Next Article in Journal
Alterations in Neurotrophins in Alcohol-Addicted Patients during Alcohol Withdrawal
Previous Article in Journal
Transcriptomic Profile of Mouse Brain Ageing in Early Developmental Stages
Previous Article in Special Issue
Do Individual Differences in Perception Affect Awareness of Climate Change?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Editorial

The Role of Consciousness in Coupling Emotions, Motivations, and Behaviors

Department of Surgical, Medical, Molecular and Critical Area Pathology, University of Pisa, 56126 Pisa, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(6), 582; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14060582
Submission received: 20 May 2024 / Accepted: 29 May 2024 / Published: 5 June 2024

1. An Introduction and the Rationale of this Special Issue: The Neglected Purpose of Consciousness

A potential function of consciousness is to integrate emotions, motivations, and subsequent behaviors into a coherent narrative [1]. Even if it is intuitive, this hypothesis is partially challenged by evidence that aversive stimuli can elicit consistent behaviors even when not consciously perceived [2] or that individuals with arachnophobia instinctively avoid spiders even knowing that they are virtual [3]. Given this information, how might consciousness be involved in linking emotions, motivations, and behaviors? Can this mechanism fail under some conditions?
To investigate the possible role of consciousness in this coupling, a tentative approach involves examining the effect of consciousness alterations in each relevant construct. This goal can be pursued through a variety of frameworks; for example, by decreasing phobic symptoms by rewarding the spontaneous activation of phobia-related brain areas [4,5], addressing the irrational motivations that induce people to indulge in risky behaviors despite massive awareness campaigns promoted in this regard [6], or trying to replicate the conscious–unconscious relationship in artificial intelligence (AI) systems [7].

2. Overview of Published Articles

This Special Issue captures a variety of frameworks in which the role of consciousness can be ascribed when considering the coupling of emotions, motivations, and behaviors. The majority of papers reported empirical data ranging from behavioral to electrophysiological correlates integrated with self-report measures; others introduced and discussed computational models.
The article by Lee et al. investigated how an altered state of consciousness induced by a meditation-based protocol (Awareness Training Program, ATP) affected self-reported psychological stress and age-related EEG correlates; they found that the ATP reduced stress and made the EEG correlates more similar to those of a younger population.
In a task involving the recognition of various emotions and of AI/real content, Tarchi et al. found EEG correlates already differentiating emotional from non-emotional and authentic from AI-generated faces in the early stages (e.g., P100, 90–140 ms) of stimulus processing.
Analogously, Hou et al. found early (e.g., N1, 80–120 ms; P1, 140–180 ms) EEG correlates differentiating more- from less-attractive faces administered at both supraliminal and subliminal levels.
These unconscious effects on behavior were also studied by Panteli et al., who evaluated the relationship between emotion regulation/awareness and externalizing problems (e.g., problematic behaviors derived from poor impulse control) in preadolescents. Both physiological and subjective data supported the idea that greater proneness to externalizing problems is correlated with weak regulation and awareness of one’s own emotions (as well as their physiological correlates).
Similarly, Cipriani et al. suggested that interoceptive sensitivity could modulate awareness of changes in climate, providing a detailed discussion of the psychophysiological mechanisms underlying these factors. The capability to detect external as well as internal signs of climate change could underlie differences in feeling (and actively trying to reduce) the risks it implies.
The entwined hypothesis described by Grindrod and Brennan proposes that interoceptive and emotional awareness can regulate the rapidity and accuracy of cognitive processes; this revisitation of LeDoux’s two-pathways theory of emotions and of Kahneman’s “thinking fast, thinking slow” approach to cognition also carries implications for artificial intelligence.
Reviewing the scientific literature on the Projective Consciousness Model (PCM), Rudrauf et al. stress the relevance that the point of view taken by an agent perceiving internal or external stimuli can have on their motivation and hence behaviors.

3. Conclusions and Future Directions

The most recurrent topics mentioned in the papers collected in this Special Issue concern the irrelevance (or, at least, the unnecessity) of conscious awareness when performing complex tasks (such as discriminating AI-generated from authentic stimuli) and potentially impactful choices (e.g., evaluating a face’s attractivity) in favor of the relevance of interoceptive sensitivity (e.g., in shifting toward more or less pro-environmental behaviors).
This evidence is in agreement with the widespread adoption of mindfulness-based practices as good practices for improving psychological well-being by leveraging improvement in proprioceptive awareness.
In addition, from studies on relationships between interoception and consciousness in this Special Issue, we foresee interesting future perspectives on the development of artificial intelligence (AI). Indeed, there is a growing scientific interest in (1) giving a corporeity to AI, for example, by providing it with a physical and (even if mechanical) sensible body [8] able to replicate the peripherical–central information flux occurring in the physiology supporting human consciousness [9,10] and in (2) adding a subconscious or unconscious layer to AI structures currently corresponding to a potentially infinite global workspace [7]. Both these research paths could benefit from the results described in the relevant articles published in this Special Issue.
In conclusion, in our opinion, a final message can be derived from the contributions to this Special Issue: knowing something and deeply feeling it and its consequences are processes that can be surprisingly different and mutually unrelated; this notion will gain increasing relevance as humanity’s future challenges—climate change and artificial intelligence—move toward unprecedented scenarios.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

List of Contributions

  • Lee, K.C.; Gao, J.; Leung, H.K.; Wu, B.W.Y.; Roberts, A.; Thach, T.-Q.; Sik, H.H. Modulating Consciousness through Awareness Training Program and Its Impacts on Psychological Stress and Age-Related Gamma Waves. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 91. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14010091.
  • Panteli, M.; Constantinou, T.; Vrachimi-Souroulla, A.; Fanti, K.; Panayiotou, G. Subjective and Autonomic Arousal toward Emotional Stimuli in Preadolescents with Externalizing Problems and the Role of Explicit and Implicit Emotion Regulation. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 84. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14010084.
  • Tarchi, P.; Lanini, M.C.; Frassineti, L.; Lanatà, A. Real and Deepfake Face Recognition: An EEG Study on Cognitive and Emotive Implications. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 1233. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13091233.
  • Grindrod, P.; Brennan, M. Cognition and Consciousness Entwined. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 872. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13060872.
  • Hou, X.; Shang, J.; Tong, S. Neural Mechanisms of the Conscious and Subliminal Processing of Facial Attractiveness. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 855. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13060855.
  • Cipriani, E.; Frumento, S.; Grassini, S.; Gemignani, A.; Menicucci, D. Do Individual Differences in Perception Affect Awareness of Climate Change? Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 266. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14030266.
  • Rudrauf, D.; Sergeant-Perthuis, G.; Tisserand, Y.; Poloudenny, G.; Williford, K.; Amorim, M.-A. The Projective Consciousness Model: Projective Geometry at the Core of Consciousness and the Integration of Perception, Imagination, Motivation, Emotion, Social Cognition and Action. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 1435. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13101435.

References

  1. LeDoux, J. The Deep History of Ourselves: The Four-Billion-Year Story of How We Got Conscious Brains. Philos. Psychol. 2023, 36, 704–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Granado, L.C.; Ranvaud, R.; Peláez, J.R. A Spiderless Arachnophobia Therapy: Comparison between Placebo and Treatment Groups and Six-Month Follow-Up Study. Neural Plast. 2007, 2007, 010241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Frumento, S.; Iannizzotto, A.; Greco, A.; Scilingo, E.P.; Gemignani, A.; Menicucci, D. Development of a Behavioral Avoidance Test in Virtual Reality (VR-BAT). In Proceedings of the 2023 IEEE International Conference on Metrology for eXtended Reality, Artificial Intelligence and Neural Engineering (MetroXRAINE), Milano, Italy, 25–27 October 2023; pp. 949–953. [Google Scholar]
  4. Taschereau-Dumouchel, V.; Liu, K.-Y.; Lau, H. Unconscious psychological treatments for physiological survival circuits. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2018, 24, 62–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Taschereau-Dumouchel, V.; Cortese, A.; Chiba, T.; Knotts, J.D.; Kawato, M.; Lau, H. Towards an unconscious neural reinforcement intervention for common fears. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 3470–3475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Bufano, P.; Poma, A.M.; Frumento, S.; Persechino, B.; Menicucci, D.; Laurino, M.; Gemignani, A. Meta-analytic study on substance intake and work-related accidents calls for attention to bio-psycho-social factors. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2023, 144, 104987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Blum, L.; Blum, M. A Theoretical Computer Science Perspective on Consciousness and Artificial General Intelligence. Engineering 2023, 25, 12–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Cominelli, L.; Hoegen, G.; De Rossi, D. Abel: Integrating Humanoid Body, Emotions, and Time Perception to Investigate Social Interaction and Human Cognition. Appl. Sci. 2021, 11, 1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cominelli, L.; Mazzei, D.; De Rossi, D.E. SEAI: Social emotional artificial intelligence based on Damasio’s theory of mind. Front. Robot. AI 2018, 5, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Consciousness Begins with Feeling, Not Thinking|Antonio Damasio, Hanna Damasio. Available online: https://iai.tv/articles/consciousness-begins-with-feelings-hanna-damasio-auid-2462 (accessed on 2 May 2024).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Frumento, S.; Menicucci, D. The Role of Consciousness in Coupling Emotions, Motivations, and Behaviors. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 582. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14060582

AMA Style

Frumento S, Menicucci D. The Role of Consciousness in Coupling Emotions, Motivations, and Behaviors. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(6):582. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14060582

Chicago/Turabian Style

Frumento, Sergio, and Danilo Menicucci. 2024. "The Role of Consciousness in Coupling Emotions, Motivations, and Behaviors" Brain Sciences 14, no. 6: 582. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14060582

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop