (auditory processing) (see Figure 4).

4. Discussion

This study investigated the role of the D2 dopaminergic receptors during pain anticipation and pain perception. To summarise, the key findings were the following: (1) The antagonist reduced an individuals’ ability to distinguish between low and high pain following uncertain anticipation, (2) the agonist and antagonist reduced neural activations in specific brain regions during anticipation and receipt of pain, and (3) pain sensitivity and rating of unpleasantness were not changed by D2R modulation.
The agonist and antagonist evoked changes in the resting blink rate—a method to measure striatal dopamine activity [96,97,98,99]. The agonist increased the blink rate, indicating higher dopamine signalling. The antagonist decreased the blink rate, indicating reduced dopaminergic signalling. Therefore, this suggests that the agonist and antagonist dosage used was sufficient to induce the desired dopamine modulation. However, there are contradictory reports of the accuracy of using the blink rate to quantify striatal dopamine activity [101,102], and therefore, without PET imaging to quantify dopaminergic activity, we cannot confirm changes in dopaminergic signalling.

4.1. Antagonist Effect on Pain Rating

The rating of the uncertain laser stimuli was modulated by the D2R antagonist, amisulpride. Across all drug conditions, uncertainty affected low and high pain differently, such that Uncertain Low was rated higher than Certain Low, whereas Uncertain High was rated lower than Certain High. Firstly this indicates that uncertainty reduces the ability to accurately rate the intensity of pain and is consistent with previous research [29,103]. The antagonist increased the effect of uncertainty on the pain rating, whereby (compared to the control) the pain rating was even higher for Uncertain Low and even lower for Uncertain High, therefore indicating that the reduction of dopaminergic signalling via D2Rs (and inhibition of 5-HT2B/7A receptors) resulted in a further reduced ability to distinguish between low and high pain when no information was provided prior to the stimuli.
Dopamine is important in the certainty and confidence of decisions and perceptions within the environment [104,105]. Schwartenbeck et al. (2015) proposed that confidence and precision in choices are encoded by the dopaminergic activity of the midbrain [27]. The Bayesian brain hypothesis, which states that perception is the integrated outcome of prior expectation and sensory information, present a potential explanation of the results. The antagonist may increase the weight of the prior (visual cue) and reduce the precision/confidence in the sensory information (laser stimulus), whereas the agonist may reduce the weight of the prior and increase the precision/confidence of the sensory information. Although no study has investigated the accuracy of pain perception, a study by Tomassini et al. (2016) reported that D2R antagonism led to increased uncertainty and reduced precision of the representation of time [106]. Therefore, we propose that the reduced dopaminergic signalling via the antagonist resulted in increased dependence on the visual cue and, therefore, decreased confidence in the sensory perception of the low and high pain stimuli following uncertain anticipation.
The antagonist also inhibits transmission at 5-HT2B/7A receptors at a lower affinity than D2Rs. Serotonergic transmission is documented to be involved in pain processing, particularly at the spinal level and within the descending pain pathway [107,108,109,110]. A global loss of serotonin has been associated with a reduced ability to discriminate laser stimuli intensity [111]; however, there is no evidence to show that 5-HT2B/7A receptors are associated with pain discrimination.

4.2. D2R Modulation Reduced Neural Activity during Anticipation

In comparison to the control condition, the agonist reduced activity in the right mid-temporal region and the right angular gyrus, whilst the antagonist reduced activity within the right postcentral, right mid-temporal, and right inferior parietal regions. These regions are known to be innervated by striatal dopaminergic neurons [112,113] and are involved in the top-down processing of salient stimuli. Therefore, we demonstrate that these regions rely (in some part) on the action of D2R-innervation during pain processing.
The agonist and antagonist reduced activity in the right mid-temporal lobe, a region rich in D2Rs [114] involved in sensory integration and semantic processing [115,116,117,118], and has shown increased activity during uncertain (non-nociceptive) anticipation [119] and pain anticipation [91]. The agonist also reduced activity in the angular gyrus, a region located between the temporal and parietal lobes, and is involved in integrating multi-sensory information to give sense to events and reorient attention to salient or informative stimuli [120,121,122,123,124]. Kluger et al. (2017) reported how uncertainty increased activity in the right angular gyrus [119].
The antagonist reduced activity in the right postcentral gyrus and the inferior parietal region. The postcentral gyrus is known to activate during pain processing [125], whilst the inferior parietal cortex is part of the right-lateralised attention network and is associated with the intentional focusing of attention toward pain, and in particular its spatial location [126,127]. A previous study using the same protocol reported increased activation of the inferior parietal cortex during anticipation [91]. This study demonstrates that these processes may involve D2R signalling during the top-down processing prior to pain receipt.
A further important consideration is the antagonist’s additional inhibition of 5-HT2B/7A receptors (albeit with lower affinity). To the best of our knowledge, there is no evidence that the 5-HT2B/7A receptors are involved in anticipatory processing and sensory integration. Nevertheless, 5-HT7 receptors are present within the mid-temporal lobe [128] and may have contributed to the reduced activity during anticipation seen in this study.
It is important to note that the agonist and antagonist both reduce neural activity despite having the desired opposite effect on dopamine signalling. One interpretation we propose is that the dose of the agonist or antagonist was not sufficiently high enough to increase or decrease dopaminergic transmission, respectively (due to the action of presynaptic D2R auto-receptors, see Figure A1), and instead both conditions result in a decrease/increase in dopamine signalling. However, the dosage selected consistent with previous reports indicating a desired pharmacodynamic effect and the resting blink-rate (a marker of striatal dopamine activity) reported a successful increase in dopamine in the agonist and a decrease in the antagonist condition.
Therefore, an alternative interpretation could be due to the inverted-u relationship of dopaminergic responses, such that both low and high dopamine levels produce the same output [126,127,128,129,130,131,132,133,134,135,136]. For instance, research by Friston et al. (2012) using Bayesian simulations demonstrated how “a single functional role for dopamine at the synaptic level can manifest in different ways at the behavioural level” [137]. However, the inverted-u relationship of dopamine is most commonly discussed in terms of behavioural outcomes, rather than neural activity, and therefore may be too simplistic to explain the current results.

4.3. Antagonist Reduced Neural Activity during Receipt of Pain

During receipt of pain, the antagonist induced a reduced level of activation within the right insula in comparison to the control condition, and reduced activity in the right hippocampus, mid- and inferior temporal region, and insula, when compared to the agonist condition.
The insula has been considered to be involved in coding intensity of pain [138] but likely plays a more complex role in integrating the sensory and affective aspects of pain [139,140]. The insula integrates top-down expectations with bottom-up nociceptive information [51,140,141]. Hence, D2R antagonism resulting in a reduced activation of the insula may reflect deficits in the optimal integration of sensory, affective, and cognitive information. This deficit in the insula’s integration of information may manifest behaviourally in the altered rating of the uncertain pain stimuli.
Dopamine’s role in the perception of stimuli has been investigated via the oddball task, whereby dopamine response increases when an unexpected tone is presented. In a rodent study, D2R antagonist (Eticlopride) reduced the neural activity within the midbrain during receipt of unexpected tones in the oddball paradigm [140], thus demonstrating how D2R antagonism can reduce neural activity in response to salient stimuli.

4.4. No Change in Pain Sensitivity or Unpleasantness

The individual’s pain sensitivity and rating of unpleasantness were not changed by D2R modulation. A study by Becker et al. (2013) reported similar results, such that neither Sulpiride (D2R (D2 and D3) antagonist) nor dopamine precursor depletion altered the unpleasantness ratings of thermal pain [141]. However, there are contrasting results from Tiemann et al. (2014), whereby they reported that an acute depletion in the dopamine precursor increased pain unpleasantness (not pain sensitivity) to laser stimuli [30]. Differing pain-delivery and dopamine manipulation methods and the acute action of modulation are likely to explain the differing findings.

4.5. Clinical Impact

Clinical research questions that follow from this study include whether chronic dopamine dysfunction (as found in conditions such as Parkinson’s disease and Fibromyalgia) might impair the top-down modulation of pain, resulting in chronic pain. The acute administration of D2R agonist and antagonist drugs may not be sufficient to understand how long-term changes in dopaminergic transmission can impact pain processing. However, the significant differences we have found following acute D2R manipulation within aspects of pain processing, along with previous research highlighting D2R dysfunction in chronic pain conditions [6], suggests that prolonged changes in dopamine signalling might have profound effects.

5. Conclusions

The study highlights the involvement of the dopaminergic system in top-down pain processing, and its involvement in sensory integration and accurate perception, rather than pain sensitivity. Neither drug modulated pain sensitivity or unpleasantness ratings; however, the antagonist (amisulpride) reduced the ability to distinguish pain intensity. EEG source localisation identified that both drugs reduced anticipatory activity in regions associated with salient stimuli processing and sensory integration. In comparison to the control condition, the antagonist reduced activity in the insula during the receipt of pain —a region associated with the integration of the sensory and affective aspects of pain. In conclusion, an acute change in D2R activation was sufficient to alter pain processing in a healthy cohort and presents a scientific rationale to investigate how chronic dysfunction of the D2R in chronic pain conditions contributes to symptoms.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.L.M., A.K.P.J., C.A.B., G.A.W., W.E.-D. and M.A.S.; data curation, S.L.M., C.A.B., G.A.W. and M.A.S.; formal analysis, S.L.M. and C.A.B.; funding acquisition, M.A.S.; investigation, S.L.M., C.K. and G.A.W.; methodology, S.L.M., A.K.P.J., C.A.B., C.K., G.A.W., W.E.-D. and M.A.S.; project administration, S.L.M., G.A.W. and M.A.S.; resources, A.K.P.J., G.A.W. and M.A.S.; supervision, C.A.B., C.K., W.E.-D. and M.A.S.; validation, A.K.P.J.; writing—original draft, S.L.M.; writing—review and editing, A.K.P.J., C.A.B., C.K., G.A.W., W.E.-D. and M.A.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Parkinson’s UK, grant number H-1404.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the University of Manchester Ethics Committee and by the UK Health Research Authority for the use of a National Health Service (NHS) site (Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust Hospital) (date of approval: 22 October 2015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent has been obtained from the patient(s) to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to additional analyses being conducted on the dataset.

Acknowledgments

G.A.W. was supported in part by the Centro Basal FB0008. Thank you to Timothy Rainey and Kate Lees for support during data collection and administration, respectively.

Conflicts of Interest

M.A.S. is a member of the editorial board of Brain Sciences. All other authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Figure A1. A schematic diagram of the action of the dopamine D2 receptor and how low and high doses of agonists and antagonists have different effects on postsynaptic D2 receptor activity. Presents the scientific rationale for using a high dose of both drugs in the current study.
Figure A1. A schematic diagram of the action of the dopamine D2 receptor and how low and high doses of agonists and antagonists have different effects on postsynaptic D2 receptor activity. Presents the scientific rationale for using a high dose of both drugs in the current study.
Brainsci 12 00351 g0a1

References

  1. Jääskeläinen, S.K.; Rinne, J.O.; Forssell, H.; Tenovuo, O.; Kaasinen, V.; Sonninen, P.; Bergman, J. Role of the dopaminergic system in chronic pain—A fluorodopa-PET study. Pain 2001, 90, 257–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Wood, P.B.; Schweinhardt, P.; Jaeger, E.; Dagher, A.; Hakyemez, H.; Rabiner, E.A.; Bushnell, M.C.; Chizh, B.A. Fibromyalgia patients show an abnormal dopamine response to pain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2007, 25, 3576–3582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Wood, P.B.; Patterson, J.C., II; Sunderland, J.J.; Tainter, K.H.; Glabus, M.F.; Lilien, D.L. Reduced Presynaptic Dopamine Activity in Fibromyalgia Syndrome Demonstrated with Positron Emission Tomography: A Pilot Study. J. Pain 2007, 8, 51–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Häuser, W. Fibromyalgia syndrome: Basic knowledge, diagnosis and treatment. Med. Mon. Pharm. 2016, 39, 504–511. [Google Scholar]
  5. Albrecht, D.S.; Mackie, P.J.; Kareken, D.A.; Hutchins, G.D.; Chumin, E.J.; Christian, B.T.; Yoder, K.K. Differential dopamine function in fibromyalgia. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016, 10, 829–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Hagelberg, N.; Forssell, H.; Aalto, S.; Rinne, J.O.; Scheinin, H.; Taiminen, T.; Någren, K.; Eskola, O.; Jääskeläinen, S.K. Altered dopamine D2 receptor binding in atypical facial pain. Pain 2003, 106, 43–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Blanchet, P.J.; Brefel-Courbon, C. Chronic pain and pain processing in Parkinson’s disease. Prog. Neuro. Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2018, 87, 200–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Silverdale, M.A.; Kobylecki, C.; Kass-Iliyya, L.; Martinez-Martin, P.; Lawton, M.; Cotterill, S.; Chaudhuri, K.R.; Morris, H.; Baig, F.; Williams, N.; et al. A detailed clinical study of pain in 1957 participants with early/moderate Parkinson’s disease. Park. Relat. Disord. 2018, 56, 27–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Taylor, A.M.W.; Becker, S.; Schweinhardt, P.; Cahill, C. Mesolimbic dopamine signaling in acute and chronic pain: Implications for motivation, analgesia, and addiction. Pain 2016, 157, 1194–1198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Yang, S.; Boudier-Revéret, M.; Choo, Y.J.; Chang, M.C. Association between Chronic Pain and Alterations in the Mesolimbic Dopaminergic System. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Serafini, R.A.; Pryce, K.D.; Zachariou, V. The Mesolimbic Dopamine System in Chronic Pain and Associated Affective Comorbidities. Biol. Psychiatry 2020, 87, 64–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Ong, W.-Y.; Stohler, C.S.; Herr, D.R. Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Pain Processing. Mol. Neurobiol. 2019, 56, 1137–1166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. van Holstein, M.; Aarts, E.; van der Schaaf, M.E.; Geurts, D.E.M.; Verkes, R.J.; Franke, B.; van Schouwenburg, M.R.; Cools, R. Human cognitive flexibility depends on dopamine D2 receptor signaling. Psychopharmacology 2011, 218, 567–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Klanker, M.; Feenstra, M.; Denys, D. Dopaminergic control of cognitive flexibility in humans and animals. Front. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Whitaker, G. Investigating the Role of Striatal Dopamine in Attentional Inhibition; The University of Manchester: Manchester, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  16. Arias-Carrión, Ó.; Pöppel, E. Dopamine, learning, and reward-seeking behavior. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2007, 67, 481–488. [Google Scholar]
  17. Nieoullon, A. Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Prog. Neurobiol. 2002, 67, 53–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kapur, S.; Mizrahi, R.; Li, M. From dopamine to salience to psychosis—Linking biology, pharmacology and phenomenology of psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 2005, 79, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Howes, O.D.; Nour, M. Dopamine and the aberrant salience hypothesis of schizophrenia. World Psychiatry 2016, 15, 3–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Shiner, T.; Symmonds, M.; Guitart-Masip, M.; Fleming, S.M.; Friston, K.J.; Dolan, R.J. Dopamine, Salience, and Response Set Shifting in Prefrontal Cortex. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 25, 3629–3639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Parr, T.; Friston, K.J. Working memory, attention, and salience in active inference. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Pultorak, K.J.; Schelp, S.; Isaacs, D.P.; Krzystyniak, G.; Oleson, E.B. A Transient Dopamine Signal Represents Avoidance Value and Causally Influences the Demand to Avoid. Eneuro 2018, 5, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Elliot, A.J.; Covington, M.V. Approach and Avoidance Motivation. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2001, 13, 73–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Salimpoor, V.N.; Benovoy, M.; Larcher, K.; Dagher, A.; Zatorre, R.J. Anatomically distinct dopamine release during an-ticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nat. Neurosci. 2011, 14, 257–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Baixauli, E. Happiness: Role of Dopamine and Serotonin on Mood and Negative Emotions. Emerg. Med. Open Access 2017, 7, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Peciña, M.; Mickey, B.J.; Love, T.; Wang, H.; Langenecker, S.; Hodgkinson, C.; Shen, P.-H.; Villafuerte, S.; Hsu, D.; Weisenbach, S.L.; et al. DRD2 polymorphisms modulate reward and emotion processing, dopamine neurotransmission and openness to experience. Cortex 2013, 49, 877–890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Friston, K.; Schwartenbeck, P.; Fitz, G.T.; Moutoussis, M.; Behrens, T.; Dolan, R. The anatomy of choice: Dopamine and decision-making. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2014, 369, 20130481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Fitzgerald, T.H.B.; Dolan, R.J.; Friston, K. Dopamine, reward learning, and active inference. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Brown, C.A.; Seymour, B.; El-Deredy, W.; Jones, A.K. Confidence in beliefs about pain predicts expectancy effects on pain perception and anticipatory processing in right anterior insula. Pain 2008, 139, 324–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Tiemann, L.; Heitmann, H.; Schulz, E.; Baumkötter, J.; Ploner, M. Dopamine Precursor Depletion Influences Pain Affect Rather than Pain Sensation. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e96167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Martikainen, I.K.; Hagelberg, N.; Jääskeläinen, S.K.; Hietala, J.; Pertovaara, A. Dopaminergic and serotonergic mechanisms in the modulation of pain: In vivo studies in human brain. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2018, 834, 337–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Urien, L.; Wang, J. Top-Down Cortical Control of Acute and Chronic Pain. Psychosom. Med. 2019, 81, 851–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Bhagyashree, A.; Manikkoth, S.; Sequeira, M.; Nayak, R.; Rao, S.N. Central dopaminergic system plays a role in the analgesic action of paracetamol: Preclinical evidence. Indian J. Pharmacol. 2017, 49, 21–25. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  34. Wood, P.B. Role of central dopamine in pain and analgesia. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2008, 8, 781–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Esposito, E.; Romandini, S.; Merlo-Pich, E.; Mennini, T.; Samanin, R. Evidence of the involvement of dopamine in the anal-gesic effect of nefopam. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 1986, 128, 157–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Cooper, J.C.; Knutson, B. Valence and salience contribute to nucleus accumbens activation. NeuroImage 2008, 39, 538–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Wulff, S.; Nielsen, M.Ø.; Rostrup, E.; Svarer, C.; Jensen, L.T.; Pinborg, L.; Glenthøj, B.Y. The relation between dopamine D2 receptor blockade and the brain reward system: A longitudinal study of first-episode schizophrenia patients. Psychol. Med. 2020, 50, 220–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gentry, R.N.; Schuweiler, D.R.; Roesch, M.R. Dopamine signals related to appetitive and aversive events in paradigms that manipulate reward and avoidability. Brain Res. 2018, 1713, 80–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Roughley, S.; Killcross, S. Differential involvement of dopamine receptor subtypes in the acquisition of Pavlovian sign-tracking and goal-tracking responses. Psychopharmacology 2019, 236, 1853–1862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Jensen, J.; Mc, I.A.; Crawley, A.P.; Mikulis, D.; Remington, G.; Kapur, S. Direct Activation of the Ventral Striatum in Anticipation of Aversive Stimuli. Neuron 2003, 40, 1251–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Horvitz, J.C. Dopamine gating of glutamatergic sensorimotor and incentive motivational input signals to the striatum. Behav. Brain Res. 2002, 137, 65–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Bromberg-Martin, E.S.; Matsumoto, M.; Hikosaka, O. Dopamine in Motivational Control: Rewarding, Aversive, and Alerting. Neuron 2010, 68, 815–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Barceló, A.C.; Filippini, B.; Pazo, J.H. The Striatum and Pain Modulation. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2011, 32, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Hagelberg, N.; Martikainen, I.K.; Mansikka, H.; Hinkka, S.; Någren, K.; Hietala, J.; Scheinin, H.; Pertovaara, A. Dopamine D2 receptor binding in the human brain is associated with the response to painful stimulation and pain modulatory capacity. Pain 2002, 99, 273–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Cobacho, N.; de la Calle, J.L.; Paíno, C.L. Dopaminergic modulation of neuropathic pain: Analgesia in rats by a D2-type receptor agonist. Brain Res. Bull. 2014, 106, 62–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Martikainen, I.K.; Hagelberg, N.; Mansikka, H.; Hietala, J.; Någren, K.; Scheinin, H.; Pertovaara, A. Association of striatal dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding potential with pain but not tactile sensitivity or placebo analgesia. Neurosci. Lett. 2005, 376, 149–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Clark, J.A.; Brown, C.A.; Jones, A.K.P.; El-Deredy, W. Dissociating nociceptive modulation by the duration of pain antic-ipation from unpredictability in the timing of pain. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2008, 119, 2870–2878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Taylor, A.G.; Goehler, L.E.; Galper, D.I.; Innes, K.E.; Bourguignon, C. Top-Down and Bottom-Up Mechanisms in Mind-Body Medicine: Development of an Integrative Framework for Psychophysiological Research. Explore 2010, 6, 29–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Forkmann, K.; Grashorn, W.; Schmidt, K.; Fründt, O.; Buhmann, C.; Bingel, U. Altered neural responses to heat pain in drug-naive patients with Parkinson disease. Pain 2017, 158, 1408–1416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Wang, J.-Y.; Zhang, H.-T.; Chang, J.-Y.; Woodward, D.J.; Baccalá, L.A.; Luo, F. Anticipation of Pain Enhances the Nociceptive Transmission and Functional Connectivity within Pain Network in Rats. Mol. Pain 2008, 4, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Brown, C.A.; Seymour, B.; Boyle, Y.; El-Deredy, W.; Jones, A.K.P.P. Modulation of pain ratings by expectation and uncertainty: Behavioral characteristics and anticipatory neural correlates. Pain 2008, 135, 240–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Atlas, L.Y.; Bolger, N.; Lindquist, M.A.; Wager, T.D. Brain Mediators of Predictive Cue Effects on Perceived Pain. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 12964–12977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Brown, C.A.; El-Deredy, W.; Jones, A.K.P. When the brain expects pain: Common neural responses to pain anticipation are related to clinical pain and distress in fibromyalgia and osteoarthritis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2013, 39, 663–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Lobanov, O.V.; Zeidan, F.; Mc, H.J.G.; Kraft, R.A.; Coghill, R.C. From cue to meaning: Brain mechanisms supporting the construction of expectations of pain. Pain 2014, 155, 129–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Atlas, L.Y.; Wager, T.D. How expectations shape pain. Neurosci. Lett. 2012, 520, 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Derbyshire, S.W. Exploring the ‘Pain Neuromatrix’. Curr. Rev. Pain 2000, 4, 467–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Ott, T.; Nieder, A. Dopamine and Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019, 23, 213–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Suhara, T.; Okubo, Y.; Yasuno, F.; Sudo, Y.; Inoue, M.; Ichimiya, T.; Nakashima, Y.; Nakayama, K.; Tanada, S.; Suzuki, K.; et al. Decreased Dopamine D2 Receptor Binding in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2002, 59, 25–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Wang, S.; Hu, S.-H.; Shi, Y.; Li, B.-M. The roles of the anterior cingulate cortex and its dopamine receptors in self-paced cost–benefit decision making in rats. Anim. Learn. Behav. 2016, 45, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Suhara, T.; Yasuno, F.; Sudo, Y.; Yamamoto, M.; Inoue, M.; Okubo, Y.; Suzuki, K. Dopamine D2 Receptors in the Insular Cortex and the Personality Trait of Novelty Seeking. NeuroImage 2001, 13, 891–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Govindaiah, G.; Wang, Y.; Cox, C. Dopamine enhances the excitability of somatosensory thalamocortical neurons. Neuroscience 2010, 170, 981–991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Fariello, R.G.; Fariello, R. Pharmacodynamic and Pharmacokinetic Features of Cabergoline. Drugs 1998, 55, 10–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. CABERGOLINE. Available online: https://drugs.ncats.io/drug/LL60K9J05T (accessed on 21 February 2022).
  64. AMISULPRIDE. Available online: https://drugs.ncats.io/drug/8110R61I4U (accessed on 21 February 2022).
  65. Möller, H.-J. Amisulpride: Limbic specificity and the mechanism of antipsychotic atypicality. Prog. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2003, 27, 1101–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Schoemaker, H.; Claustre, Y.; Fage, D.; Rouquier, L.; Chergui, K.; Curet, O.; Oblin, A.; Gonon, F.; Carter, C.; Benavides, J.; et al. Neurochemical characteristics of amisulpride, an atypical dopamine D2/D3 receptor antagonist with both presynaptic and limbic selectivity. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1997, 280, 83–97. [Google Scholar]
  67. Abbas, A.I.; Hedlund, P.B.; Huang, X.-P.; Tran, T.B.; Meltzer, H.Y.; Roth, B.L. Amisulpride is a potent 5-HT7 antagonist: Relevance for antidepressant actions in vivo. Psychopharmacology 2009, 205, 119–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Liu, Z.-Y.; Zhuang, D.-B.; Lunderberg, T.; Yu, L.-C. Involvement of 5-hydroxytryptamine1A receptors in the descending anti-nociceptive pathway from periaqueductal gray to the spinal dorsal horn in intact rats, rats with nerve injury and rats with inflammation. Neuroscience 2002, 112, 399–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Cortes-Altamirano, J.L.; Olmos-Hernández, A.; Jaime, H.B.; Carrillo-Mora, P.; Bandala, C.; Reyes-Long, S.; Alfaro-Rodríguez, A. Review: 5-HT1, 5-HT2, 5-HT3 and 5-HT7 Receptors and their Role in the Modulation of Pain Response in the Central Nervous System. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 2018, 16, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Viguier, F.; Michot, B.; Hamon, M.; Bourgoin, S. Multiple roles of serotonin in pain control mechanisms—Implications of 5-HT7 and other 5-HT receptor types. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2013, 716, 8–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Amaya-Castellanos, E.; Pineda-Farias, J.B.; Castañeda-Corral, G.; Vidal-Cantú, G.C.; Murbartián, J.; Rocha-González, H.I.; Granados-Soto, V. Blockade of 5-HT7 receptors reduces tactile allodynia in the rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2011, 99, 591–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Viguier, F.; Michot, B.; Kayser, V.; Bernard, J.-F.; Vela, J.-M.; Hamon, M.; Bourgoin, S. GABA, but not opioids, mediates the anti-hyperalgesic effects of 5-HT7 receptor activation in rats suffering from neuropathic pain. Neuropharmacology 2012, 63, 1093–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Maruya, H.; Watanabe, Y.; Okita, M.; Lawlor, G.F.; Utsumi, H.; Niitsuma, T. Inhibitory effects of d2 agonists by striatal injection on excessive release of dopamine and hyperactivity induced by bay k 8644 in rats. Neuroscience 2003, 118, 1091–1098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Ford, C. The role of D2-autoreceptors in regulating dopamine neuron activity and transmission. Neuroscience 2014, 282, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Frank, M.J.; O’Reilly, R.C. A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: Psychopharmaco-logical studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. Behav. Neurosci. 2006, 120, 497–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Nandam, L.S.; Hester, R.; Wagner, J.; Dean, A.J.; Messer, C.; Honeysett, A.; Nathan, P.J.; Bellgrove, M.A. Dopamine D2 Receptor Modulation of Human Response Inhibition and Error Awareness. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013, 25, 649–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  77. Chung, Y.-C.; Park, T.-W.; Yang, J.-C.; Huang, G.-B.; Zhao, T.; Oh, K.-Y.; Kim, M.-G. Cognitive Effects of a Single Dose of Atypical Antipsychotics in Healthy Volunteers Compared with Placebo or Haloperidol. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2012, 32, 778–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Dostinex—FDA Prescribing Information, Side Effects and Uses. Available online: https://www.drugs.com/pro/dostinex.html (accessed on 6 September 2019).
  79. Benfield, A.C.S. Amisulpride: A Review of its Pharmacodynamic and Pharmacokinetic Properties and Therapeutic Efficacy in the Management of Schizophrenia. Adis Int. 1994, 6, 3. [Google Scholar]
  80. Patton, J.H.; Stanford, M.S.; Barratt, E.S. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995, 51, 768–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Bucker, B.; Theeuwes, J. The effect of reward on orienting and reorienting in exogenous cuing. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 14, 635–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Costa, A.; la Fougère, C.; Pogarell, O.; Möller, H.-J.; Riedel, M.; Ettinger, U. Impulsivity is related to striatal dopamine transporter availability in healthy males. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2013, 211, 251–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Buckholtz, J.W.; Treadway, M.T.; Cowan, R.L.; Woodward, N.D.; Li, R.; Ansari, M.S.; Baldwin, R.M.; Schwartzman, A.N.; Shelby, E.S.; Smith, C.E.; et al. Dopaminergic Network Differences in Human Impulsivity. Science 2010, 329, 532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Dalley, J.W.; Robbins, T. Fractionating impulsivity: Neuropsychiatric implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 158–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Stanford, M.S.; Mathias, C.W.; Dougherty, D.M.; Lake, S.L.; Anderson, N.E.; Patton, J.H. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: An update and review. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2009, 47, 385–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Karson, C.N. Spontaneous Eye-Blink Rates and Dopaminergic Systems. Brain 1983, 106, 643–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Elsworth, J.D.; Lawrence, M.S.; Roth, R.H.; Taylor, J.R.; Mailman, R.; Nichols, D.E.; Lewis, M.H.; Redmond, D.E. D1 and D2 dopamine receptors independently regulate spontaneous blink rate in the vervet monkey. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1991, 259, 595–600. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  88. Lewis, T.; Pochin, E.E. The double pain response of the human skin to a single stimulus. Clin. Sci. 1937, 3, 50. [Google Scholar]
  89. Bromm, B.; Treede, R.-D. Nerve fibre discharges, cerebral potentials and sensations induced by CO2 laser stimulation. Hum. Neurobiol. 1984, 3, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  90. Brown, C.A.; Jones, A.K. A role for midcingulate cortex in the interruptive effects of pain anticipation on attention. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2008, 119, 2370–2379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Brown, C.A.; Jones, A.K. Meditation experience predicts less negative appraisal of pain: Electrophysiological evidence for the involvement of anticipatory neural responses. Pain 2010, 150, 428–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Martin, S.; Jones, A.K.P.; Brown, C.A.; Kobylecki, C.; Silverdale, M.A.; Sarah, M.L.; Anthony, J.K.P.; Christopher, B.A.; Christopher, K.; Monty, S.A. A neurophysiological investigation of anticipation to pain in Parkinson’s disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2020, 51, 611–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An Open Source Toolbox for Analysis of Single-Trial EEG Dynamics Including Independent Component Analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2004, 134, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Nielsen, F.Å. The Brede database: A small database for functional neuroimaging. NeuroImage 2003, 19, 19–22. [Google Scholar]
  95. Karson, C.N.; Burns, R.S.; Le Witt, P.A.; Foster, N.L.; Newman, R.P. Blink rates and disorders of movement. Neurology 1984, 34, 677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Shukla, D. Blink rate as clinical indicator. Neurology 1985, 35, 286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Taylor, J.; Elsworth, J.; Lawrence, M.; Sladek, J.; Roth, R.; Redmond, D. Spontaneous Blink Rates Correlate with Dopamine Levels in the Caudate Nucleus of MPTP-Treated Monkeys. Exp. Neurol. 1999, 158, 214–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Kleven, M.S.; Koek, W. Differential effects of direct and indirect dopamine agonists on eye blink rate in cynomolgus monkeys. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1996, 279, 1211–1219. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  99. Cousineau, D. Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutor. Quant. Methods Psychol. 2005, 1, 42–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Sescousse, G.; Ligneul, R.; van Holst, R.J.; Janssen, L.K.; de Boer, F.; Janssen, M.; Berry, A.S.; Jagust, W.J.; Cools, R. Spontaneous eye blink rate and dopamine synthesis capacity: Preliminary evidence for an absence of positive correlation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2018, 47, 1081–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Dang, L.C.; Samanez-Larkin, G.R.; Castrellon, J.J.; Perkins, S.F.; Cowan, R.L.; Newhouse, P.A.; Zald, D.H. Spontaneous Eye Blink Rate (EBR) Is Uncorrelated with Dopamine D2 Receptor Availability and Unmodulated by Dopamine Agonism in Healthy Adults. Eneuro 2017, 4, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Yoshida, W.; Seymour, B.; Koltzenburg, M.; Dolan, R.J. Uncertainty increases pain: Evidence for a novel mechanism of pain modulation involving the periaqueductal gray. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 5638–5646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Andreou, C.; Bozikas, V.P.; Luedtke, T.; Moritz, S. Associations between visual perception accuracy and confidence in a dopaminergic manipulation study. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Schwartenbeck, P.; Fitz, G.T.; Mathys, C.; Dolan, R.; Friston, K. The Dopaminergic Midbrain Encodes the Expected Certainty about Desired Outcomes. Cereb. Cortex 2015, 25, 3434–3445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Tomassini, A.; Ruge, D.; Galea, J.M.; Penny, W.; Bestmann, S. The Role of Dopamine in Temporal Uncertainty. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 28, 96–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Kwon, M.; Altin, M.; Duenas, H.; Alev, L. The Role of Descending Inhibitory Pathways on Chronic Pain Modulation and Clinical Implications. Pain Pract. 2014, 14, 656–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Millan, M.J. Descending control of pain. Prog. Neurobiol. 2002, 66, 355–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Ossipov, M.H.; Morimura, K.; Porreca, F. Descending pain modulation and chronification of pain. Curr. Opin. Support. Palliat. Care 2014, 8, 143–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  109. Martin, S.L.; Power, A.; Boyle, Y.; Anderson, I.M.; Silverdale, M.A.; Jones, A.K.P. 5-HT modulation of pain perception in humans. Psychopharmacology 2017, 234, 2929–2939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Berger, B.; Trottier, S.; Verney, C.; Gaspar, P.; Alvarez, C. Regional and laminar distribution of the dopamine and serotonin innervation in the macaque cerebral cortex: A radioautographic study. J. Comp. Neurol. 1988, 273, 99–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Bergson, C.; Mrzljak, L.; Smiley, J.F.; Pappy, M.; Levenson, R.; Goldman-Rakic, P.S. Regional, cellular, and subcellular vari-ations in the distribution of D1 and D5 dopamine receptors in primate brain. J. Neurosci. 1995, 15, 7821–7836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  112. Goldsmith, S.; Joyce, J. Dopamine D2 receptors are organized in bands in normal human temporal cortex. Neuroscience 1996, 74, 435–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Herath, P.; Kinomura, S.; Roland, P.E. Visual recognition: Evidence for two distinctive mechanisms from a PET study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2001, 12, 110–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Ishai, A.; Ungerleider, L.G.; Martin, A.; Schouten, J.L.; Haxby, J.V. Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1999, 96, 9379–9384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  115. Chao, L.L.; Haxby, J.V.; Martin, A. Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat. Neurosci. 1999, 2, 913–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Tranel, D.; Damasio, H.; Damasio, A.R. A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychology 1997, 35, 1319–1327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Kluger, D.S.; Schubotz, R.I. Strategic adaptation to non-reward prediction error qualities and irreducible uncertainty in f, MRI. Cortex 2017, 97, 32–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Seghier, M.L. The angular gyrus: Multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscientist 2013, 19, 43–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  119. Seghier, M.L.; Fagan, E.; Price, C. Functional Subdivisions in the Left Angular Gyrus Where the Semantic System Meets and Diverges from the Default Network. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 16809–16817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Kincade, J.M.; Abrams, R.A.; Astafiev, S.V.; Shulman, G.L.; Corbetta, M. An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study of voluntary and stimulus-driven orienting of attention. J. Neurosci. 2005, 25, 4593–4604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Rushworth, M.F.S.; Ellison, A.; Walsh, V. Complementary localization and lateralization of orienting and motor attention. Nat. Neurosci. 2001, 4, 656–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Gottlieb, J. From Thought to Action: The Parietal Cortex as a Bridge between Perception, Action, and Cognition. Neuron 2007, 53, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  123. Watson, A.; El-Deredy, W.; Iannetti, G.D.; Lloyd, D.; Tracey, I.; Vogt, B.A.; Nadeau, V.; Jones, A.K. Placebo conditioning and placebo analgesia modulate a common brain network during pain anticipation and perception. Pain 2009, 145, 24–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  124. Peyron, R.; García-Larrea, L.; Grégoire, M.-C.; Costes, N.; Convers, P.; Lavenne, F.; Mauguière, F.; Michel, D.; Laurent, B. Haemodynamic brain responses to acute pain in humans. Brain 1999, 122, 1765–1780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  125. Kulkarni, B.; Bentley, D.E.; Elliott, R.; Youell, P.; Watson, A.; Derbyshire, S.W.G.; Frackowiak, R.; Friston, K.; Jones, A. Attention to pain localization and unpleasantness discriminates the functions of the medial and lateral pain systems. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005, 21, 3133–3142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Yang, Z.; Liu, X.; Yin, Y.; Sun, S.; Deng, X. Involvement of 5-HT7 receptors in the pathogenesis of temporal lobe epilepsy. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2012, 685, 52–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Cools, R.; D’Esposito, M. Inverted-U–Shaped Dopamine Actions on Human Working Memory and Cognitive Control. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, e113–e125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  128. Vijay, R.S.; Wang, M.; Birnbaum, S.G.; Williams, G.V.; Arnsten, A.F.T. Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2007, 10, 376–384. [Google Scholar]
  129. Levy, F. Dopamine vs Noradrenaline: Inverted-U Effects and ADHD Theories. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2009, 43, 101–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Floresco, S.B. Prefrontal dopamine and behavioral flexibility: Shifting from an “inverted-U” toward a family of functions. Front. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  131. Beggs, J.M.; Plenz, D. Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 11167–11177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  132. Finke, K.; Dodds, C.M.; Bublak, P.; Regenthal, R.; Baumann, F.; Manly, T.; Müller, U. Effects of modafinil and methylphenidate on visual attention capacity: A TVA-based study. Psychopharmacology 2010, 210, 317–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Li, S.-C.; Lindenberger, U.; Bäckman, L. Dopaminergic modulation of cognition across the life span. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 34, 625–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Goto, Y.; Otani, S.; Grace, A. The Yin and Yang of dopamine release: A new perspective. Neuropharmacology 2007, 53, 583–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  135. Friston, K.J.; Shiner, T.; Fitz, G.T.; Galea, J.M.; Adams, R.; Brown, H.; Dolan, R.; Moran, R.; Stephan, K.E.; Bestmann, S. Dopamine, Affordance and Active Inference. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2012, 8, e1002327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Derbyshire, S.W.; Jones, A.; Gyulai, F.; Clark, S.; Townsend, D.; Firestone, L.L. Pain processing during three levels of noxious stimulation produces differential patterns of central activity. Pain 1997, 73, 431–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Mazzola, V.; Latorre, V.; Petito, A.; Gentili, N.; Fazio, L.; Popolizio, T.; Blasi, G.; Arciero, G.; Bondolfi, G. Affective response to a loved one’s pain: Insula activity as a function of individual differences. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e15268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Lu, C.; Yang, T.; Zhao, H.; Zhang, M.; Meng, F.; Fu, H.; Xie, Y.; Xu, H. Insular Cortex is Critical for the Perception, Modulation, and Chronification of Pain. Neurosci. Bull. 2016, 32, 191–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  139. Singer, T.; Seymour, B.; O’Doherty, J.; Kaube, H.; Dolan, R.J.; Frith, C.D. Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but not Sensory Components of Pain. Science 2004, 303, 1157–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  140. Valdés-Baizabal, C.; Carbajal, G.V.; Pérez-González, D.; Malmierca, M.S. Dopamine modulates subcortical responses to surprising sounds. PLoS Biol. 2020, 18, e3000744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  141. Becker, S.; Čeko, M.; Louis-Foster, M.; Elfassy, N.M.; Leyton, M.; Shir, Y.; Schweinhardt, P. Dopamine and Pain Sensitivity: Neither Sulpiride nor Acute Phenylalanine and Tyrosine Depletion Have Effects on Thermal Pain Sensations in Healthy Volunteers. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e80766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. A schematic diagram of a single trial of the experimental paradigm. A computer monitor showed the participant a visual cue of “Low”, “High” or “Unknown” from −3 s to +2 s. A 3-second countdown of auditory cues at −3, −2 and −1 allowed accurate anticipation of the laser stimuli at time 0s (red bar). The presentation of the visual cue at −3 s was concurrent with the first auditory cue. At +2 s, an 11-point numerical rating scale (NRS) was presented for the participant to rate the laser stimulus.
Figure 1. A schematic diagram of a single trial of the experimental paradigm. A computer monitor showed the participant a visual cue of “Low”, “High” or “Unknown” from −3 s to +2 s. A 3-second countdown of auditory cues at −3, −2 and −1 allowed accurate anticipation of the laser stimuli at time 0s (red bar). The presentation of the visual cue at −3 s was concurrent with the first auditory cue. At +2 s, an 11-point numerical rating scale (NRS) was presented for the participant to rate the laser stimulus.
Brainsci 12 00351 g001
Figure 2. The eye-blink rate (EBR) calculated for each dopamine manipulation condition produced a significant linear relationship between striatal dopamine activity and EBR (p < 0.05). The D2 receptor agonist increased the EBR and the antagonist reduced the EBR in comparison to the control condition. Data are presented as mean and 95% confidence intervals (cf. [100]).
Figure 2. The eye-blink rate (EBR) calculated for each dopamine manipulation condition produced a significant linear relationship between striatal dopamine activity and EBR (p < 0.05). The D2 receptor agonist increased the EBR and the antagonist reduced the EBR in comparison to the control condition. Data are presented as mean and 95% confidence intervals (cf. [100]).
Brainsci 12 00351 g002
Figure 3. Source estimates during mid-anticipation for Control versus agonist (Cabergoline) and antagonist (Amisulpride). The time window of mid anticipation [−1500–1000 ms] was baseline corrected to [−2500–2000 ms]. Clusters are shown at FWE correction and regions labelled using the AAL2 atlas. The agonist (blue) reduced activity within the right mid-temporal and angular regions in contrast to the control. The antagonist (pink) reduced activity within the right postcentral, mid-temporal, and inferior parietal regions. The xyz position of the slices are aligned with the peak-voxel at peak-level inference and reported in mm according to the MNI atlas. In comparison to the control, the agonist reduced activity within the right mid temporal and angular regions, and the antagonist reduced activity within the right postcentral, mid temporal, and inferior parietal region.
Figure 3. Source estimates during mid-anticipation for Control versus agonist (Cabergoline) and antagonist (Amisulpride). The time window of mid anticipation [−1500–1000 ms] was baseline corrected to [−2500–2000 ms]. Clusters are shown at FWE correction and regions labelled using the AAL2 atlas. The agonist (blue) reduced activity within the right mid-temporal and angular regions in contrast to the control. The antagonist (pink) reduced activity within the right postcentral, mid-temporal, and inferior parietal regions. The xyz position of the slices are aligned with the peak-voxel at peak-level inference and reported in mm according to the MNI atlas. In comparison to the control, the agonist reduced activity within the right mid temporal and angular regions, and the antagonist reduced activity within the right postcentral, mid temporal, and inferior parietal region.
Brainsci 12 00351 g003
Figure 4. The drug effect reported using source localisation for post-stimulus TWOI [200 600 ms]. The TWOI was baseline corrected to −500 ms prior to the noxious stimuli. There was a significant difference between the control and antagonist conditions, showing a lower degree of activity within the right insula in the antagonist condition. The antagonist condition also reported a lower activity in comparison to the agonist within the right hippocampus, mid/inferior temporal lobe, insula, and Heschl’s gyrus.
Figure 4. The drug effect reported using source localisation for post-stimulus TWOI [200 600 ms]. The TWOI was baseline corrected to −500 ms prior to the noxious stimuli. There was a significant difference between the control and antagonist conditions, showing a lower degree of activity within the right insula in the antagonist condition. The antagonist condition also reported a lower activity in comparison to the agonist within the right hippocampus, mid/inferior temporal lobe, insula, and Heschl’s gyrus.
Brainsci 12 00351 g004
Table 1. Pain rating descriptive statistics. The ratings are separated into cue type and drug condition. Ratings are reported as mean and standard deviation.
Table 1. Pain rating descriptive statistics. The ratings are separated into cue type and drug condition. Ratings are reported as mean and standard deviation.
Pain Rating (NRS Score/10)
LowHighUncertain LowUncertain High
Drug ConditionMeanSDMeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Control2.970.886.300.713.281.075.860.62
Agonist3.050.936.280.793.501.145.750.80
Antagonist2.831.006.360.603.311.105.580.68
NB: NRS: Numerical rating scale, SD: Standard deviation.
Table 2. Unpleasantness rating descriptive statistics. The ratings are separated into cue type and drug condition. Ratings are reported as mean and standard deviation.
Table 2. Unpleasantness rating descriptive statistics. The ratings are separated into cue type and drug condition. Ratings are reported as mean and standard deviation.
Unpleasantness Rating (NRS Score/10)
LowHighUncertain LowUncertain High
Drug ConditionMeanSDMeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Control2.391.306.490.912.561.447.081.30
Agonist2.251.076.490.672.351.026.980.90
Antagonist1.941.225.422.372.221.555.912.67
NB: NRS: Numerical rating scale, SD: Standard deviation.
Table 3. Significant results of the source localisation analysis within mid-anticipation and the post-stimulus TWOI. The mid-anticipation TWOI is baseline-corrected to the pre-auditory cue time window. The significant clusters were restricted to >100 voxels, reported at FWE correction with a threshold of p < 0.025 to account for multiple comparisons. Results are divided into main F-contrast and post-hoc T-contrasts. The MNI coordinates are reported for the peak voxel within the cluster. The brain regions are labelled using the Anatomical Automatic Labelling (AAL2) atlas, with the percentage overlap of the significant cluster with the brain region reported. The region with the highest percentage overlap is shown, unless an equivalent share of percentage overlap was observed. Brain regions labelled by the atlas as ‘Unknown’ are not shown.
Table 3. Significant results of the source localisation analysis within mid-anticipation and the post-stimulus TWOI. The mid-anticipation TWOI is baseline-corrected to the pre-auditory cue time window. The significant clusters were restricted to >100 voxels, reported at FWE correction with a threshold of p < 0.025 to account for multiple comparisons. Results are divided into main F-contrast and post-hoc T-contrasts. The MNI coordinates are reported for the peak voxel within the cluster. The brain regions are labelled using the Anatomical Automatic Labelling (AAL2) atlas, with the percentage overlap of the significant cluster with the brain region reported. The region with the highest percentage overlap is shown, unless an equivalent share of percentage overlap was observed. Brain regions labelled by the atlas as ‘Unknown’ are not shown.
Cluster-LevelPeak-LevelMNI CoordinatesBrain Region
p(FWE)KF/TZXYZ
Mid AnticipationF-Contrast
Drug0.00490911.624.2032−4434RInferior Parietal (48.0%)
0.0137009.793.7934−5414RMid Temporal (56.3%)
Expectation0.000409825.904.85−44−486L
L
Mid Temporal (35.3%)
Mid Occipital (23.1%)
0.01378318.644.09−562−16LMid Temporal (69.1%)
0.00793115.283.69−14−862L
L
Calcarine (52.8%)
Lingual (37.2%)
0.00539015.653.73−30−188LInsula (60.7%)
T-Contrast
Drug
Control > Agonist
0.0316933.923.8752−5810R
R
Mid Temporal (49.9%)
Angular gyrus (29.4%)
Drug
Control > Antag
0.00041844.604.5234−4040R
R
R
Postcentral (17.2%)
Mid Temporal (16.9%)
Inferior Parietal (15.0%)
Expectation
High > Low
0.00085555.094.98−44−486L
L
Mid Temporal (30.4%)
Mid Occipital (17.9%)
0.0036413.963.90−30−188LInsula (58.8%)
Post-StimulusF-Contrast
Drug0.02216410.824.0228−2614RInsula (40.9%)
T-Contrast
Drug
Control > Antag
0.0331313.733.6828−2614RInsula (45.0%)
Drug
Agonist > Antag
0.022 7483.933.8830−20−16RHippocampus (54.3%)
0.0109403.803.7564−30−6R
R
Mid Temporal (52.0%)
Inferior Temporal (38.7%)
0.008 3474.144.0728−2614R
R
Insula (32.6%)
Heschl (18.9%)
NB: Small volume correction (SVC) in regions of interest was applied using a sphere with 25 mm radius. ‘>’ Symbolises that the left-sided condition showed a greater response in comparison to the right-sided condition. FWE = Family-Wise Error; K = number of voxels; F/T = F-value/T-value; Z = Z-score; Expectation = ‘Low’ vs. ‘High’ visual cues; Antag = Antagonist; R = Right hemisphere; L = Left hemisphere.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Martin, S.L.; Jones, A.K.P.; Brown, C.A.; Kobylecki, C.; Whitaker, G.A.; El-Deredy, W.; Silverdale, M.A. Altered Pain Processing Associated with Administration of Dopamine Agonist and Antagonist in Healthy Volunteers. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 351. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12030351

AMA Style

Martin SL, Jones AKP, Brown CA, Kobylecki C, Whitaker GA, El-Deredy W, Silverdale MA. Altered Pain Processing Associated with Administration of Dopamine Agonist and Antagonist in Healthy Volunteers. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(3):351. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12030351

Chicago/Turabian Style

Martin, Sarah L., Anthony K. P. Jones, Christopher A. Brown, Christopher Kobylecki, Grace A. Whitaker, Wael El-Deredy, and Monty A. Silverdale. 2022. "Altered Pain Processing Associated with Administration of Dopamine Agonist and Antagonist in Healthy Volunteers" Brain Sciences 12, no. 3: 351. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12030351

APA Style

Martin, S. L., Jones, A. K. P., Brown, C. A., Kobylecki, C., Whitaker, G. A., El-Deredy, W., & Silverdale, M. A. (2022). Altered Pain Processing Associated with Administration of Dopamine Agonist and Antagonist in Healthy Volunteers. Brain Sciences, 12(3), 351. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12030351

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop