Procedural Memory Deficits in Preschool Children with Developmental Language Disorder in a Spanish-Speaking Population
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Stimuli and Procedure
2.2.1. SRT Task
2.2.2. Working Memory Task (Covariable)
3. Results
3.1. Reaction Time
3.1.1. General Analysis RT
3.1.2. Group Analysis RT
3.2. Accuracy
3.2.1. General Analysis Accuracy
3.2.2. Group Analysis Accuracy
3.3. Correlations
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Bishop, D.V.; Snowling, M.J.; Thomson, P.A.; Greenhalgh, T. CATALISE-2 Consortium. Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 1068–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Norbury, C.F.; Gooch, D.; Wray, C.; Baird, G.; Charman, T.; Simonoff, E.; Vamvakas, G.; Pickles, A. The impact of nonverbal ability on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder: Evidence from a population study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2016, 57, 1247–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Andreu, L.; Igualada, A.; Ahufinger, N.; Sanz-Torrent, M. La situación del trastorno específico del lenguaje en los países hispanohablantes. Rev. Investig. Logop. 2021, 12, e74552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leonard, L.B. Time-related grammatical use by children with SLI across languages: Beyond tense. Int. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2015, 17, 545–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Allende, N.; Alfaro-Faccio, P.; Góngora-Costa, B.; Alvarado, C.; Marfull-Villanueva, D. Perfil sintáctico de niños con y sin Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje: Un análisis descriptivo. Rev. Signos 2020, 53, 619–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanz-Torrent, M.; Andreu, L.L. La morfología en los niños y niñas con TDL. In El Trastorno del Desarrollo del Lenguaje (TDL); Sanz-Torrent, M., Andreu, L.L., Eds.; Pirámide: Madrid, Spain, 2023; pp. 115–144. [Google Scholar]
- Freudenthal, D.; Ramscar, M.; Leonard, L.B.; Pine, J.M. Simulating the Acquisition of Verb Inflection in Typically Developing Children and Children with Developmental Language Disorder in English and Spanish. Cogn. Sci. 2021, 45, e12945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Urrutia, M.; Roa, M. Processing speed of morphological understanding of verbs in preschool children with specific language disorders and its relationship with the inhibitory control. Rev. Investig. Logop. 2020, 10, 53–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ahufinger, N.; Ferinu, L.; Sanz-Torrent, M.; Andreu, L.; Evans, J.L. Statistical word learning in Catalan-Spanish and English-speaking children with and without developmental language disorder. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2022, 57, 42–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Archibald, L.M.; Gathercole, S.E. The complexities of complex memory span: Storage and processing deficits in specific language impairment. J. Mem. Lang. 2007, 57, 177–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lum, J.A.; Ullman, M.T.; Morgan, A.T.; Conti-Ramsden, G. Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): A meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex 2014, 51, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kapa, L.L.; Erikson, J.A. The Relationship between Word Learning and Executive Function in Preschoolers with and without Developmental Language Disorder. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2020, 63, 2293–2307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ullman, M.T. Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition 2004, 92, 231–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ullman, M.T.; Pierpont, E.I. Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex 2005, 41, 399–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ullman, M.T.; Earle, F.S.; Walenski, M.; Janacsek, K. The Neurocognition of Developmental Disorders of Language. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2020, 71, 389–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Willingham, D.B.; Salidis, J.; Gabrieli, J.D. Direct comparison of neural systems mediating conscious and unconscious skill learning. J. Neurophysiol. 2002, 88, 1451–1460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poldrack, R.A.; Clark, J.; Paré-Blagoev, E.J.; Shohamy, D.; Creso Moyano, J.; Myers, C.; Gluck, M.A. Interactive memory systems in the human brain. Nature 2001, 414, 546–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ullman, M.T. The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2001, 30, 37–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nissen, M.J.; Bullemer, P. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cogn. Psychol. 1987, 19, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, G.M.; Lum, J.A.G.; Ullman, M.T. A meta-analysis and meta-regression of serial reaction time task performance in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 2014, 28, 945–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Janacsek, K.; Shattuck, K.F.; Tagarelli, K.M.; Lum, J.A.; Turkeltaub, P.E.; Ullman, M.T. Sequence learning in the human brain: A functional neuroanatomical meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies. NeuroImage 2020, 207, 116387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hsu, H.J.; Bishop, D.V. Sequence-specific procedural learning deficits in children with specific language impairment. Dev. Sci. 2014, 17, 352–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vuolo, J.; Goffman, L.; Zelaznik, H.N. Deficits in coordinative bimanual timing control in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 393–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zelaznik, H.N.; Goffman, L. General Motor abilities and timing behavior in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2010, 53, 283–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, J.C.; Tomblin, J.B. Procedural Learning and Individual Differences in Language. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2015, 11, 215–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, K.M.; Nelson, C.A. Serial reaction time learning in preschool- and school-age children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2001, 79, 364–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lum, J.A.; Kidd, E.; Sarah, D.; Conti-Ramsden, G. Longitudinal study of declarative and procedural memory in primary school-aged children. Aust. J. Psychol. 2010, 62, 139–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamrick, P.; Lum, J.A.G.; Ullman, M.T. Child first language and adult second language are both tied to general-purpose learning systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 1487–1492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gabriel, A.; Maillart, C.; Guillaume, M.; Stefaniak, N.; Meulemans, T. Exploration of serial structure procedural learning in children with language impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 336–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gabriel, A.; Stefaniak, N.; Maillart, C.; Schmitz, X.; Meulemans, T. Procedural visual learning in children with specific language impairment. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2012, 21, 329–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gabriel, A.; Maillart, C.; Stefaniak, N.; Lejeune, C.; Desmottes, L.; Meulemans, T. Procedural learning in specific language impairment: Effects of sequence complexity. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2013, 19, 264–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gabriel, A.; Meulemans, T.; Parisse, C.; Maillart, C. Procedural learning across modalities in French speaking children with specific language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2015, 36, 747–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.; Emerson, M.; Witzki, H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Larson, C.; Ellis Weismer, S. Working Memory Performance in Children with Developmental Language Disorder: The Role of Domain. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2022, 65, 1906–1920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montgomery, J.; Evans, J.; Gillam, R. Relation of auditory attention and complex sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: A preliminary study. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2009, 30, 123–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vugs, B.; Hendriks, M.; Cuperus, J.; Knoors, H.; Verhoeven, L. Developmental Associations between Working Memory and Language in Children with Specific Language Impairment: A Longitudinal Study. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 3284–3294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackson, E.; Leitão, S.; Claessen, M.; Boyes, M. Working, declarative, and procedural memory in children with developmental language disorder. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2020, 63, 4162–4178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vugs, B.; Cuperus, J.; Hendriks, M.; Verhoeven, L. Visuospatial working memory in specific language impairment: A meta-analysis. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 2586–2597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hick, R.; Botting, N.; Conti-Ramsden, G. Short-term memory and vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2005, 47, 532–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sansavini, A.; Favilla, M.E.; Guasti, M.T.; Marini, A.; Millepiedi, S.; Di Martino, M.V.; Vecchi, S.; Battajon, N.; Bertolo, L.; Capirci, O.; et al. Developmental Language Disorder: Early Predictors, Age for the Diagnosis, and Diagnostic Tools. A Scoping Review. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MINEDUC. Ministerio de Educación de Chile. Informe de Necesidades Educativas Especiales 2017. 2018. Available online: https://www.mineduc.cl/wp-content/uploads/sites/19/2017/10/INFORME-NACIONAL.pdf (accessed on 13 February 2024).
- MINEDUC. Ministerio de Educación de Chile. Decreto Exento N°170. Establece Normativa para la Atención Educativa de Estudiantes que Presentan Necesidades Educativas Especiales Asociadas a Discapacidad. 2010. Available online: https://www.bcn.cl/leychile/navegar?idNorma=1012570 (accessed on 13 February 2024).
- Ramos, M.; Ramos, J.; Hresko, D.; Reid, K.; Hammill, D. TELD-3: S. Test of Early Language Development; PRO-ED, Inc.: Austin, TX, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [E-Prime 3.0]. 2016. Available online: https://support.pstnet.com/ (accessed on 13 February 2024).
- Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [Chronos]. 2016. Available online: https://pstnet.com/products/chronos/ (accessed on 13 February 2024).
- Tenorio, M.; Arango, P.; Aparicio, A.; Rosas, R. TENI Test de Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil. Manual de Administración y Corrección; Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile-Cedeti UC: Santiago, Chile, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Field, A. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics; Sage Publications: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck, M.W.; Calvo, M.G. Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cogn. Emot. 1992, 6, 409–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, G.; Melby-Lervåg, M.; Hulme, C. Is a procedural learning deficit a causal risk factor for developmental language disorder or dyslexia? A meta-analytic review. Dev. Psychol. 2021, 57, 749–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Conti-Ramsden, G.; Ullman, M.T.; Lum, J.A. The relation between receptive grammar and procedural, declarative, and working memory in specific language impairment. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hill, E.L. Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2001, 36, 149–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barsalou, L. Perceptual Symbol Systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 1999, 22, 577–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barsalou, L. Grounded Cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008, 59, 617–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zwaan, R.A.; Taylor, L.J. Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in language comprehension. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2006, 135, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glenberg, A.; Kaschak, M. Grounding language in action. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2002, 9, 558–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruíz, D.; Urrutia, M.; Alarcón, P.; Marrero, H. Time comprehension through space: A research study on induced plasticity in children with developmental language disorder. RLFA 2022, 42, 24–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable | DLD (n = 30) | TD (n = 30) | Comparison | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | Min–Max | M | SD | Min–Max | ||
Age (months) | 55.03 | 3.85 | 48–60 | 56.17 | 2.52 | 49–60 | t = −1.35 p = 0.18 |
TELD3-S rec | 82.06 | 5.36 | 68–91 | 100.8 | 5.59 | 91–112 | t = −13.24 p ≤ 0.001 |
TELD3-S exp | 82.47 | 5.85 | 67–93 | 96.3 | 5.97 | 82–109 | t = −9.06 p ≤ 0.001 |
TELD3-S total | 164.53 | 3.48 | 156–169 | 197.1 | 5.42 | 185–204 | t = −27.682 p ≤ 0.001 |
DLD Group | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Block 1 | Block 2 | Block 3 | Block 4 | Block 5 | |
Mean | 91.233 | 93.600 | 94.433 | 95.367 | 95.733 |
Standard error of the mean | 1.588 | 1.318 | 1.111 | 1.161 | 0.971 |
Standard deviation | 8.696 | 7.218 | 6.084 | 6.359 | 5.317 |
Skewness | −2.287 | −3.062 | −3.375 | −3.943 | −2.836 |
Standard error of skewness | 0.427 | 0.427 | 0.427 | 0.427 | 0.427 |
Minimum | 58 | 63 | 67 | 65 | 73 |
Maximum | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 |
TD Group | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Block 1 | Block 2 | Block 3 | Block 4 | Block 5 | |
Mean | 98.133 | 98.267 | 99.300 | 99.133 | 97.633 |
Standard error of the mean | 0.317 | 0.439 | 0.263 | 0.428 | 0.499 |
Standard deviation | 1.737 | 2.406 | 1.442 | 2.345 | 2.735 |
Skewness | −0.981 | −1.881 | −2.120 | −3.008 | −1.590 |
Standard error of skewness | 0.427 | 0.427 | 0.427 | 0.427 | 0.427 |
Minimum | 93 | 90 | 95 | 90 | 90 |
Maximum | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 |
Variable | DLD Accuracy | TD Accuracy | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Block 1 | Block 2 | Block 3 | Block 4 | Block 5 | Block 1 | Block 2 | Block 3 | Block 4 | Block 5 | |
TELD-3: S rec | 0.431 * | 0.399 * | 0.399 | 0.407 * | 0.348 | 0.070 | −0.121 | −0.236 | 0.005 | 0.128 |
TELD-3: S exp | −0.233 | −0.213 | −0.089 | −0.164 | −0.162 | 0.059 | 0.273 | 0.001 | 0.196 | 0.026 |
TELD-3: S total | 0.271 | 0.257 | 0.371 * | 0.351 | 0.263 | 0.138 | 0.175 | −0.242 | 0.221 | 0.161 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sanhueza, S.; Urrutia, M.; Marrero, H. Procedural Memory Deficits in Preschool Children with Developmental Language Disorder in a Spanish-Speaking Population. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 198. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030198
Sanhueza S, Urrutia M, Marrero H. Procedural Memory Deficits in Preschool Children with Developmental Language Disorder in a Spanish-Speaking Population. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(3):198. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030198
Chicago/Turabian StyleSanhueza, Soraya, Mabel Urrutia, and Hipólito Marrero. 2024. "Procedural Memory Deficits in Preschool Children with Developmental Language Disorder in a Spanish-Speaking Population" Brain Sciences 14, no. 3: 198. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030198
APA StyleSanhueza, S., Urrutia, M., & Marrero, H. (2024). Procedural Memory Deficits in Preschool Children with Developmental Language Disorder in a Spanish-Speaking Population. Brain Sciences, 14(3), 198. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030198