Next Article in Journal
Virtual Reality Applications in Neurorehabilitation: Current Panorama and Challenges
Previous Article in Journal
From Theory to Practice: Implementing the WHO 2021 Classification of Adult Diffuse Gliomas in Neuropathology Diagnosis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sleep Duration on Workdays Is Correlated with Subjective Workload and Subjective Impact of High Workload on Sleep in Young Healthy Adults

1
Institute of Anatomy II, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany
2
Institute for Occupational, Social and Environmental Medicine, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(5), 818; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13050818
Submission received: 27 April 2023 / Revised: 12 May 2023 / Accepted: 13 May 2023 / Published: 18 May 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Sleep and Circadian Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
Psychosocial stress is widespread worldwide and particularly affects young adults. There is a close and bidirectional relationship between sleep quality and mental health. Sleep duration, which is an important feature of sleep quality, shows both intra-individual variations and inter-individual differences. Internal clocks control individual sleep timing, which, in turn, defines the chronotype. On workdays, however, the end and duration of sleep are largely limited by external factors, such as alarm clocks, especially in later chronotypes. The aim of this study is to investigate whether there is a relationship between sleep timing and duration on workdays and measures for psychosocial stress, such as anxiety and depression; subjective workload; and the subjective impact of a high workload on sleep. We used a combination of Fitbit wearable actigraphy data and a questionnaire survey of young, healthy medical students and calculated correlations between the respective variables. We found that a shorter sleep duration on workdays is associated with a higher subjective workload and a higher subjective impact of a high workload on sleep, which, in turn, are associated with higher measures of anxiety and depression. Our study contributes to understanding the importance of sleep timing/duration and their regularity on weekdays for subjectively perceived psychosocial stress.

1. Introduction

Mood-related disorders such as anxiety and depression are highly prevalent globally and disproportionately affect young adults [1]. Thus, the mental health of young adults and students in higher education, in particular, remains a matter of concern [2,3,4]. Psychosocial factors such as stress, depression, and anxiety have already been well examined in medical students [5,6]. Study-related stressors experienced by medical students include high workloads and tight time schedules [7,8]. Subjective workload has a strong impact on mental health in general [9,10], as well as on motivation and academic performance [11]. Moreover, psychological stress is a risk factor for various psychosomatic diseases, such as cardiovascular diseases [12], and gut disorders [13], and is associated with muscle parafunctional activity [14].
Sleep plays a crucial role in health in general and mental health in particular [15]. Sleep problems, which are highly prevalent in modern society, are associated with multiple adverse consequences [16]. Sleep duration, which is an important attribute of sleep quality [17], shows substantial intra- and inter-individual variations [15]. Large-scale cross-sectional studies show that sleep duration is associated with general health, as well as the physical and mental quality of life [18]. Although the relationship between sleep and mood-related factors is bidirectional, there is evidence that the effect of sleep quality on mood is greater than vice versa [19]. Importantly, daily rhythms in sleep and wake are controlled by a complex interplay of different oscillators, including exogenous oscillators, such as light–dark and social cycles, and endogenous oscillators, such as circadian and homeostatic oscillators [20,21]. According to the endogenous oscillators, individuals differ in their intrinsically determined preferred sleep timing, defining the chronotype [22]. However, in modern society, sleep duration (SD) and sleep timing on workdays are mainly determined by external factors, such as the morning alarm clock. Thus, particularly in later chronotypes, early work schedules result in short sleep duration on workdays. This could be one reason why sleep duration has decreased in many countries over the past 100 years [23] and also why sleep quality is lower on workdays than on free days [24]. There is increasing evidence that sleep duration is a critical driver of major depression [25], and later chronotypes demonstrate higher levels of depression and sleep disturbances [26,27]. In addition, intra-individual variability in sleep duration as a result of shift work substantially impacts mood and depression [28]. In outpatient studies, sleep quality and timing are typically evaluated by questionnaires, which do not distinguish between workdays and free days. However, recent research shows that sleep quality differs between weekdays and workdays [24], so it is important to make this distinction. In addition, a questionnaire does not allow longitudinal data collection, which is a prerequisite for the analysis of regularity. Wearable actigraphy offers a great opportunity to longitudinally study the relationships between sleep timing, its inter- and intra-individual variation, and mental health under real-life conditions [25,26]. Using a combination of Fitbit wearable actigraphy data and a questionnaire survey of young, healthy medical students, we were recently able to show that the discrepancy between sleep duration on workdays and free days is associated with higher fragmentation of REM sleep, which, in turn, is associated with higher subjective tiredness upon waking [29].This indicates that limiting sleep duration by starting work early has a negative impact on objective sleep architecture and subjective sleep quality. Here, we examine other variables from the same dataset. Our approach, using Fitbit wearable actigraphy in combination with questionnaires, allows for the analysis of correlations of objective measures of sleep timing/duration and their regularity on workdays and free days with subjective measures of psychosocial stress, such as anxiety and depression; subjective workload; and the subjective impact of a high workload on sleep. With this study, we contribute to understanding the importance of sleep timing/duration and their regularity on weekdays for subjectively perceived psychosocial stress.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Ethics and Participants

The questionnaire surveys and the assessments of Fitbit sleep data in healthy subjects are in agreement with the ethical requirements of the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty (ChronoSleep study consent number: 2019-3786). Exclusion criteria were (i) age below 18 years, (ii) shift work, (iii) work on weekends, (iv) chronic diseases including sleep disorders, and (v) chronic medication including sleep medication. All subjects provided informed consent after receiving a complete description of the study. The data used for this study came from the same dataset as from a previous study [29]. For this dataset, medical students in their first year were invited to participate between 25 June 2021 and 19 May 2022. Importantly, during this period, most of the courses did not start before 11 am because, due to the COVID-19 pandemic, all lectures were provided as screencasts and could be attended by the students according to their own time schedule.

2.2. Study Design

Volunteers received a pseudonym and were equipped with a Fitbit Inspire multisensory (motion and heart rate) sleep-tracking device (San Francisco, CA, USA). They were asked to wear it for 90 days, i.e., 64 workdays and 26 free days, as continuously as possible, especially at night. After the data collection period of 90 days, participants filled out the online questionnaire and authorized sleep data transfer from Fitbit to our study server. All questions in the survey were mandatory and could only be answered once. Data from the questionnaire and Fitbit were assigned by a pseudonym.

2.3. Measures, Data Processing, and Statistics

The online questionnaire included items on demographics; workload (WL) (“How high would you rate your workload in general?”); the impact of workload on sleep (WLI) (“Does the heavy workload have a negative effect on your sleep quality?”); cohabitation (cohab) (“Do you live alone or together with family, shared apartment, friends, spouse”); alarm clock use on workdays and free days; and general health, including the 4-item depression and anxiety screen (PHQ-4) and others [29]. The scores and distribution of the respective variables assessed by the questionnaire are shown in Table 1.
A customized software application was used to transfer and pre-process Fitbit sleep data [29]. The following parameters were separately calculated based on Fitbit sleep data for workdays (weekdays) and free days (weekends): (i) sleep duration (SD) in a decimal format of hours, (ii) intra-individual variation in sleep duration as standard deviation (SD_std), and (iii) start and end time of sleep in a decimal format of clock time. The decimal format of hours/clock time was calculated by dividing the minutes by 60.
Statistical analyses were performed using Prism 7.01 (Graph Pad, Boston, MA, USA). Data were expressed as a percentage of the total sample or mean +/− standard deviation. A confidence interval of 95% was used consistently. The Gaussian distribution was analyzed using the D’Agostino–Pearson normality test. Differences between workdays and free days were analyzed using paired t-tests. If variables were normally distributed, correlations were analyzed using Pearson’s test, and linear regression was performed. Correlations between variables, of which at least one was not normally distributed, were calculated using the Spearman test. p-values < 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

The sample characteristics are described in detail in [29]. Briefly, the sample consisted of 59 participants, of which 43 were women (73%). The high proportion of women reflects the gender distribution among all medical students of the year. The participants were 21.18 +/− 2.36 years old. A total of 82.4 (+/− 9.7) days of Fitbit sleep data were recorded. According to the questionnaire, all participants used an alarm clock on workdays, while only 15 participants (25%) used an alarm clock on free days.

3.2. Variables Assessed by Questionnaire

The definition and distribution of variables assessed by the questionnaire are summarized in Table 1. Most of the participants lived in the community (64%) and reported a high (52%) or very high (5%) workload (WL). A little less than half reported that a high WL had an impact on their sleep (WLI) (41%). A small proportion of participants showed moderate (10%) or severe (7%) symptoms of anxiety and depression. Only WL passed the normality test.
Subjective workload (WL) is positively correlated with the subjective impact of workload on sleep (WLI) (r = 0.73, p < 0.0001). Thus, a higher subjective workload is associated with a higher subjective impact of workload on sleep.
Depression and anxiety (PHQ4) are positively correlated with both WL (r = 0.3, p = 0.02) and WLI (r = 0.34, p = 0.009) and negatively correlated with living in the community (cohab) (r = −0.27, p = 0.04). Thus, a higher score for depression and anxiety is associated with a higher subjective workload and a higher subjective impact of a high workload on sleep.

3.3. Objective Measures for Sleep Timing on Workdays and Free Days Longitudinally Assessed by Wearable Actigraphy

All of the following sleep duration and sleep timing variables passed the normality test.
Sleep duration on workdays (SD on WDs, 7.89 +/− 0.67) is significantly shorter (t = 2.57, df = 58, p = 0.012) than sleep duration on free days (SD on FDs, 8.09 +/− 0.72). SD on WD and SD on FD are positively correlated (r = 0.66, p < 0.0001). On WD, three participants (5%) had a sleep duration shorter than 7 h, while on FD, the sleep duration was longer than 7 h for all participants.
Intra-individual variation (_std) in sleep duration is significantly lower (t = 4.88, df = 58, p < 0.0001) on workdays (SD_std on WDs, 1.25 +/− 0.35) than on free days (SD_std on FDs, 1.46 +/− 0.40). SD_std on WD and SD_std on FD are positively correlated (r = 0.65, p < 0.0001).
There is no significant correlation between sleep duration and intra-individual variation in sleep duration on workdays (Figure 1a) or free days (Figure 1b).
Sleep onset is significantly earlier (t = 11.99, df = 58, p < 0.0001) on workdays (00:04 +/− 0.89) than on free days (00:92 +/− 0.97). Additionally, sleep offset is significantly earlier (t = 12.88, df = 58, p < 0.0001) on workdays (08:01 +/− 0.72) than on free days (09:02 +/− 0.87).
On workdays, SD is negatively correlated with the time of sleep onset (r = −0.58, p < 0.0001; Figure 2a). Thus, later sleep onset is associated with shorter sleep duration on workdays. On free days, SD is negatively correlated with the time of sleep onset (r = −0.40, p = 0.002) and positively correlated with the time of sleep offset (r = 0.34, p = 0.008; Figure 2b). Thus, longer sleep duration is associated with both earlier sleep onset and later sleep offset on free days.
Intra-individual variation in SD (SD_std) on workdays is positively correlated with the time of sleep onset (r = 0.26, p = 0.045) and offset (r = 0.34, p = 0.008) on free days (Figure 3a). Thus, later sleep timing on free days is associated with higher intra-individual variation in sleep duration on workdays. Intra-individual variation in SD (SD_std) on free days is not significantly correlated with the time of sleep onset or offset (Figure 3b).

3.4. Correlations between Sleep Duration and Variables Assessed by Questionnaire

Subjective workload (WL) is negatively correlated with SD on WD (r = −0.28, p = 0.03) but not significantly correlated with SD on FD (r = −0.19, p = 0.13; Figure 4a).
The subjective impact of workload on sleep is negatively correlated with SD on WD (r = −0.29, p = 0.03) and SD on FD (r = −0.29, p = 0.03; Figure 4b). Thus, a shorter sleep duration on workdays is associated with both a higher subjective workload and a higher subjective impact of a high workload on sleep, while a shorter sleep duration on free days is associated with a higher subjective impact of a high workload on sleep.
PHQ4 is negatively correlated with SD_std on FD (r = −0.38, p = 0.003) but not significantly correlated with SD_std on WD (r = −0.19, p = 0.14; Figure 5a).
Living in the community (cohab) is positively correlated with SD_std on WD (r = 0.29, p = 0.03) but not significantly correlated with SD_std on FD (r = 0.18, p = 0.17; Figure 5b).
In summary, sleep duration on workdays, which is mainly determined by sleep onset and restricted by sleep offset, is interconnected with the subjective measures of psychosocial stress (Figure 6).

4. Discussion

Consistent with our previous study on sleep timing and sleep composition in this cohort [29] and expectations from the literature [30], sleep duration is shorter and sleep time is significantly earlier on workdays than on free days. On free days, sleep duration is correlated with both the time of sleep onset and offset. On workdays, sleep duration is only correlated with the time of sleep onset. This is consistent with sleep duration on workdays being constrained by external factors, such as alarm clocks in the morning. This is, nevertheless, surprising because the participants in our study were free to choose the start of their working hours, as most morning lectures were provided as screencasts due to the COVID-19 pandemic. This indicates a high degree of discipline and is probably due to the fact that getting up late is fraught with prejudice in our society. The lower intra-individual variability in sleep duration on workdays compared to free days might also be a consequence of the constrained time of sleep offset on workdays. The positive correlation of sleep time on free days with intra-individual variability in sleep duration on workdays suggests that later chronotypes show greater variability in their sleep duration on workdays.
The results of PHQ4 screening for depression and anxiety in our cohort were comparable to a larger cohort study also conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic at another major German medical school [5]. There seems to be a sustained association between COVID-19-related stress [31] and negative mental health outcomes [32,33] but also with positive mental health outcomes [33]. Notably, a lower PHQ4 score is associated with living in the community. Living alone may have a particularly negative reinforcing effect in our cohort because during the COVID-19 pandemic, social interactions were largely reduced to flatmates, and loneliness and stress responses, as well as symptoms of depression/anxiety, were increased in adults living alone [34] or with fewer resources [35]. Living in the community and a lower PHQ4 score are associated with higher intra-individual variability in sleep duration on workdays and free days, respectively. This suggests that in our study, irregular sleep duration is more a consequence of social interactions due to living in the community and evening activities on weekends, despite COVID-19-related social confinement. Importantly, in our study, which excludes shift work, intra-individual variation in sleep duration has no negative effects on psychosocial health. In contrast, for shift work, intra-individual variability in sleep duration is correlated with depression and anxiety [25], which is consistent with the aversive effect of shift work on mental health [36,37] and health in general [38]. In this context, we would like to emphasize that we examined a relatively homogeneous cohort of young, healthy adults that did not show any major deviations. In contrast to other studies [15,26,39,40], we did not find a direct correlation between sleep duration and depression scores. This may be because the sample size in our study is too small to observe this effect or because, in contrast to recording sleep duration using questionnaires, we measured it objectively, longitudinally, and separately for workdays and free days. We were able to establish a positive correlation between PHQ4 and subjective workload and the subjective impact of a high workload on sleep, both of which are negatively correlated with sleep duration on workdays. In addition, the subjective impact of a high workload on sleep is negatively correlated with sleep duration on free days. Thus, the perception of a high workload and its subjective effect on sleep, both of which have a negative impact on depression and anxiety, are associated with shorter sleep duration. This is consistent with the association of short sleep with a high mental workload [41] and the association of psychosocial stress with poor sleep quality [42,43]. The reciprocal relationship between stress, which activates the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and sympathetic nervous system, and insomnia is well known [44]. However, it is still surprising that even for normal sleepers, there is a relationship between a slight reduction in the duration of sleep on workdays compared to days off and subjective stress levels. In addition, we would like to point out that although all participants have a comparable objective workload, the subjective workload should not be underestimated, as it has a strong influence not only on mental health [8,9] but also on academic motivation and performance [10]. Thus, our study also provides a new approach to elucidate the complex interaction between sleep timing and academic performance [45].

5. Conclusions

Our study suggests that a longer sleep duration on workdays is associated with lower levels of psychosocial stress and, hence, better mental health. Since the duration of sleep on workdays is largely determined by the endogenous chronotype, modern concepts of education and work should allow for flexible and individual working hours.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, resources, data curation, data interpretation, writing, visualization, and project administration, C.v.G.; conceptualization, methodology, data interpretation, and writing, T.M. and P.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the Research Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty (ChronoSleep study, consent number: 2019-3786).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are unavailable due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the participants in this study. We thank Ralf Faßbender and Torge Zense for administrative support. We thank Leon Holub for software development.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Steel, Z.; Marnane, C.; Iranpour, C.; Chey, T.; Jackson, J.W.; Patel, V.; Silove, D. The global prevalence of common mental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis 1980–2013. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014, 43, 476–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Son, C.; Hegde, S.; Smith, A.; Wang, X.; Sasangohar, F. Effects of COVID-19 on College Students’ Mental Health in the United States: Interview Survey Study. J. Med. Internet Res. 2020, 22, e21279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Dederichs, M.; Weber, J.; Muth, T.; Angerer, P.; Loerbroks, A. Students’ perspectives on interventions to reduce stress in medical school: A qualitative study. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0240587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Rotenstein, L.S.; Ramos, M.A.; Torre, M.; Segal, J.B.; Peluso, M.J.; Guille, C.; Sen, S.; Mata, D.A. Prevalence of Depression, Depressive Symptoms, and Suicidal Ideation among Medical Students A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. JAMA-J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2016, 316, 2214–2236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Guse, J.; Heinen, I.; Kurre, J.; Mohr, S.; Bergelt, C. Perception of the study situation and mental burden during the COVID-19 pandemic among undergraduate medical students with and without mentoring. GMS J. Med. Educ. 2020, 37, Doc72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Dyrbye, L.N.; Harper, W.; Durning, S.J.; Moutier, C.; Thomas, M.R.; Massie, F.S., Jr.; Eacker, A.; Power, D.V.; Szydlo, D.W.; Sloan, J.A.; et al. Patterns of distress in US medical students. Med. Teach. 2011, 33, 834–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Heinen, I.; Bullinger, M.; Kocalevent, R.D. Perceived stress in first year medical students—Associations with personal resources and emotional distress. BMC Med. Educ. 2017, 17, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Thun-Hohenstein, L.; Hobinger-Ablasser, C.; Geyerhofer, S.; Lampert, K.; Schreuer, M.; Fritz, C. Burnout in medical students. Neuropsychiatrie 2021, 35, 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Harvey, S.B.; Modini, M.; Joyce, S.; Milligan-Saville, J.S.; Tan, L.; Mykletun, A.; Bryant, R.A.; Christensen, H.; Mitchell, P.B. Can work make you mentally ill? A systematic meta-review of work-related risk factors for common mental health problems. Occup. Environ. Med. 2017, 74, 301–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. van der Molen, H.F.; Nieuwenhuijsen, K.; Frings-Dresen, M.H.W.; de Groene, G. Work-related psychosocial risk factors for stress-related mental disorders: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open 2020, 10, e034849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Atalay, K.D.; Can, G.F.; Erdem, S.R.; Muderrisoglu, I.H. Assessment of mental workload and academic motivation in medical students. J. Pak. Med. Assoc. 2016, 66, 574–578. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  12. Steptoe, A.; Kivimaki, M. Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nat. Rev. Cardiol. 2012, 9, 360–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Schaper, S.J.; Stengel, A. Emotional stress responsivity of patients with IBS—A systematic review. J. Psychosom. Res. 2022, 153, 110694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Zielinski, G.; Ginszt, M.; Zawadka, M.; Rutkowska, K.; Podstawka, Z.; Szkutnik, J.; Majcher, P.; Gawda, P. The Relationship between Stress and Masticatory Muscle Activity in Female Students. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 3459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Consensus Conference Panel; Watson, N.F.; Badr, M.S.; Belenky, G.; Bliwise, D.L.; Buxton, O.M.; Buysse, D.; Dinges, D.F.; Gangwisch, J.; Grandner, M.A.; et al. Joint Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society on the Recommended Amount of Sleep for a Healthy Adult: Methodology and Discussion. Sleep 2015, 38, 1161–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Medic, G.; Wille, M.; Hemels, M.E. Short- and long-term health consequences of sleep disruption. Nat. Sci. Sleep 2017, 9, 151–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Nelson, K.L.; Davis, J.E.; Corbett, C.F. Sleep quality: An evolutionary concept analysis. Nurs. Forum 2022, 57, 144–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Killgore, W.D. Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog. Brain Res. 2010, 185, 105–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Triantafillou, S.; Saeb, S.; Lattie, E.G.; Mohr, D.C.; Kording, K.P. Relationship between Sleep Quality and Mood: Ecological Momentary Assessment Study. JMIR Ment. Health 2019, 6, e12613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dijk, D.J.; von Schantz, M. Timing and consolidation of human sleep, wakefulness, and performance by a symphony of oscillators. J. Biol. Rhythms 2005, 20, 279–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Borbely, A.A.; Daan, S.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Deboer, T. The two-process model of sleep regulation: A reappraisal. J. Sleep Res. 2016, 25, 131–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Roenneberg, T.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Merrow, M. Life between clocks: Daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2003, 18, 80–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Roenneberg, T. The human sleep project. Nature 2013, 498, 427–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Pilz, L.K.; Keller, L.K.; Lenssen, D.; Roenneberg, T. Time to rethink sleep quality: PSQI scores reflect sleep quality on workdays. Sleep 2018, 41, zsy029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Fang, Y.; Forger, D.B.; Frank, E.; Sen, S.; Goldstein, C. Day-to-day variability in sleep parameters and depression risk: A prospective cohort study of training physicians. Npj Digit. Med. 2021, 4, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Weiss, C.; Woods, K.; Filipowicz, A.; Ingram, K.K. Sleep Quality, Sleep Structure, and PER3 Genotype Mediate Chronotype Effects on Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 2028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Bakotic, M.; Radosevic-Vidacek, B.; Bjelajac, A.K. Morningness-eveningness and daytime functioning in university students: The mediating role of sleep characteristics. J. Sleep Res. 2017, 26, 210–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Wefers, B.; Hitz, C.; Holter, S.M.; Trumbach, D.; Hansen, J.; Weber, P.; Putz, B.; Deussing, J.M.; de Angelis, M.H.; Roenneberg, T.; et al. MAPK signaling determines anxiety in the juvenile mouse brain but depression-like behavior in adults. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e35035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. von Gall, C.; Holub, L.; Pfeffer, M.; Eickhoff, S. Chronotype-related sleep loss is associated with reduced amplitude in circadian rhythm and higher fragmentation of REM sleep in young health adults. J. Med. Internet Res. 2023. submitted. Available online: https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/46361 (accessed on 26 April 2023).
  30. Wittmann, M.; Dinich, J.; Merrow, M.; Roenneberg, T. Social jetlag: Misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol. Int. 2006, 23, 497–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Mahgoub, I.M.; Abdelrahman, A.; Abdallah, T.A.; Ahmed, K.A.H.M.; Omer, M.E.A.; Abdelrahman, E.; Salih, Z.M.A. Psychological effects of the COVID-19 pandemic: Perceived stress, anxiety, work-family imbalance, and coping strategies among healthcare professionals in Khartoum state hospitals, Sudan, 2021. Brain Behav. 2021, 11, e2318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Xiong, J.; Lipsitz, O.; Nasri, F.; Lui, L.M.W.; Gill, H.; Phan, L.; Chen-Li, D.; Iacobucci, M.; Ho, R.; Majeed, A.; et al. Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in the general population: A systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 277, 55–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Hu, J.; Huang, Y.; Liu, J.; Zheng, Z.; Xu, X.; Zhou, Y.; Wang, J. COVID-19 Related Stress and Mental Health Outcomes 1 Year After the Peak of the Pandemic Outbreak in China: The Mediating Effect of Resilience and Social Support. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 13, 828379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Hopf, D.; Schneider, E.; Aguilar-Raab, C.; Scheele, D.; Morr, M.; Klein, T.; Ditzen, B.; Eckstein, M. Loneliness and diurnal cortisol levels during COVID-19 lockdown: The roles of living situation, relationship status and relationship quality. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 15076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Qi, Y.; Lepe, A.; Almansa, J.; Ots, P.; de Kroon, M.L.A.; Lifelines Corona Research Initiative; Vrooman, J.C.; Reijneveld, S.A.; Brouwer, S. Increases in symptoms of depression and anxiety in adults during the initial phases of the COVID-19 pandemic are limited to those with less resources: Results from the Lifelines Cohort Study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2022, 154, 151–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Brown, J.P.; Martin, D.; Nagaria, Z.; Verceles, A.C.; Jobe, S.L.; Wickwire, E.M. Mental Health Consequences of Shift Work: An Updated Review. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2020, 22, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Angerer, P.; Schmook, R.; Elfantel, I.; Li, J. Night Work and the Risk of Depression. Dtsch. Arztebl. Int. 2017, 114, 404–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Wu, Q.J.; Sun, H.; Wen, Z.Y.; Zhang, M.; Wang, H.Y.; He, X.H.; Jiang, Y.T.; Zhao, Y.H. Shift work and health outcomes: An umbrella review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses of epidemiological studies. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2022, 18, 653–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Van den Berg, J.F.; Kivela, L.; Antypa, N. Chronotype and depressive symptoms in students: An investigation of possible mechanisms. Chronobiol. Int. 2018, 35, 1248–1261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Dimitrov, A.; Veer, I.M.; Kleeblatt, J.; Seyfarth, F.; Roenneberg, T.; Ising, M.; Uhr, M.; Keck, M.E.; Kramer, A.; Berger, M.; et al. Chronotype is associated with psychological well-being depending on the composition of the study sample. J. Health Psychol. 2018, 25, 1236–1247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Jansen, E.C.; Peterson, K.E.; O’Brien, L.; Hershner, S.; Boolani, A. Associations between Mental Workload and Sleep Quality in a Sample of Young Adults Recruited from a US College Town. Behav. Sleep Med. 2020, 18, 513–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Almojali, A.I.; Almalki, S.A.; Alothman, A.S.; Masuadi, E.M.; Alaqeel, M.K. The prevalence and association of stress with sleep quality among medical students. J. Epidemiol. Glob. Health 2017, 7, 169–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Rezaei, M.; Khormali, M.; Akbarpour, S.; Sadeghniiat-Hagighi, K.; Shamsipour, M. Sleep quality and its association with psychological distress and sleep hygiene: A cross-sectional study among pre-clinical medical students. Sleep Sci. 2018, 11, 274–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Han, K.S.; Kim, L.; Shim, I. Stress and sleep disorder. Exp. Neurobiol. 2012, 21, 141–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Ahrberg, K.; Dresler, M.; Niedermaier, S.; Steiger, A.; Genzel, L. The interaction between sleep quality and academic performance. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2012, 46, 1618–1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Correlation between sleep duration and intra-individual variation (_std) on (a) workdays and (b) free days.
Figure 1. Correlation between sleep duration and intra-individual variation (_std) on (a) workdays and (b) free days.
Brainsci 13 00818 g001
Figure 2. Correlation of sleep duration with sleep onset and sleep offset on (a) workdays and (b) free days. ** p < 0.01, **** p < 0.0001; slope of linear regression is significantly different from zero.
Figure 2. Correlation of sleep duration with sleep onset and sleep offset on (a) workdays and (b) free days. ** p < 0.01, **** p < 0.0001; slope of linear regression is significantly different from zero.
Brainsci 13 00818 g002
Figure 3. Correlation of intra-individual variation in sleep duration (SD_std) on (a) workdays and (b) free days with sleep onset and sleep offset. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; slope of linear regression is significantly different from zero.
Figure 3. Correlation of intra-individual variation in sleep duration (SD_std) on (a) workdays and (b) free days with sleep onset and sleep offset. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; slope of linear regression is significantly different from zero.
Brainsci 13 00818 g003
Figure 4. Correlation of sleep duration on workdays (green) and free days (blue) with (a) subjective workload and (b) subjective impact of high workload on sleep. * p < 0.05, slope of linear regression is significantly different from zero.
Figure 4. Correlation of sleep duration on workdays (green) and free days (blue) with (a) subjective workload and (b) subjective impact of high workload on sleep. * p < 0.05, slope of linear regression is significantly different from zero.
Brainsci 13 00818 g004
Figure 5. Correlation of intra-individual variation in sleep duration (SD_std) on workdays ((WDs) green) and free days ((FDs) blue) with (a) symptoms of depression and anxiety (PHQ4) and (b) living in the community (cohab). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; slope of linear regression is significantly different from zero.
Figure 5. Correlation of intra-individual variation in sleep duration (SD_std) on workdays ((WDs) green) and free days ((FDs) blue) with (a) symptoms of depression and anxiety (PHQ4) and (b) living in the community (cohab). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; slope of linear regression is significantly different from zero.
Brainsci 13 00818 g005
Figure 6. Graphic summary of relationships between sleep timing assessed by Fitbit wearable actigraphy and subjective measures for psychosocial stress assessed by questionnaires. Negative correlations are indicated in red, positive correlations are indicated in green. Time of sleep onset on workdays (WDs) and free days (FDs) is negatively correlated with sleep duration (SD) on WD and FD, respectively. Time of sleep offset on FD is positively correlated with SD on FD. SD on WD is positively correlated with SD on FD and negatively correlated with subjective workload (WL) and subjective impact of high workload on sleep (WLI). WL and WLI are positively correlated, and both are positively correlated with symptoms of anxiety and depression (PHQ4). PHQ4 is negatively correlated with living in the community (cohab). Intra-individual variation in sleep duration (SD_std) on workdays is positively correlated with sleep onset and offset on FD, SD_std on free days, and cohab. SD_std on FD is negatively correlated with PHQ4.
Figure 6. Graphic summary of relationships between sleep timing assessed by Fitbit wearable actigraphy and subjective measures for psychosocial stress assessed by questionnaires. Negative correlations are indicated in red, positive correlations are indicated in green. Time of sleep onset on workdays (WDs) and free days (FDs) is negatively correlated with sleep duration (SD) on WD and FD, respectively. Time of sleep offset on FD is positively correlated with SD on FD. SD on WD is positively correlated with SD on FD and negatively correlated with subjective workload (WL) and subjective impact of high workload on sleep (WLI). WL and WLI are positively correlated, and both are positively correlated with symptoms of anxiety and depression (PHQ4). PHQ4 is negatively correlated with living in the community (cohab). Intra-individual variation in sleep duration (SD_std) on workdays is positively correlated with sleep onset and offset on FD, SD_std on free days, and cohab. SD_std on FD is negatively correlated with PHQ4.
Brainsci 13 00818 g006
Table 1. Scoring and distribution of variables assessed by questionnaire. Standard deviation (std) shows the inter-individual variation.
Table 1. Scoring and distribution of variables assessed by questionnaire. Standard deviation (std) shows the inter-individual variation.
VariableScoreMean +/− stdTotal (%)
Cohabitation 0.64 +/− 0.48
  Living alone0 21 (36%)
  Living in the community1 40 (64%)
Workload 2.6 +/− 0.62
  Low1 1 (2%)
  Medium2 24 (41%
  High3 31 (52%)
  Very high4 3 (5%)
Workload impact on sleep 0.41 +/− 0.49
  No0 35 (59%)
  Yes1 24 (41%)
Anxiety and depression 3.18 +/− 2.56
  Normal0–2 29 (49%)
  Mild3–5 20 (34%)
  Moderate6–8 6 (10%)
  Severe9–12 4 (7%)
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

von Gall, C.; Muth, T.; Angerer, P. Sleep Duration on Workdays Is Correlated with Subjective Workload and Subjective Impact of High Workload on Sleep in Young Healthy Adults. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 818. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13050818

AMA Style

von Gall C, Muth T, Angerer P. Sleep Duration on Workdays Is Correlated with Subjective Workload and Subjective Impact of High Workload on Sleep in Young Healthy Adults. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(5):818. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13050818

Chicago/Turabian Style

von Gall, Charlotte, Thomas Muth, and Peter Angerer. 2023. "Sleep Duration on Workdays Is Correlated with Subjective Workload and Subjective Impact of High Workload on Sleep in Young Healthy Adults" Brain Sciences 13, no. 5: 818. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13050818

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop