Next Article in Journal
Short-Term Memory for Auditory Temporal Patterns and Meaningless Sentences Predicts Learning of Foreign Word Forms
Previous Article in Journal
A Circuit Model for Working Memory Based on Hybrid Positive and Negative-Derivative Feedback Mechanism
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

The Percentages of Cognitive Skills Deficits among Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

1
School of Information Science and Technology, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei 230026, China
2
Artificial Intelligence Research Institute, iFLYTEK Co., Ltd., Hefei 230088, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(5), 548; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12050548
Submission received: 25 March 2022 / Revised: 17 April 2022 / Accepted: 21 April 2022 / Published: 26 April 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Educational Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
The current study was conducted to examine the percentages of cognitive skills deficits among Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Via a systematic review, we collated twenty-two available studies on the proportion of cognitive skills deficits, including phonological awareness, rapid automatized naming, morphological awareness, orthographic knowledge, short-term memory and working memory, and visual and motor skills deficits, among Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. The results of a meta-analysis showed that the rapid automatized naming deficits are the core deficit of developmental dyslexia among Chinese children, with a pooled percentage of 44%. This is followed by orthographic knowledge deficits (43%), phonological awareness deficits (41%), morphological awareness deficits (40%), visual and motor skills deficits (33%), and short-term memory and working memory deficits (25%). At the same time, we compared the proportions of different locations, ages, standards and control groups.

1. Introduction

Developmental dyslexia, defined as a specific language-based disorder, is not attributable to a disorder of intellectual development, neurological disorder, lack of availability of education, lack of proficiency in the language of academic instruction, or psycho-social adversity. A percentage for developmental dyslexia has been reported as approximately 7% of the general population in western countries [1]. In china, about 5~8% of school-aged children have difficulties in reading Chinese [2,3].
In recent years, researchers have put forward various theories about cognitive deficits in Chinese dyslexia [4,5]. Exploring the cognitive skill deficits of Chinese dyslexia is helpful to understand the cause of children with dyslexia. In developmental dyslexia research, the identification of phonological awareness, rapid automatized naming, orthographic knowledge, morphological awareness, short-term memory and working memory, and visual and motor skills as important factors in learning to read and in the specific reading difficulties of developmental dyslexia reflects the general consensus [6]. Phonological awareness refers to the ability to detect and manipulate the sound structure of words independently of their meaning [7]. The assessment of phonological awareness usually includes rhyme awareness, syllable awareness and phonemic awareness tasks [8]. Rapid automatized naming refers to the ability to name as fast as possible highly familiar stimuli. The tasks include digits, letters, characters, objects/pictures, and colors. Orthographic knowledge refers to the ability to abstract representation of character [9]. The assessment usually includes a character decision task and partial cue-based recognition task. Morphological awareness refers to the awareness of morpheme structures and the ability to manipulate them [10]. The assessment usually includes a morpheme identification task, morphological construction test and homophone production test. Short-term memory and working memory refer to the ability to temporarily retain information. The assessment usually includes phonological memory tasks and digit span tasks. Visual skill refers to a child’s general cognitive abilities. Move skill refers to movements with muscles. The visual attention span task is one of the most common measurement tasks.
A large number of studies in western countries show that phonological processing is the core deficit of developmental dyslexia. However, there are many differences between Chinese and pinyin characters, and researchers have different views on the core deficit of Chinese dyslexia. Ho examined patterns of cognitive deficits in dyslexia and found that 29% of children in the Chinese dyslexia group had phonological deficits, 57% had rapid automatized naming deficits, 42% had orthographic skills deficits and 27% had visual and motor skills deficits [11]. Therefore, rapid automatized naming deficits may be the core deficits in Chinese dyslexia. However, the study by Chung found that 22% of children in the Chinese dyslexia group had phonological deficits, 48% had rapid automatized naming deficits, 78% had orthographic skills deficits, 67% had morphological deficits, and 52% had short-memory deficits [12]. Based on the study of Chung, orthographic skills deficits may be the core deficit. Indeed, some researchers still believe that phonological awareness is the core deficit of Chinese dyslexia. In the study by Liu, 45% had phonological deficits, 41% had rapid automatized naming deficits, 35% had orthographic skills deficits and 14% had morphological deficits [13]. The variability of percentage may also be related to other factors, such as age, control group and location [14].
It can be observed from the above research that Chinese dyslexia has multiple language deficits, but the core deficit is still controversial among researchers. Understanding the core deficits of dyslexia can lead to targeted interventions and treatments. It is also important that clinicians have reliable prevalence estimates to gain an understanding of the proportion of individuals with developmental dyslexia who may meet the criteria for cognitive deficits at a given point in time, in order to appropriately assess and plan tailored treatment to maximize recovery outcomes. The study aim was to conduct a meta-analysis to estimate the percentage of cognitive deficits for developmental dyslexia in Chinese dyslexia. We reported the percentage for the different criteria. Furthermore, the study also wanted to identify which cognitive deficit is the core deficit in Chinese dyslexia.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Search Strategy and Procedure

The articles for this meta-analysis were identified by searching the Web of Science (core collection) and CNKI. The combination of search terms applied included “reading dis* OR reading dif* OR poor read* OR developmental dyslexi*”, “individual difference OR deficit OR subtype” and “Chinese”. The titles, abstracts, keywords and full texts were screened to determine whether the inclusion criteria were met. Databases including Web of Science and CNKI were searched to identify articles from inception to September 18th, 2021. The initial search yielded 2719 articles. The protocol for the systematic review was conceived based on the PRISMA 2020 Statement (Table A1). It was submitted for registration in the PROSPERO international prospective register of systematic reviews (ID: 321448, status: waiting for approval). Two researchers (M.H. and H.L.) independently conducted a literature search. Then, a search of the reference lists of the articles included in the first step was performed to complement our database searches.

2.2. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

The articles were included if (1) the type of study was experimental, including a group of native Chinese-speaking people with DD(reading disability, reading disorder, reading difficulties, poor reading, developmental dyslexia); (2) the percentage of cognitive deficits for Chinese developmental dyslexia were reported or can be calculated; (3) published in English or Chinese; (4) the studies are not duplicated in the existing literature; (5) different literatures came from the same sample, and the results with the most comprehensive reports and up-to-date data were selected. Articles were excluded if they were conference papers, review papers or qualitative studies. In addition, unpublished papers were not included, due to the difficulty of obtaining the full text and detailed information.

2.3. Recorded Variables and Coding

2.3.1. Coding Procedure

The variables were discussed until a consensus was reached among all the authors. Then, two raters used the recorded variables to conduct the coding of all the articles. Across the total variable matrix, the mean inter-rater agreement coefficient (M.H. and H.L.) was 0.96. Any disagreements between raters were resolved by discussion with the third person (X.L.).

2.3.2. Variables

For each study, the following variables were recorded: (1) the sample characteristics; (2) the definition criteria of cognitive deficits; (3) the type of cognitive deficits and percentages of different cognitive deficits. It is important to note that different cognitive skills may be measured using different tasks in different studies. In order to minimize the impact of the tasks, when a cognitive skill involved multiple tasks for evaluation, the average percentage in the various tasks was selected. Table 1 shows a detailed explanation of the variables.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

In the study, we used Stata Statistical 15.0 software, including summary estimation, forest mapping and publication bias assessment. If the heterogeneity was low (p > 0.1, I2 ≤ 50%), the fixed effects model was selected for analysis. Otherwise, the random effects model was selected. A subgroup analysis was also performed to explore the possible sources of heterogeneity among studies. Publication bias was established based on the funnel plot and Egger test. For the meta-analysis results, p < 0.05 was considered as statistically significant.

3. Results

Figure 1 shows the main process of literature search and study selection. The search yielded 2719 records. A total of 22 articles including twenty-six studies met the study inclusion criteria and were included in this meta-analysis (Figure A1). Among these, one reported the results of sample tracking twice, so we obtained two results. One study reported results from the same sample compared to two different control groups, so we recoded the two results. In addition, one study with three different age groups recorded three results.

3.1. Study Characteristics

Twenty-two articles, including twenty-six studies, met the criteria for inclusion in this meta-analysis and are listed in Table 2. The combined sample size of dyslexia in all the studies was 1284, while the individual study sample of dyslexia ranged from 15 to 223 participants. The studies were all conducted in China, including Hong Kong, from 2002 to 2021, and all the participants speak Chinese as a first language. Eighteen of the studies reported the percentages of phonological awareness deficits, sixteen reported the percentages of rapid automatized naming deficits, eleven reported the percentages of orthographic knowledge deficits, ten reported the percentages of morphological awareness deficits, ten reported the percentages of short-memory deficits and nine reported the percentages of visual and motor skills deficits.
Publication bias was established based on the funnel plot (Figure A2) and Egger test (t = 0.81, p = 0.432, for phonological awareness; t = 0.73, p = 0.478, for rapid automatized naming; t = 0.30, p = 0.769 for orthographic knowledge; t = −0.7, p = 0.502, for morphological awareness; t = 1.11, p = 0.291, for short-term memory and working memory; t = 0.65, p = 0.531, for visual and motor skills).

3.2. Pooled Percentage

We used I2 to test the heterogeneity between studies. If the heterogeneity was low (p > 0.1, I2 ≤ 50%), the fixed-effect model was selected to estimate pooled percentage, otherwise, the random effect model was selected.

3.2.1. Phonological Awareness

Table 3 showed the results of overall and subgroup meta-analysis about phonological awareness deficits. The percentages of phonological awareness deficits range from 9% to 76%, with a pooled percentage of 41% (95% CI: 31–52%).
For age, we divided the sample into two groups, with a cut-off age of 11. For the age group of children younger than 11 years old, 11 studies reported a pooled percentage of 41% (95% CI: 27–55%). The remaining six studies reported a pooled percentage of 38% (95% CI: 19–56%). For the type of areas, 12 studies reported ae pooled percentage of 49% (95% CI: 42–56%), with the sample from Mainland China. In addition, six studies reported a pooled percentage of 26% (95% CI: 5–47%), with the sample from Hong Kong, China. For the type of control group, fourteen studies used age-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have phonological awareness deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 37% (95% CI: 26–47%). Two studies used reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have phonological awareness deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 49% (95% CI: 38–59%). In addition, two studies used a cluster analysis and reported a pooled percentage of 64% (95% CI: 41–87%). For the criterion of deficits, ten studies used the cut-off criteria of 1.5 standard deviations below the mean on phonological awareness deficits screening and reported a pooled percentage of 36% (95% CI: 24–48%). Three studies used the cut-off criteria of 1 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 31% (95% CI: 5–57%). Two studies used the cluster method and reported a pooled percentage of 64% (95% CI: 41–87%). A study used the criteria of 1.65 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 47% (95% CI: 21–72%), a study used the criteria of 2 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 40% (95% CI: 21–59%), and a study used the cut-off criteria of the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 73% (95% CI: 51–96%).

3.2.2. Rapid Automatized Naming

Table 4 showed the results of overall and subgroup meta-analysis about rapid automatized naming deficits. The percentages of rapid automatized naming deficits range from 17% to 66%, with a pooled percentage of 44% (95% CI: 37–51%).
In the age group of children younger than 11 years old, nine studies reported a pooled percentage of 46% (95% CI: 36–56%). The remaining seven studies reported a pooled percentage of 42% (95% CI: 32–53%). For the type of areas, nine studies reported a pooled percentage of 36% (95% CI: 29–43%), with the sample from Mainland China. In addition, seven studies reported a pooled percentage of 56% (95% CI: 51–61%), with the sample from Hong Kong, China. For the type of control group, 13 studies used age-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have rapid automatized naming deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 48% (95% CI: 41–54%). Two studies used reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have rapid automatized naming deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 28% (95% CI: 5–52%). In addition, one study used a cluster analysis and reported a pooled percentage of 37% (95% CI: 31–44%). For the criterion of deficits, eleven studies used the cut-off criteria of 1.5 standard deviations below the mean on rapid automatized naming deficits screening and reported a pooled percentage of 44% (95% CI: 34–54%). Four studies used the cut-off criteria of 1 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of48% (95% CI: 35–61%). A study used the cluster method and reported a pooled percentage of 37% (95% CI: 31–44%).

3.2.3. Orthographic Knowledge

Table 5 showed the results of overall and subgroup meta-analysis about orthographic knowledge deficits. The percentages of orthographic knowledge deficits range from 27% to 64%, with a pooled percentage of 43% (95% CI: 36–50%).
In the age group of children younger than 11 years old, eight studies reported a pooled percentage of 40% (95% CI: 32–49%). The remaining three studies reported a pooled percentage of 51% (95% CI: 41–62%). For the type of areas, four studies reported a pooled percentage of 46% (95% CI: 29–64%), with the sample from Mainland China. In addition, seven studies reported a pooled percentage of 41% (95% CI: 34–49%), with the sample from Hong Kong, China. For the type of control group, eight studies used age-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have orthographic knowledge deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 43 % (95% CI: 35–51%). Two studies used reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have orthographic knowledge deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 31 % (95% CI: 22–41%). Furthermore, one study used a cluster analysis and reported a pooled percentage of 57 % (95% CI: 47–68%). For the criterion of deficits, seven studies used the cut-off criteria of 1.5 standard deviations below the mean on orthographic knowledge deficits screening and reported a pooled percentage of 43% (95% CI: 35–52%). Three studies used the cut-off criteria of 1 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 33 % (95% CI: 24–42%). A study used the cluster method and reported a pooled percentage of 57 % (95% CI: 47–68%).

3.2.4. Morphological Awareness

Table 6 showed the results of overall and subgroup meta-analysis about morphological awareness deficits. The percentages of morphological awareness deficits range from 12% to 76%, with a pooled percentage of 40% (95% CI: 24–55%). The study with a percentage of 100% was excluded from the actual meta-analysis.
In the age group of children younger than 11 years old, four studies reported a pooled percentage of 24% (95% CI: 0–48%). The remaining six studies reported a pooled percentage of 51% (95% CI: 35–67%). For the type of areas, the seven studies reported a pooled percentage of 37% (95% CI: 18–57%), with the sample from Mainland China. In addition, three studies reported a pooled percentage of 47% (95% CI: 23–71%), with the sample from Hong Kong, China. For the type of control group, seven studies used age-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have morphological awareness deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 46% (95% CI: 28–64%). Two studies used reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have morphological awareness deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 13% (95% CI: 6–20%). Furthermore, one study used a cluster analysis and reported a pooled percentage of 53% (95% CI: 47–60%). For the criterion of deficits, five studies used the cut-off criteria of 1.5 standard deviations below the mean on morphological awareness deficits screening and reported a pooled percentage of 44% (95% CI: 16–73%). Three studies used the cut-off criteria of 1 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 35% (95% CI: 11–60%). A study used the cut-off criteria of 2 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 16% (95% CI: 2–30%). A study used the cluster method and reported a pooled percentage of 53% (95% CI: 47–60%).

3.2.5. Short-Term Memory and Working Memory

Table 7 showed the results of overall and subgroup meta-analysis about short-term memory and working memory deficits. The percentages of short-term memory and working memory deficits range from 10% to 52%, with a pooled percentage of 25% (95% CI: 18–31%).
In the age group of children younger than 11 years old, six studies reported a pooled percentage of 23% (95% CI: 13–33%). The remaining six studies reported a pooled percentage of 26% (95% CI: 17–36%). For the type of areas, five studies reported a pooled percentage of 28% (95% CI: 13–43%), with the sample from Mainland China. In addition, seven studies reported a pooled percentage of 23% (95% CI: 16–30%), with the sample from Hong Kong, China. For the type of control group, all twelve studies used age-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have short-term memory and working memory deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 25% (95% CI: 18–31%). For the criterion of deficits, eight studies used the cut-off criteria of 1.5 standard deviations below the mean on short-term memory and working memory deficits screening and reported a pooled percentage of 26% (95% CI: 16–35%). Three studies used the cut-off criteria of 1 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 25% (95% CI: 17–33%). A study used the cut-off criteria of 2 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 16% (95% CI: 2–30%).

3.2.6. Visual and Motor Skills

Table 8 showed the results of overall and subgroup meta-analysis about visual and motor skills deficits. The percentages of visual and motor skills deficits range from 5% to 65%, with a pooled percentage of 33% (95% CI: 20–46%).
In the age group of children younger than 11 years old, eight studies reported a pooled percentage of 31% (95% CI: 16–45%). The remaining one study reported a pooled percentage of 39% (95% CI: 16–61%). For the type of areas, five studies reported a pooled percentage of 35% (95% CI: 12–58%), with the sample from Mainland China. In addition, four studies reported a pooled percentage of 29% (95% CI: 23–35%), with the sample from Hong Kong, China. For the type of control group, eight studies used age-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have deficits on visual and motor skills and reported a pooled percentage of 29% (95% CI: 18–40%). One study used reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls to confirm whether children with DD have visual and motor skills deficits and reported a pooled percentage of 24% (95% CI: 7–41%). In addition, one study used a cluster analysis and reported a pooled percentage of 65% (95% CI: 54–76%). For the criterion of deficits, five studies used the cut-off criteria of 1.5 standard deviations below the mean on visual and motor skills deficits screening and reported a pooled percentage of 31% (95% CI: 25–36%). Three studies used the cut-off criteria of 1.65 standard deviations below the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 15% (95% CI: −1–31%). A study used the cut-off criteria of the mean and reported a pooled percentage of 53 % (95% CI: 28–79%), and a study used the cluster method and reported a pooled percentage of 65 % (95% CI: 54–76%).

4. Discussion

After conducting a meta-analysis of all the available studies that adhered to our inclusion criteria (22 articles), we calculated the pooled percentages under different categories.

4.1. Pooled Percentage

We found that the rapid automatized naming deficits are the core deficit of Chinese developmental dyslexia, with a pooled percentage of 44% through meta-analysis. This is followed by orthographic knowledge deficits (43%), phonological awareness deficits (41%), morphological awareness deficits (40%), visual and motor skills deficit (33%), and short-term memory and working memory deficits (25%).
It can be observed from the results that the incidence of rapid automatized naming deficits and orthographic knowledge deficits is relatively high in Chinese dyslexia. In a recent meta-analysis on the deficit profiles of Chinese children with reading difficulties, Peng et al. found that rapid automatized naming deficits and orthographic knowledge deficits may have a greater impact on developmental dyslexia than on any other skill deficits [6]. This is similar to our results. Many studies have shown that rapid automatized naming has a strong predictive effect on developmental dyslexia and can effectively identify developmental dyslexia [11,36]. Rote learning is usually the main method of learning Chinese character, and it is a way of learning that may have led to rapid automatized naming skills as the basis of Chinse character acquisition [37]. Based on Wolf’s idea, rapid automatized naming tasks are complex and involve cognitive perceptual and linguistic processes [38]. Therefore, children with rapid automatized naming deficits may also have deficits in orthographic knowledge deficits. In addition, according to previous studies, the orthographic knowledge of Chinese reading involves determining the pronunciation of Chinese characters according to the phonetic element radicals, obtaining the semantics based on radicals and grasping the overall structure of Chinese characters. For children with Chinese developmental dyslexia, it takes more time and effort to acquire these complex rules. So, rapid automatized naming and orthographic knowledge skills may be the most important Chinese reading skills [18]. Unlike the language of the West, phonological awareness deficits do not show a higher incidence in Chinese dyslexia. Chinese characters are semiotic characters, and their form and meaning are closely related, so the causes of Chinese dyslexia may be more complicated [17].

4.2. Type of Control Group

Compared to the age-matched typically developing children, children with dyslexia have a higher percentage of rapid automatized naming deficit (48%). This is followed by morphological awareness deficits (46%), orthographic knowledge deficits (43%), phonological awareness deficits (37%), visual and motor skills deficits (29%), and short-term memory and working memory deficits (25%). However, compared to the reading-level-matched typically developing children, children with dyslexia have a higher percentage of phonological awareness deficits (49%). This is followed by orthographic knowledge deficits (31%), rapid automatized naming deficits (28%), visual and motor skills deficits (24%), and morphological awareness deficit (13%). According to the existing results, the percentage of rapid automatized naming deficits and orthographic knowledge deficits was relatively high, when the control group was age-matched typically developing children or reading-level-matched typically developing children. In addition, the percentage of visual and motor skills deficits and short-term memory and working memory deficits was relatively low. Since reading is a language activity, the deficits of dyslexia children were mainly related to reading language skills. So, researchers paid more attention to the linguistic cognitive deficits of dyslexia, such as phonological awareness deficits, orthographic knowledge deficits and rapid automatized naming deficits. However, in recent years, visual deficits have also been proposed as the core deficit of dyslexia. Bosse found in two studies of people in France and Britain that dyslexia did not seem to be due to phonological deficits and the visual attention deficit is likely to be the underlying cause of dyslexia [39]. Franceschini et al. found that visual spatial attention in preschool children could predict future reading acquisition [40]. Although the results of the study cannot prove the importance of basic cognitive skills, the explanation of the causes of dyslexia should be found from more perspectives to find deeper reasons.

4.3. Age, Location and Standard

Studies have shown that age may influence the deficit profile of children with dyslexia [41]. According to the results of our study, the percentage of cognitive skill deficits in different age groups is relatively close, except for the relatively large difference in morphological awareness deficits (24% vs. 51%). We found that there was an imbalanced development of morphological awareness. This reminds us to pay more attention to the development of morphological awareness in the lower grades and intervene in time to avoid morphological awareness deficits in the higher grades. Although some studies also found that age may influence the deficit profiles of rapid automatized naming [6], we did not find a significant difference between the two age groups, in terms of proportion of occurrence. It is possible that the sample size we have at present is relatively narrow in age range and the span is not large enough. Therefore, it is necessary to further study the interaction between age and cognitive skills.
Location may also be the reason for the difference in the incidence of cognitive deficits among dyslexic groups, as there are still many differences in spoken language, writing scripts and early reading instructions between Mainland China and Hong Kong [6]. The percentages of phonological awareness deficits (49% vs. 26%), rapid automatized naming deficits (36% vs. 56%), morphological awareness deficits (37% vs. 47%) between Mainland and Hong Kong have a relatively large difference. Although some studies suggested that the education environment is similar between Mainland China and Hong Kong [6], the children from Hong Kong may be more familiar with English than the children from Mainland China. This may have a certain effect on the skill deficits of dyslexia.
The differences in the definition of a skill deficit may also lead to differences in incidence. Although most studies used standard deviation segmentation, some used 1 standard deviation lower than the control group, while others used 1.5 standard deviation or 2 standard deviation lower than the control group. However, this study did not find a trend of decreasing incidence with the stricter standards, which may be due to the fact that most of the existing studies were based on the cut-off score of 1.5 standard deviations, while the sample size of other standards was limited.

4.4. Limitations

Our findings are only based on the combined results of 22 articles, which is a small number of studies for a meta-analysis. This may be due to our poor search coverage and stringent screening criteria, which also reduce the reliability of the findings. In particular in the subgroup analysis, many groups involved only one study, which brings great challenges to the reliability of our research results. In addition, we paid more attention to language cognitive skills and general cognitive skills that affect developmental dyslexia, while higher-order cognitive skills, such as creativity, were not involved. However, some studies have found that dyslexia may be related to higher levels of creativity [42,43]. Therefore, higher-order cognitive skills that affect developmental dyslexia may also need further exploration.

5. Conclusions

The present study is the first meta-analysis to systematically investigate the core deficit among Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Based on the above analysis, we found that the rapid automatized naming deficits are the core deficit of Chinese developmental dyslexia. In addition, the pooled percentages of orthographic knowledge deficits, phonological awareness deficits, and morphological awareness deficits among Chinese children with dyslexia are also relatively higher. The pooled percentages of short-term memory and visual and motor skills deficit are relatively lower. These findings could have important implications for the screening of developmental dyslexia. The accuracy of diagnosis could be improved through the measurement of cognitive skills of developmental dyslexia. Moreover, in the daily teaching of Chinese, we should emphasize rapid automatized naming, orthographic knowledge and phonological awareness and strengthen skills training to reduce the incidence of developmental dyslexia. Certainly, the findings support the multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia.

Author Contributions

X.L. and M.H. conceived and designed the protocol. X.L. critically revised the manuscript for methodological and intellectual content. M.H. and H.L. participated in the development of the search strategy and data analysis. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 62106246).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

All data related to the research are presented in the article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. PRISMA 2020 Checklist.
Table A1. PRISMA 2020 Checklist.
Section and Topic Item #Checklist Item Location Where Item Is Reported
TITLE
Title 1Identify the report as a systematic review.Title: Row 1 through 3
ABSTRACT
Abstract 2See the PRISMA 2020 for Abstracts checklist.Abstract: Row 9 through 19
INTRODUCTION
Rationale 3Describe the rationale for the review in the context of existing knowledge.1. Introduction
Objectives 4Provide an explicit statement of the objective(s) or question(s) the review addresses.1. Introduction
METHODS
Eligibility criteria 5Specify the inclusion and exclusion criteria for the review and how studies were grouped for the syntheses.2.2. Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Information sources 6Specify all databases, registers, websites, organizations, reference lists and other sources searched or consulted to identify studies. Specify the date when each source was last searched or consulted.2.1. Search strategy and procedure
Search strategy7Present the full search strategies for all databases, registers and websites, including any filters and limits used.2.1. Search strategy and procedure
Selection process8Specify the methods used to decide whether a study met the inclusion criteria of the review, including how many reviewers screened each record and each report retrieved, whether they worked independently, and if applicable, details of automation tools used in the process.2.2. Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Data collection process 9Specify the methods used to collect data from reports, including how many reviewers collected data from each report, whether they worked independently, any processes for obtaining or confirming data from study investigators, and if applicable, details of automation tools used in the process.2.3.1. Coding procedure
Data items 10aList and define all outcomes for which data were sought. Specify whether all results that were compatible with each outcome domain in each study were sought (e.g., for all measures, time points, analyses), and if not, the methods used to decide which results to collect.2.3.2. Variables
10bList and define all other variables for which data were sought (e.g., participant and intervention characteristics, funding sources). Describe any assumptions made about any missing or unclear information.2.3. Recorded variables and coding
Study risk of bias assessment11Specify the methods used to assess risk of bias in the included studies, including details of the tool(s) used, how many reviewers assessed each study and whether they worked independently, and if applicable, details of automation tools used in the process.2.4. Statistical analysis
Effect measures 12Specify for each outcome the effect measure(s) (e.g., risk ratio, mean difference) used in the synthesis or presentation of results.3.2. Pooled percentage
Synthesis methods13aDescribe the processes used to decide which studies were eligible for each synthesis (e.g., tabulating the study intervention characteristics and comparing against the planned groups for each synthesis (item #5)).3.1. Study characteristics
13bDescribe any methods required to prepare the data for presentation or synthesis, such as the handling of missing summary statistics, or data conversions.3.1. Study characteristics
13cDescribe any methods used to tabulate or visually display results of individual studies and syntheses.3.1. Study characteristics
13dDescribe any methods used to synthesize results and provide a rationale for the choice(s). If a meta-analysis was performed, describe the model(s), method(s) to identify the presence and extent of statistical heterogeneity, and software package(s) used.2.4. Statistical analysis
13eDescribe any methods used to explore the possible causes of heterogeneity among the study results (e.g., subgroup analysis, meta-regression).2.4. Statistical analysis
13fDescribe any sensitivity analyses conducted to assess the robustness of the synthesized results.3.2. Pooled percentage
Reporting bias assessment14Describe any methods used to assess risk of bias due to missing results in a synthesis (arising from reporting biases).2.4. Statistical analysis
Certainty assessment15Describe any methods used to assess certainty (or confidence) in the body of evidence for an outcome.2.4. Statistical analysis
RESULTS
Study selection 16aDescribe the results of the search and selection process, from the number of records identified in the search to the number of studies included in the review, ideally using a flow diagram.3. Results
16bCite studies that might appear to meet the inclusion criteria, but were excluded, and explain why they were excluded.3. Results
Study characteristics 17Cite each included study and present its characteristics.3.1. Study characteristics
Risk of bias in studies 18Present assessments of risk of bias for each included study.3.2. Pooled percentage
Results of individual studies 19For all outcomes present for each study: (a) summary statistics for each group (where appropriate) and (b) an effect estimate and its precision (e.g., confidence/credible interval), ideally using structured tables or plots.3.2. Pooled percentage
Results of syntheses20aFor each synthesis, briefly summarize the characteristics and risk of bias among contributing studies.3.2. Pooled percentage
20bPresent results of all statistical syntheses conducted. If meta-analysis was carried out, present for each the summary estimate and its precision (e.g., confidence/credible interval) and measures of statistical heterogeneity. If comparing groups, describe the direction of the effect.3.2. Pooled percentage
20cPresent results of all investigations of the possible causes of heterogeneity among study results.3.2. Pooled percentage
20dPresent results of all sensitivity analyses conducted to assess the robustness of the synthesized results.3.2. Pooled percentage
Reporting biases21Present assessments of risk of bias due to missing results (arising from reporting biases) for each synthesis assessed.3.2. Pooled percentage
Certainty of evidence 22Present assessments of certainty (or confidence) in the body of evidence for each outcome assessed.3.2. Pooled percentage
DISCUSSION
Discussion 23aProvide a general interpretation of the results in the context of other evidence.4. Discussion
23bDiscuss any limitations of the evidence included in the review.4.4. Limitations
23cDiscuss any limitations of the review processes used.4.4. Limitations
23dDiscuss implications of the results for practice, policy, and future research.5. Conclusions
OTHER INFORMATION
Registration and protocol24aProvide registration information for the review, including register name and registration number, or state that the review was not registered.2.1. Search strategy and procedure
24bIndicate where the review protocol can be accessed, or state that a protocol was not prepared.2.1. Search strategy and procedure
24cDescribe and explain any amendments to the information provided at registration or in the protocol.2.1. Search strategy and procedure
Support25Describe sources of financial or non-financial support for the review, and the role of the funders or sponsors in the review.Institutional Review Board Statement
Competing interests26Declare any competing interests of the review authors.Conflicts of Interest
Availability of data, code and other materials27Report which of the following are publicly available and where they can be found; template data collection forms; data extracted from included studies; data used for all analyses; analytic code; any other materials used in the review.Data Availability Statement
Figure A1. PRISMA flow diagram of the literature search and study selection.
Figure A1. PRISMA flow diagram of the literature search and study selection.
Brainsci 12 00548 g0a1
Figure A2. Funnel plot.
Figure A2. Funnel plot.
Brainsci 12 00548 g0a2

References

  1. Ring, J.; Black, J.L. The multiple deficit model of dyslexia: What does it mean for identification and intervention? Ann. Dyslexia 2018, 68, 104–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Stevenson, H.W.; Stigler, J.W.; Lucker, G.W.; Lee, S.Y.; Hsu, C.C.; Kitamura, S. Reading disabilities: The case of Chinese, Japanese, and English. Child Dev. 1982, 53, 1164–1181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Zhang, C.F.; Zhang, J.; Yin, R. Experimental research on the reading disability of Chinese students. Psychol. Sci. 1996, 19, 222–226. [Google Scholar]
  4. Peng, P.; Tao, S.; Li, B.L. The deficit profile of working memory, inhibition, and updating in Chinese children with reading difficulties. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2013, 25, 111–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Shu, H.; McBride-Chang, C.; Wu, S.; Liu, H. Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. J. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 98, 122–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Peng, P.; Wang, C.; Tao, S.; Sun, C. The Deficit Profiles of Chinese Children with Reading Difficulties: A Meta-analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 29, 513–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Tamer, A.E.; Rawhia, A.; Haidy, E.H. Assessment of Arabic phonological awareness and its relation to word reading ability. Logop. Phoniatr. Vocol. 2016, 41, 174–180. [Google Scholar]
  8. Denckla, M.B.; Rudel, R.G. Rapid ‘automatized’ naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia 1976, 14, 471–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Apel, K. What Is Orthographic Knowledge? Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2011, 42, 592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kuo, L.J.; Anderson, R.C. Morphological Awareness and Learning to Read: A Cross-Language Perspective. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 41, 161–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ho, C.S.H.; Chan, D.W.-O.; Lee, S.-H.; Tsang, S.-M.; Luan, V.H. Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition 2004, 91, 43–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Chung, K.K.H.; Lo, J.C.M.; McBride, C. Cognitive-linguistic profiles of Chinese typical-functioning adolescent dyslexics and high-functioning dyslexics. Ann. Dyslexia 2018, 68, 229–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Liu, W.; Liu, X.; Zhang, J. A Preliminary Study Subtypes of Chinese Developmental Dyslexia. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2006, 38, 681–693. [Google Scholar]
  14. Yang, L.; Li, C.; Li, X.; Zhai, M.; An, Q.; Zhang, Y.; Zhao, J.; Weng, X. Prevalence of Developmental Dyslexia in Primary School Children: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Cheung, H.; Chung, K.K.; Wong, S.W.; McBride-Chang, C.; Penney, T.B.; Ho, C.S. Perception of tone and aspiration contrasts in Chinese children with dyslexia. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2009, 50, 726–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Goswami, U.; Bryant, P. The interpretation of studies using the reading level design. J. Read. Behav. 1989, 21, 413–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Xiong, J.; Yan, G. Analysis of the Main Subtypes of Chinese Developmental Dyslexia. Stud. Psychol. Behav. 2014, 12, 496–500. [Google Scholar]
  18. Wang, X.; Li, Q.; Deng, C. An Experimental Study on the Phonetic Processing and Orthographic Processing Deficit of the Chinese Reading Disability. J. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 37, 803–808. [Google Scholar]
  19. Wu, S.; Shu, H.; Wang, Y. The Heterogeneity of Chinese Developmental Dyslexia. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 2004, 3, 46–50. [Google Scholar]
  20. Peng, H.; Liang, W.L.; Zhang, Z.X.; Li, H.; Shu, H.; Tardif, T.; Fletcher, P. Screen of Chinese Reading at-risk children. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 2007, 23, 89–92. [Google Scholar]
  21. Li, H.; Shu, H. The Linguistic Cognitive Deficiency of Developmental Dyslexic Children. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 32, 301–303+300. [Google Scholar]
  22. Meng, X.; Zhou, X.; Zeng, B.; Kong, R.; Zhuang, J. Visual Perceptual Skills and Reading Abilities in Chinese Speaking Children. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2002, 34, 16–22. [Google Scholar]
  23. Wu, S.; Shu, H.; Liu, Y. The Role of Morphological Awareness in Chinese Children Reading. Stud. Psychol. Behav. 2005, 3, 35–38. [Google Scholar]
  24. Chen, H.; Yang, Z.; Tang, X. Subtypes of Reading Disorders in Chinese Children. Chin. Ment. Health J. 2002, 16, 52–54. [Google Scholar]
  25. Ho, C.S.H.; Chan, D.W.O.; Tsang, S.M.; Lee, S.H. The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Dev. Psychol. 2002, 38, 543–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Chen, N.T.; Zheng, M.; Ho, C.S.H. Examining the visual attention span deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Read. Writ. 2019, 32, 639–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Song, S.; Zhang, Y.; Shu, H.; Su, M.; McBride, C. Universal and Specific Predictors of Chinese Children with Dyslexia – Exploring the Cognitive Deficits and Subtypes. Front. Psychol. 2020, 10, 2904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Huo, S.; Wu, K.C.; Mo, J.; Wang, J.; Maurer, U. Children with Chinese Dyslexia Acquiring English Literacy: Interaction Between Cognitive Subtypes of Dyslexia and Orthographies. J. Learn. Disabil. 2021, 55, 229–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Li, H.; Shu, H.; McBride-Chang, C.; Liu, H.Y.; Xue, J. Paired associate learning in Chinese children with dyslexia. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2009, 103, 135–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Chung, K.K.H.; Ho, C.S.-H.; Chan, D.W.; Tsang, S.-M.; Lee, S.-H. Cognitive Profiles of Chinese Adolescents with Dyslexia. Dyslexia 2010, 16, 2–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Chung, K.K.H.; Lo, J.C.M.; Ho, C.S.-H.; Xiao, X.; Chan, D.W. Syntactic and discourse skills in Chinese adolescent readers with dyslexia: A profiling study. Ann. Dyslexia 2014, 64, 222–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Wang, X.; Georgiou, G.K.; Das, J.P.; Li, Q. Cognitive Processing Skills and Developmental Dyslexia in Chinese. J. Learn. Disabil. 2012, 45, 526–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Chan, W.S.R.; Hung, S.F.; Liu, S.N.; Lee, C.K.K. Cognitive profiling in Chinese developmental dyslexia with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Read. Writ. 2008, 21, 661–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Zhao, J.; Liu, M.; Liu, H.; Huang, C. Increased deficit of visual attention span with development in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 3153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Cheng, C.; Yao, Y.; Wang, Z.; Zhao, J. Visual attention span and phonological skills in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2021, 116, 104015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Song, S.; Georgiou, G.K.; Su, M.; Hua, S. How Well Do Phonological Awareness and Rapid Automatized Naming Correlate with Chinese Reading Accuracy and Fluency? A Meta-Analysis. Sci. Stud. Read. 2016, 20, 99–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. McBride-Chang, C.; Ho, C.S.H. Developmental issues in Chinese children’s character acquisition. J. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 92, 50–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Wolf, M. A Provisional, Integrative Account of Phonological and Naming-Speed Deficits in Dyslexia: Implications for Diagnosis and Intervention; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  39. Bosse, M.L.; Tainturier, M.J.; Valdois, S. Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition 2007, 104, 198–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Franceschini, S.; Gori, S.; Ruffino, M.; Pedrolli, K.; Facoetti, A. A Causal Link between Visual Spatial Attention and Reading Acquisition. Curr. Biol. 2012, 22, 814–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Lei, L.; Pan, J.; Liu, H.; McBride-Chang, C.; Li, H.; Zhang, Y.; Chen, L.; Tardif, T.; Liang, W.; Zhang, Z.; et al. Developmental trajectories of reading development and impairment from ages 3 to 8 years in Chinese children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2011, 52, 212–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Majeed, N.M.; Hartanto, A.; Tan, J.J.X. Developmental dyslexia and creativity: A meta-analysis. Dyslexia 2021, 27, 187–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Cockcroft, K.; Hartgill, M. Focusing on the abilities in learning disabilities: Dyslexia and creativity. Educ. Chang. 2004, 8, 61–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the literature search and study selection.
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the literature search and study selection.
Brainsci 12 00548 g001
Table 1. The detailed explanation of the variables.
Table 1. The detailed explanation of the variables.
VariablesContentsSpecific Description
The Sample CharacteristicsSample sizeThe number of people with Chinese developmental dyslexia was coded.
AgeThe mean age of the sample was coded.
The Definition Criteria of Cognitive DeficitsType of Control GroupThe researchers used age-matched typically developing or reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls to further confirm whether children with DD have certain deficits [15,16]. The type of control was coded.
Criterion of Cognitive deficitsThe children were identified as having a cognitive deficit if their performance was below the cut-off criteria of the control group (e.g., 1.5SD below the mean of a participant’s respective age group) on the cognitive deficits screening measures.
The Type of Cognitive Deficits and Percentage of Different Cognitive DeficitsType of Cognitive DeficitsThe cognitive deficits included phonological awareness, rapid automatized naming, orthographic knowledge, morphological awareness, short-term memory and working memory, and visual and motor skills. If there was a cognitive deficit in the paper that did not fall into any of the above categories, it went into the other category.
The Percentage of Cognitive DeficitsThe percentage or the sample size of different cognitive deficits was coded.
Table 2. Characteristics of the included studies, examining the percentage of cognitive skill deficits for the dyslexia group.
Table 2. Characteristics of the included studies, examining the percentage of cognitive skill deficits for the dyslexia group.
ResearchLocationDyslexia (n)Age (Years)Control GroupStandardPhonological
Awareness Deficits
Rapid
Automatized
Naming Deficits
Orthographic Knowledge DeficitsMorphological
Awareness Deficits
Short-Term Memory and Working Memory DeficitsVisual and Motor Skills Deficits
Liu, Liu and Zhang, 2006 [13] Mainland2910.43RC1 SD0.4480.4140.3450.138
Xiong and Yan, 2014 [17]Mainland5710.58RC1.5 SD0.5090.1750.2980.123
Wang, Li and Deng, 2014 [18] Mainland3311.54CA1.5 SD0.6970.3480.636 0.364
Wu, Shu and Wang, 2004 [19]Mainland1511.6CA1.65 SD0.467 1
Peng et al., 2007 [20]Mainland253.5CA2 SD0.4 0.160.16
Li and Shu, 2009 [21]Mainland4111.7CA1 SD0.4150.341 0.5120.354
Meng, Zhou, Zeng, Kong and Zhuang, 2002 [22]Mainland1510–11.5CAMean0.733 0.533
Wu, Shu and Liu, 2005 [23]Mainland9111.83–12.17CA1.5 SD0.4290.407 0.758
Chen, Yang and Tang, 2002 [24]Mainland7710ClusterCluster 0.649
Ho et al., 2004 [11]Hong Kong1478.275CA1.5 SD0.2520.5710.42 0.3210.272
Ho, Chan, Tsang and Lee, 2002 [25]Hong Kong308.67CA1.5 SD0.1670.50.389 0.13350.367
Chen, Zheng and Ho, 2019 [26]Hong Kong2510.45CA1.5 SD 0.4
Hong Kong2510.45RC1.5 SD 0.24
Song, Zhang, Shu, Su and McBride, 2020 [27] Mainland22310.84ClusterCluster0.5250.372 0.534
Huo, Wu, Mo, Wang and Maurer, 2021 [28]Hong Kong848.39ClusterCluster0.762 0.571
Li, Shu, McBride-Chang, Liu and Xue, 2009 [29]Mainland4111.73CA1.5 SD0.220.268 0.3660.0976
Chung et al., 2010 [30]Hong Kong2713.65CA1.5 SD0.0740.3520.4070.2960.259
Chung, Lo, Ho, Xiao and Chan, 2014 [31]Hong Kong5213.42CA1.5 SD 0.61 0.670.33
Wang, Georgiou, Das and Li, 2012 [32]Mainland279.98CA1.5 SD0.51850.44450.5926 0.5185
Chung, Lo and McBride, 2018 [12]Hong Kong509.04CA1 SD0.090.520.267 0.207
Hong Kong2513.31CA1 SD 0.660.460.420.26
Chan, Hung, Liu and Lee, 2008 [33]Hong Kong438.17CA1.5 SD0.2330.6280.372 0.116
Zhao, Liu, Liu and Huang, 2018 [34]Mainland208.88CA1.65 SD 0.1
Mainland1910.19CA1.65 SD 0.0526
Mainland1811.68CA1.65 SD 0.3889
Cheng, Yao, Wang and Zhao 2021 [35]Mainland4510.11CA1.5 SD0.60.533 0.4
Abbreviations: CA = age-matched typically developing children as controls; RC = reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls; SD = standard deviation.
Table 3. The percentage of phonological awareness deficits for dyslexia group.
Table 3. The percentage of phonological awareness deficits for dyslexia group.
IndexNumber of StudiesHeterogeneity Test ModelResults
pI2Pooled Percentage95% CI
Total18<0.00193.20%random0.41(0.31, 0.52)
Age
Younger than 11 years old11<0.00194.50%random0.41(0.27, 0.55)
Older than 11 years old6<0.00191.00%random0.38(0.19, 0.56)
Location
Mainland12<0.00169.10%random0.49(0.42, 0.56)
Hong Kong6<0.00196.70%random0.26(0.05, 0.47)
Control group
CA14<0.00189.70%random0.37(0.26, 0.47)
RC20.5910.00%fixed0.49(0.38, 0.59)
Standard
mean1fixed0.73(0.51, 0.96)
1SD3<0.00191.10%random0.31(0.05, 0.57)
1.5SD10<0.00190.00%random0.36(0.24, 0.48)
1.65SD1fixed0.47(0.21, 0.72)
2SD1fixed0.4(0.21, 0.59)
Cluster2<0.00194.20%random0.64(0.41, 0.87)
Abbreviations: CA= age-matched typically developing children as controls; RC= reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls; SD = standard deviation.
Table 4. The percentage of rapid automatized naming deficits for the dyslexia group.
Table 4. The percentage of rapid automatized naming deficits for the dyslexia group.
IndexNumber of StudiesHeterogeneity Test ModelResults
pI2Pooled Percentage95% CI
Total16<0.00179.70%random0.44(0.37, 0.51)
Age
Younger than 11 years old9<0.00184.70%random0.46(0.36, 0.56)
Older than 11 years old7<0.00180.70%random0.42(0.32, 0.53)
Location
Mainland90.00464.80%random0.36(0.29, 0.43)
Hong Kong70.20729.10%fixed0.56(0.51, 0.61)
Control group
CA13<0.00167.20%random0.48(0.41, 0.54)
RC20.02280.90%random0.28(0.05, 0.52)
Standard
1SD40.04762.20%random0.48(0.35, 0.61)
1.5SD11<0.00183.80%random0.44(0.34, 0.54)
Cluster1fixed0.37(0.31, 0.44)
Abbreviations: CA = age-matched typically developing children as controls; RC = reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls; SD = standard deviation.
Table 5. The percentage of orthographic knowledge deficits for the dyslexia group.
Table 5. The percentage of orthographic knowledge deficits for the dyslexia group.
IndexNumber of StudiesHeterogeneity Test ModelResults
pI2Pooled Percentage95% CI
Total110.00165.80%random0.43(0.36, 0.50)
Age
Younger than 11 years old80.00268.40%random0.4(0.32, 0.49)
Older than 11 years old30.15845.90%fixed0.51(0.41, 0.62)
Location
Mainland40.00279.60%random0.46(0.29, 0.64)
Hong Kong70.02458.70%random0.41(0.34, 0.49)
Control group
CA80.0258.10%random0.43(0.35, 0.51)
RC20.6610.00%fixed0.31(0.22, 0.41)
Standard
1SD30.25427.00%fixed0.33(0.24, 0.42)
1.5SD70.02458.90%random0.43(0.35, 0.52)
Cluster1fixed0.57(0.47, 0.68)
Abbreviations: CA= age-matched typically developing children as controls; RC= reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls; SD = standard deviation.
Table 6. The percentage of morphological awareness deficits for dyslexia group.
Table 6. The percentage of morphological awareness deficits for dyslexia group.
IndexNumber of StudiesHeterogeneity Test ModelResults
pI2Pooled Percentage95% CI
Total10<0.00194.50%random0.4(0.24, 0.55)
Age
Younger than 11 years old4<0.00196.00%random0.24(0.00, 0.48)
Older than 11 years old6<0.00187.30%random0.51(0.35, 0.67)
Location
Mainland7<0.00196.00%random0.37(0.18, 0.57)
Hong Kong30.00284.40%random0.47(0.23, 0.71)
Control group
CA7<0.00191.30%random0.46(0.28, 0.64)
RC20.8460%fixed0.13(0.06, 0.2)
Standard
1SD30.00186.80%random0.35(0.11, 0.60)
1.5SD5<0.00197.50%random0.44(0.16, 0.73)
2SD1fixed0.16(0.02, 0.30)
Cluster1fixed0.53(0.47, 0.60)
Abbreviations: CA = age-matched typically developing children as controls; RC = reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls; SD = standard deviation.
Table 7. The percentage of short-term memory and working memory deficits for dyslexia group.
Table 7. The percentage of short-term memory and working memory deficits for dyslexia group.
IndexNumber of StudiesHeterogeneity Test ModelResults
pI2Pooled Percentage95% CI
Total12<0.00171.20%random0.25(0.18, 0.31)
Age
Younger than 11 years old6<0.00178.70%random0.23(0.13, 0.33)
Older than 11 years old60.01265.80%random0.26(0.17, 0.36)
Location
Mainland5<0.00181.80%random0.28(0.13, 0.43)
Hong Kong70.01362.80%random0.23(0.16, 0.30)
Control group
CA12<0.00171.20%random0.25(0.18, 0.31)
RC0
Standard
1SD30.490.00%fixed0.25(0.17, 0.33)
1.5SD8<0.00180.30%random0.26(0.16, 0.35)
2SD1fixed0.16(0.02, 0.30)
Abbreviations: CA= age-matched typically developing children as controls; RC= reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls; SD = standard deviation.
Table 8. The percentage of visual and motor skills deficits for dyslexia group.
Table 8. The percentage of visual and motor skills deficits for dyslexia group.
IndexNumber of StudiesHeterogeneity Test ModelResults
pI2Pooled Percentage95% CI
Total10<0.00189.00%random0.33(0.20, 0.46)
Age
Younger than 11 years old8<0.00191.00%random0.31(0.16, 0.45)
Older than 11 years old1__fixed0.39(0.16, 0.61)
Location
Mainland5<0.00193.70%random0.35(0.12, 0.58)
Hong Kong40.4510.00%fixed0.29(0.23, 0.35)
Control group
CA8<0.00179.90%random0.29(0.18, 0.40)
RC1__fixed0.24(0.07, 0.41)
Standard
mean1fixed0.53(0.28, 0.79)
1.5SD50.33512.40%fixed0.31(0.25, 0.36)
1.65SD30.02872.10%random0.15(−0.01, 0.31)
Cluster1fixed0.65(0.54, 0.76)
Abbreviations: CA = age-matched typically developing children as controls; RC = reading-level-matched typically developing children as controls; SD = standard deviation.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Li, X.; Hu, M.; Liang, H. The Percentages of Cognitive Skills Deficits among Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 548. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12050548

AMA Style

Li X, Hu M, Liang H. The Percentages of Cognitive Skills Deficits among Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(5):548. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12050548

Chicago/Turabian Style

Li, Xin, Mingming Hu, and Huadong Liang. 2022. "The Percentages of Cognitive Skills Deficits among Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" Brain Sciences 12, no. 5: 548. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12050548

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop