Next Article in Journal
The Role of Ketamine in the Treatment of Bipolar Depression: A Scoping Review
Previous Article in Journal
Cultural Differences in Inhibitory Control: An ALE Meta-Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Musical Expertise and Executive Functions in Experienced Musicians

1
Institute of Music Physiology and Musicians’ Medicine, University of Music, Drama and Media Hannover, 30175 Hanover, Germany
2
Department of Neurology, Hannover Medical School, 30625 Hannover, Germany
3
Department of Neurology, Klinikum Rechts der Isar, Technical University of Munich; 80333 Munich, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(6), 908; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13060908
Submission received: 4 May 2023 / Revised: 30 May 2023 / Accepted: 1 June 2023 / Published: 4 June 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Behavioral Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
Extensive music practice has been suggested to enhance the development of cognitive abilities over and above musical expertise. Executive functions (EFs) have been particularly investigated, given their generalizability across different domains and their crucial role in almost all aspects of cognition. However, the relationship between musical expertise and EFs is still not completely understood, as several studies have reported conflicting results. The present study aims to investigate the relationship between musical expertise and EFs, determining which facets—if any—of EFs might be particularly relevant to extensive music practice. Thirty-five student pianists completed a set of neuropsychological tasks which assessed EFs (the Trail Making Task, Design Fluency, Numerical Stroop, and the Tower of London). They also performed a short musical excerpt inspired by the piano literature. Musical expertise was assessed by considering three parameters, namely the highest academic degree in music, the lifetime amount of music practice, and the quality of the sample-based musical performance. The results indicate that postgraduate piano students did not show advantages in EFs compared to undergraduate piano students. More extensive lifetime practice in music was solely associated with faster visual reaction times on the Numerical Stroop task. The Trail Making and Design Fluency scores were significant predictors of the quality of the sample-based musical performance. In conclusion, the present data suggests that EFs and the amount of music practice do not seem to be correlated in student pianists. Nevertheless, some facets of EFs and the quality of musical performance may share substantial amounts of variance.

1. Introduction

Music practice relies heavily on motor and cognitive abilities that are also beneficial to other everyday activities [1,2]. For instance, musicians seem to have superior fine motor control, bimanual coordination, auditory discrimination abilities and musical sophistication, compared to non-musicians [3,4,5,6]. These advantages are likely due to similarities between the behaviors tested in the laboratory and the skills trained in the practice rooms; they are usually considered near-transfer effects. Far transfer instead occurs when music training enhances performance in domains and activities which are not closely related to music, such as mathematical skills [7] and learning a second language [8]. Nevertheless, recent meta-analyses have questioned the reliability of far-transfer effects [9,10].

1.1. Executive Functions

Far-transfer effects are likely promoted by domain-general cognitive processes, which may be exploited and trained when practicing music. Executive functions (EFs) have been heavily investigated, given their generalizability across different cognitive domains and their widespread utility in everyday life [11].
EFs are a set of abilities which play a crucial role in almost all aspects of cognition. They are usually grouped under three core components: cognitive inhibition, working memory, and set-shifting [12]. In addition, these components form the basis for higher-order EFs such as planning, problem solving and cognitive fluency [13,14]. They develop during childhood and they reach a period of relative stability in early adulthood before they decline in older age [15]. In fact, age-related decline in EFs is already evident in the third and fourth decades of life [16,17,18].
Previous studies have shown that musicians perform better than non-musicians in neuropsychological tests measuring working memory [19], set-shifting [20], cognitive fluency [11] and cognitive inhibition [21]. However, these findings are rather inconsistent and several studies have reported conflicting results [13,22,23].

1.2. Causal Relationship

Therefore, the causal relationship between music training and EFs is still not completely understood. According to Okada and Slevc [24], their relationship can be represented by three alternative models. First, music training might enhance EFs by drawing on common cognitive resources. Alternatively, optimal EFs might be necessary for skilled music making, and thus musical expertise might be the result of a selection bias: only individuals who possess sufficient cognitive resources can successfully pursue music studies and careers as professional musicians. Finally, EFs and musical expertise might mutually influence each other. Therefore, pre-existing cognitive advantages may facilitate music making, and these individuals may further increase their advantage by training EFs through music practice.

1.3. Music Interventions

Causality is usually investigated through longitudinal study designs and music interventions in music-naïve individuals. These studies are usually focused on samples of children and older people. A recent meta-analysis [13] suggests that improvements in cognitive inhibition are the most common outcome of music making in children, while results related to working memory, set-shifting, planning and cognitive fluency are rather inconsistent. Similarly to children, older people seem to benefit from music lessons by improving their gross- and fine-motor coordination [25,26], processing speed and working memory [27], as well as speech perception [28].
Nevertheless, it is not clear which executive functions are specifically improved through music interventions. The findings are inconsistent, probably due to the variety of the experimental designs implemented [13]. Likewise, specific musical activities, which compose musicians’ professional routines, might affect distinct cognitive processes. Thus, knowledge about the cognitive resources that are recruited while making music might help better understand the relationships between musical expertise and EFs.

1.4. Experienced Musicians

Only a few studies have investigated the relationship between musical expertise and EFs in samples of experienced musicians. Slevc and colleagues [29] and Okada and Slevc [24] measured cognitive inhibition, working memory, and set-shifting abilities in both auditory and visual domains in a large sample of university-aged students. Notably, instead of dichotomizing participants into groups, i.e., musicians and non-musicians, they assessed musical expertise by implementing self-report measures of musicality as well as by melody and rhythmic discrimination tests. The results indicated a positive association between musical abilities, expertise and working memory. Set-shifting and cognitive inhibition were unrelated to musical abilities. However, the authors implemented measures of musical expertise that did not consider participants’ proficiency with the instrument, nor other fundamental aspects of musical expertise. In fact, according to a comprehensive review by Mishra [30], musical expertise is a complex construct, which can be assessed by considering three main parameters: academic degrees in music, the amount of practice achieved during lifetime, and the quality of musical performance. None of these parameters was measured in the mentioned studies.

1.5. Aim

The present project aims to investigate the relationship between executive functions, musical expertise, and musical performance in a sample of university-aged student pianists practicing a short musical excerpt. Musical expertise was assessed by considering multiple parameters, including performance quality. The results will allow us to determine which facets of EFs might be particularly relevant to music practice and performance. Given the lack of consistency in previous findings, we hypothesize a low level of generalizability of our results across measures of musical expertise.

2. Methods

2.1. Design

Experienced pianists completed a battery of neuropsychological tasks (NT), and practiced and performed a short musical excerpt. The experimental procedure and the results discussed here are part of a broader research project whose findings have been partially reported in Passarotto and colleagues [31].

2.2. Participants

Thirty-five healthy, right-handed pianists took part in the experiment. A total of 60% were females and 40% were males. Their mean age was 24.2 years (SD = 3.8, range 18–35 years). All participants were enrolled in music universities in northern Germany as undergraduate (N = 15) or postgraduate (N = 20) students, majoring in piano and classical music. Only 30 participants were considered for the analyses involving music performance quality scores: in fact, 5 participants were excluded, due to their previous experience with the musical task used for the experiment (N = 3) or for not following the guidelines given by the principal investigator (N = 2). Participation was voluntary, and it was compensated with EUR 50. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

2.3. Materials

The experiment comprised a battery of neuropsychological tasks, as well as three measures of musical expertise suggested by the literature [30]. Detailed descriptive statistics and operational definitions of individual NT parameters are reported in Table 1.

2.3.1. Design Fluency (DF)

Design Fluency (DF) is a neuropsychological task aimed at assessing cognitive fluency in generating visual patterns. The task was presented in its computerized version developed by Woods and colleagues [18]. During the task, five fixed points were displayed on the screen, and participants were asked to connect them by drawing designs of four continuous lines. The aim of the task was to draw as many unique designs as possible in 90 s, avoiding repetitions. Five supervised practice trials were provided at the beginning of the task.

2.3.2. Numerical Stroop (nSTROOP)

The numerical Stroop task aims to measure processing speed and response inhibition when evaluating visual stimuli. The nSTROOP task used in the present study was programmed according to the indications set out by Heine and colleagues [32]. However, the number of trials was halved, to reduce completion time to approximately 10 min. The task was divided into two parts, the numerical-magnitude and the physical-size comparison. In each part, participants were asked to decide which of the two numbers shown on the screen was either numerically or physically larger than the other. Participants were asked to answer as quickly and as accurately as possible by pressing two dedicated keyboard keys (Q and P, controlled by the index fingers of the right and the left hand, respectively), indicating either the stimulus on the right or on the left side of the screen. Stimuli were manipulated in terms of physical size and numerical magnitude. They were presented in three different conditions: congruent (i.e., greater magnitude corresponded to bigger physical size, 9 2), incongruent (i.e., smaller stimuli indicated a greater numerical value, 9 2 ), and neutral, where only one aspect of the stimuli was altered (i.e., only numerical magnitude was altered, 9 2). The task consisted of 96 trials evenly split into parts, that is, the numerical-magnitude and physical-size comparison (48 trials each), and the conditions, namely congruent, incongruent, and neutral (16 trials each). The order of the experimental phases and trials within each phase was randomized across participants. Each part of the task was introduced by ten supervised practice trials. For further details, see Heine and colleagues [32].

2.3.3. Trail Making Task (TMT)

The Trail Making Task measures visual attention and set-shifting. The present study used a computerized version of the task developed by Woods and colleagues [17]. TMT is divided into two conditions, Trail A and Trail B. In the first condition, 25 circles containing numbers ranging between 1 and 25 were displayed on the screen and participants were asked to click them in chronological order as fast as possible, avoiding errors. During Trail B, the 25 circles contained both numbers, ranging between 1 and 13, and letters, from “A” to “L”. Participants were asked to quickly click on the circles, following both numerical and alphabetical order and alternating between the two processes (i.e., 1–A–2–B–3–C). Both trails were preceded by five supervised practice trials.

2.3.4. Tower of London (TOL)

The Tower of London (Drexel University) (TOL) task is designed to assess planning and problem solving abilities [33]. The task was presented in its computerized version, programmed by Mueller and Piper [34]. During the task, two structures consisting of three columns of different length were displayed on the screen. Each structure contained three disks of different colors (blue, red, and green), with a different arrangement of the disks between the two structures. Participants were allowed to move the disks in only one of them, with the objective of matching the arrangement of the disks between the two structures by using the least number of moves. Moreover, participants were only allowed to move one disk at a time, and they could not pile up more disks in a column than it could accommodate. The task consisted of 15 subtasks of increasing difficulty. For a detailed description, see Culbertson and Zillmer [33].

2.3.5. Musical Expertise

Musical expertise was assessed based on three different parameters suggested in Mishra’s review [30]: academic degrees in music, lifetime practice and performance quality. In the first case, participants were divided into two groups: undergraduate (N = 15) and postgraduate (N = 20) pianists. Lifetime practice, i.e., the cumulative amount of practice during a lifetime measured in hours, was assessed through a dedicated scale developed by Butkovic and colleagues [35]. Performance quality was measured through the task and computerized assessment procedure presented in Passarotto and colleagues [31]. Note that performance quality is an inverted scale: high scores indicate poor musical performance, and vice versa.

2.4. Procedure

Participants took part in the experiment in a quiet room of approximately 9 square meters. At the beginning of the experimental procedure, they provided information about their demographics, musical background, and injury history. The experiment was organized into two main parts: the performance quality assessment and the neuropsychological assessment.
The performance quality assessment consisted of the same procedure as presented in Passarotto and colleagues [31]. In summary, participants practiced a short musical excerpt inspired by the piano literature on a MIDI keyboard. Participants did not have any previous experience with the musical task or the piece on which this was based. They were free to practice as much as they wanted, using the practice strategies they preferred. As shown in Figure 1, performance quality was assessed at baseline and at acquisition, i.e., right before and after the practice sessions. Time indicated at what timepoint each performance was recorded, taking individual baseline performances as a reference, when time = 0. During each test, musicians were asked to perform the musical excerpt five times, assisted by a metronome. MIDI recordings were used to objectively measure performance quality in terms of rhythmic accuracy, wrong and missed pitches, as well as loudness homogeneity through a computerized assessment procedure. For detailed information regarding the performance quality assessment procedure, see Passarotto and colleagues [31].
The neuropsychological assessment consisted of a battery of neuropsychological tasks aimed at investigating executive functions, as detailed above. All tasks were performed on a desktop computer. Participants were seated approximately 50 cm from a monitor with a resolution of 2560 × 1440 pixels and refresh rate of 144 Hz. They completed the tasks via a QWERTZ keyboard and a 1600 dpi corded mouse. The principal investigator (PI) provided the participants with detailed instructions for each task and supervised all introductory practice trials.
The order of the two experimental parts and of the individual NTs within the neuropsychological assessment phase were counterbalanced across participants. The study lasted approximately 2 h. Participants were encouraged to take short breaks at the end of each neuropsychological task and each experimental part.

2.5. Software and Database

The nSTROOP task was developed by the PI according to indications set out by Heine and colleagues [32], using the open-source software OpenSesame [36], version 3.3.11. Open-source versions of computerized DF and TMT tasks by Woods and colleagues [17,18] are publicly available at http://www.ebire.org/hcnlab (accessed in 1 October 2022). The computerized TOL task used in the study is part of the open source PEBL Psychological Test Battery, which can be downloaded at https://pebl.sourceforge.net/battery.html (accessed in 1 October 2022). Statistical analyses were performed in RStudio [37] and through the R-package brms [38].

2.6. Data Analyses

Between-group t-tests were run to assess differences in NT scores between undergraduate and postgraduate pianists. This allowed us to identify differences in executive functions due to academic degrees in music. Correlation matrices were used to investigate the relationship between participants’ age and their performance on the NTs.
Multivariate multiple regressions were run to investigate the relationship between lifetime practice and NT scores, while accounting for age differences. The model entered the NT parameters as dependent variables and lifetime practice, age and their interaction (lifetime practice*age) as predictors.
Bayesian mixed-effects regression models for repeated measures analyses were used to investigate the effect of time and NT parameters on performance quality scores. The models entered baseline and acquisition performance quality scores as criteria, and time, age and individual NT parameters, as well as their two- and three-way interactions, as fixed effects. Performer, with random slopes per time, was considered as a random effect. Time was coded as 0 for baseline performance quality scores, and corresponded to the length of the individual practice sessions for the acquisition performance quality scores. Thus, the main effects of the NT parameters and age indicated their influence on the model intercepts. The main effect of time quantified the participants’ average improvement in performance quality per minute of practice. Time*NT parameter interactions described the development of the NT effects during the experiment.
Welch t-tests were run to compare participants’ performances on the TMT and DF tasks to the normative data provided by Woods and colleagues [17,18].

3. Results

3.1. Academic Degrees

The first part of the analyses aimed at identifying differences in NT scores related to academic degrees in music. Table 1 reports detailed descriptive statistics for all neuropsychological tasks and task parameters considered in the study, subdivided into degree groups. The results showed significant group differences only in TMT scores. Undergraduate students’ performance was significantly better in terms of tmt_time_b (completion time of Trail B), tmt_time_b-a (difference in completion times between Trail B and Trail A), tmt_time_b/a (ratio between completion times of Trail B and Trail A) and tmt_errors_b-a (difference in errors between Trail B and Trail A) scores. Nevertheless, undergraduate students were significantly younger than postgraduate ones, suggesting a possible age-related bias. There were no significant differences in NT scores due to participants’ gender (p > 0.05). Thus, a post hoc multivariate multiple regression model was run to regress individual NT parameters on degree, age and degree*age interaction: when accounting for participants’ age, the effect of academic degrees on the NT scores was no longer significant (p > 0.05).

3.2. Lifetime Practice

Subsequently, the analyses focused on the relationship between lifetime practice and NT parameters. Table 2 reports the results of the multivariate multiple regressions analysis: when accounting for participants’ age, lifetime practice did not have significant effects on any of the NT parameters, with the only exception being stroop_rt_size (the average reaction time in the physical-size comparison part of the nSTROOP task). Thus, high amounts of lifetime practice were associated with faster reaction times in evaluating the physical size of visual stimuli. However, this did not generalize to the reaction times measured in the numeric-magnitude comparison part of the nSTROOP task. Age was a significant predictor in the model, and it was associated with higher TMT (tmt_time_b, tmt_time_b-a, tmt_time_b/a and tmt_errors_b-a) and nSTROOP scores (stroop_rt_size).

3.3. Performance Quality

The third part of the analyses aimed to investigate the effect of individual NT parameters and time on performance quality scores, by means of Bayesian mixed effects regression models. Age was also entered in the model as a predictor to account for possible age-related differences in NT scores. Table 3 reports the three most relevant models identified during the analyses: performance quality scores were predicted by either tmt_time_b/a (model 1), tmt_errors_b-a (model 2) or df_unique_designs (model 3). As shown in Figure 2, tmt_time_b/a and tmt_errors_b-a had meaningful main effects on performance quality scores: large differences in completion times and errors between Trail B and Trail A were associated with poor quality of musical performances. Df_unique_designs also had a meaningful effect on performance quality scores: a greater number of unique designs drawn in the DF task corresponded to poorer performance in the musical task.
The results showed a meaningful interaction between time and tmt_errors_b-a, suggesting that the effect of tmt_errors_b-a on performance quality scores was partially reduced over time. No other relevant time*NT interactions were identified. Furthermore, the results showed a significant interaction between tmt_errors_b-a and age, indicating that the effect of tmt_errors_b-a on performance quality scores increased with increasing age.

3.4. Normative Data Comparison

Participants’ performance on the TMT was similar to the normative data provided by Woods and colleagues (2015), with the only exception being the completion times for Trail A (M = 25.49 s, SD = 5.45) and Trail B (M = 39.81 s, SD = 10.39), which were significantly faster than the reported reference values (Trail A, M = 30.28 s, SD = 7.11 s, t(82.25) = 3.51, p < 0.01 and Trail B, M = 46.75 s, SD = 16.17, t(82.49) = 2.41, p < 0.05). Moreover, participants draw significantly more unique designs (M = 14.14, SD = 3.93) in the DF task than the reported reference values (M = 11.98, SD = 2.83, t(63.04) = 3.14, p < 0.01) collected by Woods and colleagues (2016).
Unfortunately, it was not possible to find compatible normative data published (i.e., with similar age and education level) for the nSTROOP and TOL tasks.

4. Discussions

The present study investigated the relationship between EFs and musical expertise in university-aged pianists.

4.1. Summary of the Results

The results indicate that postgraduate piano students did not show advantages in EFs compared to undergraduate piano students. More extensive lifetime practice in music was only associated with faster visual reaction times in the physical-size comparison part of the nSTROOP task. TMT and DF scores were significant predictors of the quality of the sample-based musical performance.
In detail, undergraduate students performed better on the TMT performance than postgraduate students. Nevertheless, they were also significantly younger and had cumulated a lower amount of practice during their lifetime. Thus, differences in NT scores were likely due to participants’ age, and not related to academic degrees per se. Subsequent analyses confirmed this hypothesis, showing that when accounting for participants’ age, the effect of academic degrees on the TMT scores was no longer significant. Bayesian mixed effects regression models showed that two parameters from the TMT (tmt_time_b/a, tmt_errors_b-a) and df_unique_designs scores from the DF were related to music performance quality. Large differences in completion times and errors between the Trail B and Trail A of the TMT were associated with poor quality of musical performances. More fluent productivity and a greater number of unique designs drawn in the DF task corresponded to poorer performance in the musical task. The same models identified a meaningful interaction between tmt_errors_b-a and age, suggesting a possible magnification effect of tmt_errors_b-a on music performance quality related to participants’ age.

4.2. Causal Relationship

The present results indicate that academic degrees in music and the amount of practice cumulated during a lifetime are not meaningful predictors of EFs in advanced learners. Musicians who practiced more and earned higher academic degrees in music performed similarly to their colleagues on the NT tasks, with the only exception being the physical-size comparison part of the nSTROOP task. TMT and DF scores were related to the technical proficiency in the instrument, indicating that specific EFs might provide an advantage in music playing. It could be argued that music playing can promote the development of those EFs that are mostly recruited during music performance and practice. However, the comparison with normative data from other studies does not support this hypothesis, as musicians’ performance in the NTs was not significantly different from the references provided. For these reasons, the present study does not seem to provide evidence for the effects of music training on the development of EFs.

4.3. Inconsistency across Expertise Measures

We investigated the association of EFs and musical expertise across three measures of expertise: academic degree in music, lifetime practice, and performance quality. Most of the meaningful results referred to the technical proficiency in the instrument (performance quality scores), evidencing a low level of generalizability of the results across these three measures of musical expertise. The complexity of assessing musical expertise is well known in the literature [30]. As expected, the measures included in this study likely captured different facets of expertise, leading to different results. The same argument might explain the incongruence between our results and the previous literature. For instance, Slevc and colleagues [29] and Okada and Slevc [24] did not find any significant association between musical abilities and set-shifting, but rather with working memory. However, the first study measured expertise in terms of music discrimination abilities, but it did not consider the motor aspect of music playing. The second study used a self-report measurement instrument that assesses participants’ own perception of musicianship, which can be independent of the level of music education attained [5]. Thus, we consider the present findings as complementary to, rather than conflicting with, the literature, as previous studies have considered different facets of musical expertise.

4.4. Cognitive Development in Advanced Musicians

Our results are also in conflict with findings from intervention studies on children and older people [1,25]. One possible explanation may be that the cognitive advantages due to music making do not generalize to high levels of musical expertise, due to ceiling effects in cognitive development. In other words, practicing music might be beneficial to cognitive abilities in the initial stages of learning a musical instrument, with a decreasing effect over time. Alternatively, this effect might depend on learners’ age: music making might be particularly beneficial only in life periods of significant cognitive growth and decline [15,39]. However, to the authors’ knowledge, the literature does not provide any evidence of music interventions on middle-aged adults (i.e., individuals between 20 and 50 years old) and life stages of cognitive stability. Therefore, it is not possible to evaluate the validity of this hypothesis.

4.5. Trail Making Task, nSTROOP and Design Fluency

The results showed that participants’ performance on the TMT and the physical-size comparison part of the nSTROOP task decreased with age. These findings are in line with previous studies [16,17] showing age-related increments in completion times for TMT and reaction times for nSTROOP from the third decade of life.
Participants’ performance on the TMT was similar to the normative data provided by Woods and colleagues [17], with the exception of the completion times for Trail A and Trail B, where musicians were significantly faster than the norm in completing the task but showed similar alternating-switch costs. Moreover, high amounts of practice during a lifetime were associated with faster reaction times in the physical-size comparison part of the nSTROOP task. These findings are in line with the literature showing that adult musicians have a faster auditory and visuospatial processing speed than non-musicians [11,40,41,42]. This is likely promoted by the extensive multisensory integration and temporal processing trained through music practice [43,44], which involve the rapid interpretation of visual stimuli into fine-grained movements.
Lower DF scores were linked to better music performance quality. This might be due to the musical task considered in the experiment, as participants were asked to play the musical excerpt as accurately as possible, performing movements with extreme regularity and little variability. Therefore, the ability to generate multiple geometric patterns while avoiding repetitions was likely detrimental to the objective of the musical task. Nevertheless, participants draw significantly more unique designs in the DF task than the norms provided by Woods and colleagues [18], in line with the literature [11]. Thus, it is important to consider that the present experiment does not represent music practice in its entirety and that design fluency might be beneficial to other aspects of music learning (i.e., expressivity and musicality).

4.6. Limitations

The present study has several limitations. The results might strongly depend on the musical task considered, as different musical activities might rely on different cognitive processes. For instance, creativity might increase the expressivity of a musical performance, which was not measured here.
The study did not account for possible interindividual differences in socio-economic background or in other variables which might influence executive functioning. Nevertheless, participants had comparable levels of education and musical expertise. Moreover, the study did not include any control group of amateurs or non-musicians, which would have allowed us to make broader inferences regarding participants’ performance in the nSTROOP and TOL tasks. Performance quality was measured only at two timepoints, which was insufficient for measuring the transient effects of individual cognitive abilities: it might be that individual EFs are recruited only at specific intermediate stages of learning a musical piece, rather than after only one practice session.

4.7. Future Developments

Music interventions on healthy, middle-aged adults would help clarify the effect of music training on executive functioning. Moreover, future studies might investigate the long-term effects of suboptimal executive functioning in musicians, specifically assessing whether these are a comorbidity of playing-related injuries.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the present data show that high amounts of music practice may not be associated with better executive functioning in early adulthood. Nevertheless, some facets of EFs and the quality of musical performance may share substantial amounts of variance, highlighting the importance of optimal cognitive functionality in music practice and performance.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.P., E.A. and B.K.; methodology, E.P. and B.K.; software, E.P.; validation, B.K., E.A. and A.L.; formal analysis, E.P.; investigation, E.P.; resources, E.A.; data curation, E.P.; writing—original draft preparation, E.P., B.K., E.A. and A.L.; writing—review and editing, E.P.; visualization, E.P.; supervision, E.A., B.K., A.L.; project administration, E.A. and E.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the Central Ethics Committee at Leibniz University Hannover (approval number EV LUH 01/2022, approved in July 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The database and the R script used in the study are publicly available at the following link: https://osf.io/u2bgt (accessed on 29 May 2023).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Frischen, U.; Schwarzer, G.; Degé, F. Music training and executive functions in adults and children: What role do hot executive functions play? Z. Für Erzieh. 2022, 25, 551–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Gaser, C.; Schlaug, G. Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 9240–9245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Chang, X.; Wang, P.; Zhang, Q.; Feng, X.; Zhang, C.; Zhou, P. The effect of music training on unimanual and bimanual responses. Music Sci. 2014, 18, 464–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Kuo, Y.-L.; Kutch, J.J.; Fisher, B.E. Relationship between interhemispheric inhibition and dexterous hand performance in musicians and non-musicians. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 11574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Müllensiefen, Gingras, Stewart the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (Gold-MSI). Available online: https://www.gold.ac.uk/music-mind-brain/gold-msi/ (accessed on 12 November 2019).
  6. Neves, L.; Correia, A.I.; Castro, S.L.; Martins, D.; Lima, C.F. Does music training enhance auditory and linguistic processing? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Behavioral and Brain Evidence. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2022, 140, 104777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Ribeiro, F.S.; Santos, F.H. Persistent effects of musical training on mathematical skills of children with developmental dyscalculia. Front. Psychol. 2020, 10, 2888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Talamini, F.; Grassi, M.; Toffalini, E.; Santoni, R.; Carretti, B. Learning a second language: Can music aptitude or music training have a role? Learn. Individ. Differ. 2018, 64, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bigand, E.; Tillmann, B. Near and far transfer: Is music special? Mem. Cognit. 2022, 50, 339–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Sala, G.; Gobet, F. Cognitive and academic benefits of music training with children: A multilevel meta-analysis. Mem. Cognit. 2020, 48, 1429–1441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zuk, J.; Benjamin, C.; Kenyon, A.; Gaab, N. Behavioral and neural correlates of executive functioning in musicians and non-musicians. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e99868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Doebel, S. Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 15, 942–956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Degé, F.; Frischen, U. The impact of music training on executive functions in childhood—A systematic review. Z. Für Erzieh. 2022, 25, 579–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Alwin, D.F.; Thomas, J.R.; Wray, L.A. Cognitive development and the life course: Growth, stability and decline. In Handbook of the Life Course: Volume II; Shanahan, M.J., Mortimer, J.T., Kirkpatrick Johnson, M., Eds.; Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 451–488. ISBN 978-3-319-20880-0. [Google Scholar]
  16. Williams, B.R.; Strauss, E.H.; Hultsch, D.F.; Hunter, M.A. Reaction time inconsistency in a spatial stroop task: Age-related differences through childhood and adulthood. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2007, 14, 417–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Woods, D.L.; Wyma, J.M.; Herron, T.J.; Yund, E.W. The effects of aging, malingering, and traumatic brain injury on computerized trail-making test performance. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0124345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Woods, D.L.; Wyma, J.M.; Herron, T.J.; Yund, E.W. A computerized test of design fluency. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0153952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Talamini, F.; Carretti, B.; Grassi, M. The working memory of musicians and nonmusicians. Music Percept. 2016, 34, 183–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Slama, H.; Rebillon, E.; Kolinsky, R. Expertise and cognitive flexibility: A musician’s tale. J. Cult. Cogn. Sci. 2017, 1, 119–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Sævild Ree, A. Visual and Auditory Response Inhibition in Musicians and Non-Musicians. Master’s Thesis, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  22. Boebinger, D.; Evans, S.; Rosen, S.; Lima, C.F.; Manly, T.; Scott, S.K. Musicians and non-musicians are equally adept at perceiving masked speech. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2015, 137, 378–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Schellenberg, E.G. Examining the association between music lessons and intelligence. Br. J. Psychol. 2011, 102, 283–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Okada, B.M.; Slevc, L.R. Individual differences in musical training and executive functions: A latent variable approach. Mem. Cognit. 2018, 46, 1076–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Bugos, J.A. The effects of bimanual coordination in music interventions on executive functions in aging adults. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Maclean, L.M.; Brown, L.J.E.; Astell, A.J. The effect of rhythmic musical training on healthy older adults’ gait and cognitive function. Gerontologist 2014, 54, 624–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Degé, F.; Kerkovius, K. The effects of drumming on working memory in older adults. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018, 1423, 242–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Worschech, F.; Marie, D.; Jünemann, K.; Sinke, C.; Krüger, T.H.C.; Großbach, M.; Scholz, D.S.; Abdili, L.; Kliegel, M.; James, C.E.; et al. Improved speech in noise perception in the elderly after 6 months of musical instruction. Front. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 696240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Slevc, L.R.; Davey, N.S.; Buschkuehl, M.; Jaeggi, S.M. Tuning the mind: Exploring the connections between musical ability and executive functions. Cognition 2016, 152, 199–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Mishra, J. Musical Expertise; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2019; Volume The Oxford Handbook of Expertise, ISBN 978-0-19-183705-0. [Google Scholar]
  31. Passarotto, E.; Worschech, F.; Altenmüller, E. The effects of anxiety on practice behaviors and performance quality in expert pianists. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1152900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Heine, A.; Tamm, S.; De Smedt, B.; Schneider, M.; Thaler, V.; Torbeyns, J.; Stern, E.; Verschaffel, L.; Jacobs, A. The numerical stroop effect in primary school children: A comparison of low, normal, and high achievers. Child Neuropsychol. J. Norm. Abnorm. Dev. Child. Adolesc. 2010, 16, 461–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Culbertson, W.C.; Zillmer, E.A. The tower of londondx: A standardized approach to assessing executive functioning in children. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1998, 13, 285–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Mueller, S.T.; Piper, B.J. The psychology experiment building language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. J. Neurosci. Methods 2014, 222, 250–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Butkovic, A.; Ullén, F.; Mosing, M.A. Personality related traits as predictors of music practice: Underlying environmental and genetic influences. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 74, 133–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Mathôt, S.; Schreij, D.; Theeuwes, J. OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2012, 44, 314–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R; RStudio: Boston, MA, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  38. Bürkner, P.-C. Advanced Bayesian Multilevel Modeling with the R Package Brms. arXiv 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Chen, J.; Scheller, M.; Wu, C.; Hu, B.; Peng, R.; Liu, C.; Liu, S.; Zhu, L.; Chen, J. The relationship between early musical training and executive functions: Validation of effects of the sensitive period. Psychol. Music 2022, 50, 86–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Amer, T.; Kalender, B.; Hasher, L.; Trehub, S.E.; Wong, Y. Do older professional musicians have cognitive advantages? PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e71630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Bugos, J.; Mostafa, W. Musical training enhances information processing speed. Bull. Counc. Res. Music Educ. 2011, 187, 7–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Helmbold, N.; Rammsayer, T.; Altenmüller, E. Differences in primary mental abilities between musicians and nonmusicians. J. Individ. Differ. 2005, 26, 74–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Paraskevopoulos, E.; Kuchenbuch, A.; Herholz, S.C.; Pantev, C. Multisensory integration during short-term music reading training enhances both uni- and multisensory cortical processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 26, 2224–2238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Rammsayer, T.H.; Buttkus, F.; Altenmüller, E. Musicians do better than nonmusicians in both auditory and visual timing tasks. Music Percept. 2012, 30, 85–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Timeline of the performance quality assessment procedure and descriptive statistics for time and performance quality. Note: N = 30; a participants were allowed to practice as much as they wanted. The duration of the practice sessions (in parentheses) varied across individuals. Performance quality is an inverted scale: high scores indicate poor musical performance, and vice versa.
Figure 1. Timeline of the performance quality assessment procedure and descriptive statistics for time and performance quality. Note: N = 30; a participants were allowed to practice as much as they wanted. The duration of the practice sessions (in parentheses) varied across individuals. Performance quality is an inverted scale: high scores indicate poor musical performance, and vice versa.
Brainsci 13 00908 g001
Figure 2. The effect of NT parameters and time on performance quality scores. Notes: The figure reports model-estimated effects of NT parameters and time on performance quality scores. For illustration purposes, performance quality scores are predicted by considering NT parameters’ values at either one SD below or above the mean of the overall sample. Performance quality is an inverted scale (i.e., high scores indicate poor musical performance). Detailed descriptive statistics and effect estimates are reported in Table 1 and Table 3.
Figure 2. The effect of NT parameters and time on performance quality scores. Notes: The figure reports model-estimated effects of NT parameters and time on performance quality scores. For illustration purposes, performance quality scores are predicted by considering NT parameters’ values at either one SD below or above the mean of the overall sample. Performance quality is an inverted scale (i.e., high scores indicate poor musical performance). Detailed descriptive statistics and effect estimates are reported in Table 1 and Table 3.
Brainsci 13 00908 g002
Table 1. NT scores from undergraduate (N = 15) and postgraduate (N = 20) students, as well as from the complete sample (N = 35).
Table 1. NT scores from undergraduate (N = 15) and postgraduate (N = 20) students, as well as from the complete sample (N = 35).
TaskParameterDescriptionUndergraduatePostgraduateOverall (SD)
TMTtmt_time_acompletion time of Trail A (in seconds)25.92025.17925.497 (5.453)
tmt_time_bcompletion time of Trail B (in seconds)35.47143.080 *39.819 (10.394)
tmt_time_b-adifference in completion times between Trail B and Trail A (in seconds)9.55117.901 **14.323 (8.930)
tmt_time_b/aratio between completion times of Trail B and Trail A1.3851.731 **1.583 (0.372)
tmt_errors_b-adifference in errors a between Trail B and Trail A−0.2000.650 *0.286 (1.126)
TOLtol_movestotal number of moves across all subtasks110.06796.947102.571 (27.349)
tol_timecompletion time of the TOL task (in minutse)10.22810.57710.427 (4.815)
nSTROOPstroop_rt_numeric baverage reaction time in comparing numeric magnitudes (in msec)515.128527.381522.130 (76.631)
stroop_rt_size baverage reaction time in comparing physical sizes (in msec)373.150378.501376.208 (48.114)
stroop_delta_rt bdifference in reaction times between incongruent and congruent conditions (in msec)38.57335.94137.069 (19.396)
stroop_delta_errdifference in errors a between incongruent and congruent conditions1.7331.5011.600 (1.418)
DFdf_unique_designsnumber of unique designs drawn 15.06713.4514.142 (3.934)
df_repeated_absnumber of designs which were drawn more than once0.9330.8010.857 (1.331)
df_repeated_pctratio of repeated trials to the overall number of designs0.0500.0460.047 (0.068)
Ageparticipants’ age (in years)21.6026.15 ***24.20 (3.826)
Years of practiceyears of music learning14.6719.40 ***17.37 (3.89)
Lifetime practicecumulative amount of practice hours during lifetime13,09320,451 *17,298 (9934)
Notes: N = 35; * between groups t-test significant at p < 0.05, ** between groups t-test significant at p < 0.01, *** between groups t-test significant at p < 0.001, a number of wrong responses, b measured on correct trials only. TMT = Trail Making Task, TOL = Tower of London, nSTROOP = numerical STROOP, DF = Design Fluency. When accounting for participants’ age, the results did not indicate any significant difference in NT scores related to academic degrees (p > 0.05).
Table 2. The effect of age and lifetime practice on NT scores.
Table 2. The effect of age and lifetime practice on NT scores.
TaskDVInterceptAgeLifetime PracticeAge: Lifetime Practice
TMTtmt_time_a0.086−0.057−0.089−0.332
tmt_time_b0.0640.395 *−0.230−0.247
tmt_time_b-a0.0690.380 *−0.162−0.265
tmt_time_b/a−0.0210.475 *−0.1530.080
tmt_errors_b-a0.0220.494 **−0.213−0.085
TOLtol_moves0.049−0.301−0.022−0.187
tol_time0.027−0.2010.274−0.104
nSTROOPstroop_rt_numeric a0.0320.258−0.247−0.124
stroop_rt_size a0.1600.337 *−0.358 *−0.617 *
stroop_delta_rt a0.0210.027−0.093−0.079
stroop_delta_err0.023−0.2400.233−0.090
DFdf_unique_designs−0.0950.114−0.1690.366
df_repeated_abs0.0680.094−0.177−0.261
df_repeated_pct0.0970.130−0.185−0.372
Notes: N = 35; * beta coefficient is significant at p < 0.05, ** beta coefficient is significant at p < 0.01, a reaction time measurements have been log-transformed to compensate for their skewness. TMT = Trail Making Task, TOL = Tower of London, nSTROOP = numerical STROOP, DF = Design Fluency. For the analyses, all dependent variables and predictors have been standardized across participants. For a description of each task parameter, see Table 1.
Table 3. The effect of NT parameters and time on performance quality scores.
Table 3. The effect of NT parameters and time on performance quality scores.
Model 1Model 2Model 3
Fixed EffectsEstimate [95% CI]Estimate [95% CI]Estimate [95% CI]
Intercept0.36 [0.06, 0.65]0.37 [0.07, 0.66]0.37 [0.07, 0.66]
time−0.05 [−0.07, −0.03]−0.05 [−0.08, −0.03]−0.05 [−0.08, −0.03]
age−0.06 [−0.39, 0.28]−0.01 [−0.30, 0.26]−0.01 [−0.30, 0.26]
time: age0.00 [−0.02, 0.03]0.01 [−0.01, 0.03]0.01 [−0.01, 0.03]
tmt_time_b/a0.30 [0.00, 0.62]--
time: tmt_time_b/a−0.01 [−0.04, 0.02]--
age: tmt_time_b/a0.14 [−0.11, 0.40]--
time: age: tmt_time_b/a0.01 [−0.01, 0.03]--
tmt_errors_b-a-0.57 [0.26, 0.87]-
time: tmt_errors_b-a-−0.03 [−0.05, −0.01]-
age: tmt_errors_b-a-0.38 [0.00, 0.78]-
time: age: tmt_errors_b-a-0.01 [−0.02, 0.04]-
df_unique_designs--0.35 [0.04, 0.68]
time: df_unique_designs--−0.01 [−0.03, 0.01]
age: df_unique_designs--−0.01 [−0.36, 0.34]
time: age: df_unique_designs--0.00 [−0.02, 0.03]
Random Effects
Performer:
Intercept0.41 [0.23, 0.59]0.41 [0.24, 0.58]0.41 [0.24, 0.59]
time0.01 [0.00, 0.03]0.01 [0.00, 0.03]0.02 [0.00, 0.03]
cor (Intercept, time) a−0.17 [−0.79, 0.60]−0.10 [−0.76, 0.65]−0.11 [−0.75, 0.62]
residuals0.76 [0.59, 0.96]0.69 [0.52, 0.88]0.78 [0.62, 0.98]
Coefficients of determination
Conditional R20.54 [0.39, 0.69]0.60 [0.45, 0.73]0.48 [0.33, 0.63]
Marginal R20.45 [0.31, 0.56]0.48 [0.36, 0.59]0.39 [0.25, 0.50]
Notes: N = 30; a correlation between random effects. Performance quality was predicted by either tmt_time_b/a (model 1), tmt_errors_b-a (model 2) or df_unique_designs (model 3). Performance quality is an inverted scale (i.e., high scores indicate poor musical performance). Age, tmt_time_b/a, tmt_errors_b-a and df_unique_designs were standardized across participants. The table reports fixed and random effects, followed with 95% credible intervals in square brackets [ ]. For a description of each NT parameter, see Table 1.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Passarotto, E.; Kopp, B.; Lee, A.; Altenmüller, E. Musical Expertise and Executive Functions in Experienced Musicians. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 908. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13060908

AMA Style

Passarotto E, Kopp B, Lee A, Altenmüller E. Musical Expertise and Executive Functions in Experienced Musicians. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(6):908. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13060908

Chicago/Turabian Style

Passarotto, Edoardo, Bruno Kopp, André Lee, and Eckart Altenmüller. 2023. "Musical Expertise and Executive Functions in Experienced Musicians" Brain Sciences 13, no. 6: 908. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13060908

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop