Language Distance Moderates the Effect of a Mixed-Language Environment on New-Word Learning for 4-Year-Old Children
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Vocabulary Learning in Children in a Mixed-Language Environment
1.2. The Moderating Effect of Language Distance on the Influence of Mixed-Language Environments on Vocabulary Learning
1.3. Hypotheses of the Present Study
2. Experiment 1: The Effect of a Mixed-Language Environment on New-Word Learning in 4-Year-Old Children
2.1. Materials and Methods
2.1.1. Purpose
2.1.2. Participants
2.1.3. Design
2.1.4. Materials
2.1.5. Procedure
2.2. Results
3. Experiment 2: The Effect of Language Distance on the Effectiveness of Vocabulary Acquisition
3.1. Materials and Methods
3.1.1. Purpose
3.1.2. Participants
3.1.3. Design
3.1.4. Materials
3.1.5. Procedure
3.2. Results
3.3. Combined Analysis of Experiments 1 and 2
4. Discussion
4.1. The Inhibitory Effect of Mixed-Language Environments
4.2. Moderating Effects of Language Distance on Inhibition
4.3. Research Limitations
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Antón, E.; Thierry, G.; Goborov, A.; Anasagasti, J.; Duñabeitia, J.A. Testing Bilingual Educational Methods: A Plea to End the Language-Mixing Taboo. Lang. Learn. 2016, 66, 29–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Myers-Scotton, C. Social Motivations for Codeswitching: Evidence from Africa; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2023; pp. 151–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosjean, F. Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 1998, 1, 131–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stymne, S. Evaluating Word Embeddings for Indonesian-English Code-mixed Text Based on Synthetic Data. In Proceedings of the LREC 2020 Workshop, Marseille, France, 11–15 May 2020; European Language Resources Association (ELRA): Marseille, France, 2020; pp. 26–35. [Google Scholar]
- Pratapa, A.; Choundhury, M.; Sitaram, S. Word Embeddings for Code-mixed Language Processing. In Proceedings of the 2018 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, EMNLP 2018, Brussels, Belgium, 31 October–4 November 2018; Association for Computational Linguistics: Brussels, Belgium, 2020; pp. 3067–3072. [Google Scholar]
- Ningsih, O.S.; Setiawan, T. Code Mixing and Code Switching in the “Yowis Ben” Movie: Sociolinguistic Study. Int. J. Linguist. Lit. Transl. 2021, 4, 14–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poplack, S. Sometimes I’ll start a sentence in Spanish Y TERMINO EN ESPAÑOL: Toward a typology of code-switching. Linguistics 1980, 18, 581–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muysken, P. Bilingual speech: A typology of code-mixing. Lang. Soc. 2000, 31, 621–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cook, V. Using the First Language in the Classroom. Can. Mod. Lang. Rev. 2001, 57, 402–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bullock, B.E.; Toribio, A.J.E. The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Code-Switching; Cambridge University Press (CUP): Cambridge, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Chaer, A.; Agustina, L. Sosiolinguistik: Perkenalan Awal; Rineka Cipta: Jakarta, Indonesia, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Romaine, S. Bilingualism, 2nd ed.; Blackwell Publishers: Malden, MA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Appel, R.; Muysken, P.C. Language Contact and Bilingualism; Walter de Gruyter GmbH: Berlin, Germany, 2006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev. Rev. 2006, 26, 55–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, M.; Farrar, M.J. Object permanence and relational words: A lexical training study. J. Child Lang. 1986, 13, 495–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Byers-Heinlein, K. Parental language mixing: Its measurement and the relation of mixed input to young bilingual children’s vocabulary size. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2013, 16, 32–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Byers-Heinlein, K.; Jardak, A.; Fourakis, E.; Lew-Williams, C. Effects of language mixing on bilingual children’s word learning. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2022, 25, 55–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ervin-Tripp, S.; Reyes, I. Child codeswitching and adult content contrasts. Int. J. Biling. 2005, 9, 69–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhl, P.K. Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 5, 831–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Werker, J.F.; Yeung, H.H.; Yoshida, K.A. How Do infants become experts at native-speech perception? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 221–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yow, W.Q.; Tan, J.S.H.; Flynn, S. Code-switching as a marker of linguistic competence in bilingual children. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 1075–1090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Birdsong, D. Age and Second Language Acquisition and Processing: A Selective Overview. Lang. Learn. 2006, 56, 9–49. [Google Scholar]
- Place, S.; Hoff, E. Properties of Dual Language Exposure That Influence 2-Year-Olds’ Bilingual Proficiency: Dual Language Exposure and Bilingual Proficiency. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 1834–1849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruan, Y.; Byers-Heinlein, K.; Orena, A.J.; Polka, L. Mixed-language input and infant volubility: Friend or foe? Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2023, 26, 1051–1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bail, A.; Morini, G.; Newman, R.S. Look at the gato! Code-switching in speech to toddlers. J. Child Lang. 2015, 42, 1073–1101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wei, L. Translanguaging as a Practical Theory of Language. Appl. Linguist. 2017, 39, 9–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yang, C.; Zhang, J. The effect of language distance on bilingual children’s representation and reasoning of periodic time. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2011, 43, 1123–1134. [Google Scholar]
- Green, D.W. Mental Control of the Bilingual Lexico-Semantic System. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 1998, 1, 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hammer, C.S.; Hoff, E.; Uchikoshi, Y.; Gillanders, C.; Castro, D.C.; Sandilos, L.E. The language and literacy development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Child. Res. Q. 2014, 29, 715–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linck, J.A.; Kroll, J.F.; Sunderman, G. Losing Access to the Native Language While Immersed in a Second Language: Evidence for the Role of Inhibition in Second-Language Learning. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 20, 1507–1515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hartshorne, J.K.; Tenenbaum, J.B.; Pinker, S. A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers. Cognition 2018, 177, 263–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernández-Dobao, A.; Herschensohn, J. Acquisition of Spanish verbal morphology by child bilinguals: Overregularization by heritage speakers and second language learners. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2021, 24, 56–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morini, G.; Newman, R.S. Dónde está la ball? Examining the effect of code switching on bilingual children’s word recognition. J. Child Lang. 2019, 46, 1238–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brooks, P.J.; Kempe, V. More Is More in Language Learning: Reconsidering the Less-Is-More Hypothesis. Lang. Learn. 2018, 69, 13–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radman, N.; Jost, L.; Dorood, S.; Mancini, C.; Annoni, J. Language distance modulates cognitive control in bilinguals. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 24131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xiang, C.; Xiang, Z. Linguistic Distance Review and Stratification Strategies of China’s Foreign Communication Practices: The Case of Confucius Institutes. Mod. Commun. 2022, 44, 65–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cui, M.; Zhang, W.; Sun, T. Linguistic distance, native language differences and Chinese language acquisition: An empirical study based on linguistic economics. Chin. Lang. Teach. World 2018, 32, 280–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muñoz, C.; Cadierno, T.; Casas, I. Different starting points for english language learning: A comparative study of Danish and Spanish young learners. Lang. Learn. 2018, 68, 1076–1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ou, Y.; Liu, C. The effect of linguistic distance on phonological transfer of Mongolian students learning English as a third language. J. Xi’an Int. Stud. Univ. 2009, 17, 93–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, R.; Jiao, L.; Fan, X.; Liu, C. The advantages and disadvantages of second language learning: From the perspective of cognition and language. J. East China Norm. Univ. (Educ. Sci.) 2016, 34, 90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schepens, J.J.; Van der Slik, F.; Van Hout, R. L1 and L2 distance effects in learning L3 dutch. Lang. Learn. 2016, 66, 224–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gao, Y.; Zheng, L.; Liu, X.; Nichols, E.S.; Liu, L. First and second language reading difficulty among Chinese–English bilingual children: The prevalence and influences from demographic characteristics. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chen, Y.; Zhang, P. Linguistic Distance and L2 Effect on the L3 Lexical Processing of Chinese-English-Japanese Trilinguals. J. Beijing Int. Stud. Univ. 2019, 41, 74–87. [Google Scholar]
- Marinova-Todd, S.H.; Zhao, J.; Bernhardt, M.; Bernhardt, M. Phonological awareness skills in the two languages of mandarin–english bilingual children. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2010, 24, 387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Romeo, R.R.; Flournoy, J.C.; McLaughlin, K.A.; Lengua, L.J. Language development as a mechanism linking socioeconomic status to executive functioning development in preschool. Dev. Sci. 2022, 25, e13227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, G.; Chen, Y.; Guan, T.; Zhang, D. The composition and characteristics of indicators of subjective social status of college students. J. Southwest Univ. (Nat. Sci. Ed.) 2015, 37, 156–162. [Google Scholar]
- Hu, M.; Wang, M.; Cai, L.; Zhu, X.; Yao, S. Preliminary development of adolescents’ subjective socioeconomic status scale. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 2012, 20, 155–161. [Google Scholar]
- Zhong, J.; Huang, B. A review of the concept, measurement and application of students’ socioeconomic status. Glob. Educ. Outlook 2012, 41, 31–39. [Google Scholar]
- Aneta, P. The Bilingual Mental Lexicon: Interdisciplinary Approaches; Multilingual Matters: Bristol, UK, 2009; pp. 125–160. [Google Scholar]
- Dong, Y.; Gui, S.; MacWhinney, B. Shared and separate meanings in the bilingual mental lexicon. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2005, 8, 221–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moetia, M. Code Mixing and Code Switching in The Classroom Interaction. Engl. Educ. J. 2018, 9, 229–242. [Google Scholar]
- Antón, E.; Carreiras, M.; Duñabeitia, J.A. The impact of bilingualism on executive functions and working memory in young adults. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0206770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E. Bilingualism in Development. Language, Literacy, and Cognition. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E. Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children. A framework for research. Lang. Learn. Suppl. 2007, 57, 45–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Single | Mixed | |
---|---|---|
Learning phase | 59.6% ± 4.1% | 52.9% ± 3.0% |
Test phase | 66.8% ± 4.8% | 56.3% ± 2.9% |
Single | Mixed | |
---|---|---|
Learning phase | 60.3% ± 3.7% | 51.6% ± 3.5% |
Test phase | 64.1% ± 3.8% | 52.7% ± 5.5% |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Niu, Z.; Li, Z.; Ma, Y.; Yu, K.; Wang, R. Language Distance Moderates the Effect of a Mixed-Language Environment on New-Word Learning for 4-Year-Old Children. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 411. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14050411
Niu Z, Li Z, Ma Y, Yu K, Wang R. Language Distance Moderates the Effect of a Mixed-Language Environment on New-Word Learning for 4-Year-Old Children. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(5):411. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14050411
Chicago/Turabian StyleNiu, Zhengkai, Zilong Li, Yunxiao Ma, Keke Yu, and Ruiming Wang. 2024. "Language Distance Moderates the Effect of a Mixed-Language Environment on New-Word Learning for 4-Year-Old Children" Brain Sciences 14, no. 5: 411. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14050411
APA StyleNiu, Z., Li, Z., Ma, Y., Yu, K., & Wang, R. (2024). Language Distance Moderates the Effect of a Mixed-Language Environment on New-Word Learning for 4-Year-Old Children. Brain Sciences, 14(5), 411. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14050411