Next Article in Journal
Lead Excretion in Spanish Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Microarray Analysis Reveals Higher Gestational Folic Acid Alters Expression of Genes in the Cerebellum of Mice Offspring—A Pilot Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Mechanistic Approach to Cross-Domain Perceptual Narrowing in the First Year of Life
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Multisensory Integration and Child Neurodevelopment

by
Emmanuelle Dionne-Dostie
1,2,*,
Natacha Paquette
1,2,
Maryse Lassonde
1,2 and
Anne Gallagher
1,2,*
1
Sainte-Justine University Hospital Research Center, Montreal H3T1C5, QC, Canada
2
Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition (CERNEC), Departement of Psychology, University of Montreal, C.P. 6128, Montreal H3C3J7, QC, Canada
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2015, 5(1), 32-57; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci5010032
Submission received: 6 September 2014 / Accepted: 27 January 2015 / Published: 11 February 2015
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Cognition in Infants)

Abstract

:
A considerable number of cognitive processes depend on the integration of multisensory information. The brain integrates this information, providing a complete representation of our surrounding world and giving us the ability to react optimally to the environment. Infancy is a period of great changes in brain structure and function that are reflected by the increase of processing capacities of the developing child. However, it is unclear if the optimal use of multisensory information is present early in childhood or develops only later, with experience. The first part of this review has focused on the typical development of multisensory integration (MSI). We have described the two hypotheses on the developmental process of MSI in neurotypical infants and children, and have introduced MSI and its neuroanatomic correlates. The second section has discussed the neurodevelopmental trajectory of MSI in cognitively-challenged infants and children. A few studies have brought to light various difficulties to integrate sensory information in children with a neurodevelopmental disorder. Consequently, we have exposed certain possible neurophysiological relationships between MSI deficits and neurodevelopmental disorders, especially dyslexia and attention deficit disorder with/without hyperactivity.

1. Introduction to Multisensory Information

Most species, including humans, are equipped with various highly specialized sensory systems that give them access to numerous types of information on the surrounding environment. Each sensory modality gives us a unique outlook on the world: Color, for instance, can only be perceived through sight, sound through hearing and temperature feel through the somatosensory system. However, our surroundings never cease to present us with situations that stimulate several senses at once. In day-to-day life, events are rarely unimodal; they are multisensory experiences, deriving from a combination of information acquired through several sensory modalities. The brain integrates this multisensory information to provide a complete and coherent representation of what is being perceived and consequently for appropriate behavioral responses to be generated [1]. For example, if we hear the sound of a siren at a distance and then see a vehicle approaching, the association of visual image and sound leads us instantly to perceive an ambulance. Our perception and behavior are not based on the juxtaposition of independent sensory experience but on the global impression resulting from the combination of information acquired through various modalities. This phenomenon has been called Multisensory Integration (MSI).
In many cases, the survival of an organism depends on appropriate responses to multisensory stimuli. Therefore, our ability to integrate sensory information becomes a fundamental element of our everyday behavior, allowing us to identify events and apply optimal responses to these events. From that perspective, many studies conducted to investigate the behavioral effects of a multisensory presentation have shown that it has beneficial effects. Incidentally, it has been noted that the combination of two sensory modalities could, in various contexts, decrease response time [2,3,4], increase target detection accuracy [5,6], increase acuity in one of the modalities [7], reduce detection thresholds [8] and even facilitate and optimize learning applied to a single sensory modality [9]. Furthermore, these facilitative behavioral benefits are noted regardless of which sensory modalities are being tested.
To improve our understanding of the benefits afforded by the simultaneous presentation, studies have used various type of stimulation. For instance, several studies have shown behaviorally that viewing a stimulated body part can enhance tactile detection and perception at the stimulated site [5] and that this visuo-tactile enhancement may last up to minutes [7,10]. Thus, using visual and proprioceptive stimuli, seeing one’s own arm may shorten the response time to an unseen tactile stimulus to the arm [2]. In return, somatosensory information can also facilitate visual processing [11]. For instance, the response time to a light projected on one’s own finger is shorter than when the light is projected on an object far removed from the body [11]. Similarly, studies have shown that vibrotactile stimuli (for example, short vibrations applied to the palm) shorten the response time to visual stimuli [3]. Finally, the integration of tactile and auditory inputs was also investigated, albeit succinctly, revealing that tactile stimuli can be beneficial to auditory perception. It appears that a tactile signal, even non-pertinent, can sometimes facilitate the detection of a sound, thus increasing the impression of loudness [6,7,8]. In that perspective, research in healthy adults has repeatedly shown behavioral benefit of MSI that might be linked to the underlying ability of the brain to integrate different sensory inputs related to a single event [12].

2. MSI and Development

2.1. Innate or Acquired

Although MSI capacity has been extensively studied in adults, little has been done to study it in young children. The first important question would be: Does this capacity exist at birth, or does it develop as a result of experience? One school of thought (early integration) argues for the innate nature of MSI while another (late integration) emphasizes the role of experience in the development of MSI.

2.1.1. Early Integration Approach

According to the Early Integration Approach, the nervous system is multisensorial right from its early development stage, possessing the capacities to detect redundant aspects of the surrounding environment [13]. In support of this approach, Bower, Broughton and Moore (1970) [14], have observed that infants are able to move their hands toward visual targets as early as six days after birth, which indicates that hand-eye coordination occurs very early on in life. Even in the first months of life, infants are able to perceive and derive meaning from the abundance of multisensorial information. Although the literature on infants is relatively recent and small, a few authors have suggested that MSI is not a unitary process and that different mechanisms might be implicated depending on the specific type of multisensory interaction [15].
Bahrick and Lickliter (2000) [16] proposed the “intersensory redundancy hypothesis”, in order to explain how infants perceive coherent, unified multimodal objects and events through different sensory modalities. This hypothesis refers to the presentation of the same information spatially coordinated and temporally synchronous across two or more sensory modalities, and is only possible for amodal properties that are not specific to a single sense modality (e.g., shape, rhythm, duration, intensity) [17]. In other words, regardless of which sensory modality, similar qualities are perceived when we integrate information. For instance, when we hear and look at a bouncing ball, we detect that the auditory and visual stimulations that originate from the same location share a common tempo and rhythm. This sensitivity to amodal properties allows the infant to direct his attention to unitary and meaningful events in which information from different sensory modalities originates from a unique point of origin [18,19].
Studies on the cross-modal transfer of information from touch to vision revealed that neonates are able to process and encode shape information about manually experienced objects and to discriminate between subsequently presented visual objects [20,21]. Newborns are also able to visually recognize the texture that they previously felt and tactually recognize the texture that they previously saw [22]. Others have reported that one-month-old infants can benefit from the tactile-oral properties of an object during visual recognition, showing a clear visual preference for objects with which they had been familiarized through oral presentation [23,24,25].
It has been demonstrated that the ability to perceive audio-visual relations also emerges early in human development. For example, Lewkowicz and Turkewitz (1980) [26] have shown that three-week-old infants responded to the equivalence of different levels of auditory loudness and visual brightness inputs on the basis of intensity. Therefore, sound presented concurrently with visual stimulation modified visual behavior. It has been shown that in the first few months of life, infants benefit from temporally and/or spatially associated multisensory information [16,27,28]. While temporal synchrony refers to the capacity to specify whether a particular sound and image go together or not, spatial synchrony concerns whether signals in diverse modalities comes from a common or different location. The importance of synchrony for infant perception has been well documented [29,30,31] and is also reflected in the intermodal preference procedure, in which the bimodal information is typically presented synchronously. It has been reported that on the basis of synchrony, newborn infants are able to associate objects and linguistic stimuli [32] and that just a few hours old infants can learn sight-sound pairings [31]. Bahrick (2001) [28] found that early as four weeks after birth, infants are sensitive and able to learn arbitrary relations between audiovisual inputs, Moreover, it has been demonstrated that infants between three and six months old have the ability to detect the difference and discriminate temporal information of audio-visual inputs [33,34]. In addition, four-month-old infants can connect and bind visual objects to the specific sounds produced by these objects [35,36]. For instance, they are able to look longer at a puppet whose bouncing rate is the same as the rate at which the sound occurred [35]. These signs of integration, detected early on in life, remind us that the temporal synchrony is an important characteristic for MSI.
Behavioral benefits of early integration are imperative in the development of face and emotion recognition and are important features in a child’s abilities to adapt to his environment later in life. Studies using a visual preference paradigm in a multimodal context for human faces have reported interesting results in infants [36,37,38,39]. For instance, two-month-old babies can link phonetic information between voices and lip movements [39] and show an enhanced response when lip movements are synchronized with sounds in contrast to unsynchronized ones [40]. In addition, four-month-old infants can perceive affect (joy, sadness or anger) in words that are supported by audio-visual presentations [41], and discriminate these affects in a multimodal context (i.e., establishing relations between faces (visual modality) and voices (auditory modality) [41,42]. Overall, this data might support a form of multisensory association present early on in life, favoring the innate hypothesis for MSI development. However, all studies include post-natal investigations, which limits the opportunity to draw firm conclusions. Studies on children born prematurely could be used to make more compelling arguments in favor of this hypothesis and could be an interesting avenue of research. Also, evidence of multisensory processes in newborns and infants has been demonstrated largely by behavioral data and needs further investigation. Few researches have been carried to study the presence of multisensory mechanisms using neuroimaging techniques. Ethical and practical limitations such as recruitment may have diminished the possibility to undertake such studies on newborns. Nevertheless, the few studies found in the literature will be discussed in Section 2.2.

2.1.2. The Late Integration Approach

In opposition to the Early Integration Approach, the Late Integration Approach emphasizes the acquired nature of MSI. According to this hypothesis, all sensory systems work independently of each other at birth. At this time, these systems are not mature and become increasingly refined during the child’s development. This is a long-term process during which cognitive changes and neuronal reorganization keep going on until adolescence in which the brain must continuously adapt its neuronal networks between sensory and motor communications [43]. In support of this, Putzar and colleagues (2007) [44] have recently shown that temporary visual deprivation in the first two years of life affects the level of audio-visual integration that can be achieved once normal vision is restored. These difficulties also persisted into adulthood [44]. This suggests that a critical period in infant’s development might underlie the emergence and maturation of MSI, therefore reaching maturity ontogenetically late. During the first months of life, contact with multisensory information seems to be a prerequisite to develop and refine integration of these various sources of information. Thus, if relevant experience is not gained in infancy, individuals cannot compensate for this loss later in life [45].
Furthermore, the various senses do not develop at the same rate. For instance, in humans, all the sensory systems are functional at varying degrees by the end of gestation as these systems progress towards full structural and functional maturity. Sensory structures underlying touch seem to be the first to emerge during ontogenesis [46], followed by the onset of functional hearing (third trimester) and the development of the visual system (largely after birth) [47,48]. While these findings indicate that there is a difference in the rate at which the various sensory systems develop between one another, there is also a difference in the way diverse characteristics develop within each of these systems. For instance, visual acuity and contrast sensitivity keep improving until the child is five- or six-years-old [49], while the period between two and five years of age is the time of development of perceptual language [50], which corresponds to the processing of speech signal, both acoustically and visually. A study by Morrongiello et al. (1994) [51] found an age effect in their study on object exploration and object recognition in children between 3 and 8 years of age with common objects of different sizes. Their results showed a distinct developmental pattern: Older children were faster, recognized more objects and were more thorough in their exploratory strategies than the younger children [51]. Moreover, object handling skills improve until the child is 8- to 14-year-old [52]. Thus, the child will have to learn to integrate multisensory information during his development [13].
Recent studies conducted on very young infants have also suggested that post-natal experience might contribute to MSI development [53]. Neil and colleagues (2006) [54] have studied the development of eye and head movements in 1- to 10-month-old infants while they were subjected to auditory, visual and audio-visual stimuli. Results showed that only 8- to 10-month-old infants responded significantly faster in bimodal conditions than in unimodal conditions, suggesting that audio-visual integration emerges at a late stage in the first year of life. Other studies have supported the assumption that the integration of different modalities does not become optimal until relatively late in childhood. In contrast to adults, when confronted to different sources of information from various sensory modalities, children do not optimally integrate the information from the two sensory modalities but make their perceptual judgments based only on one or the other sense. This unisensory dominance has been found in visuo-tactile integration task [55], visual and tactile cues for size and orientation discrimination [27], nonvisual self-motion and visual landmark information [56], in judgments of surface slant based on stereoscopic and texture information [57] and in audio and visual space and time perception [58]. According to Burr and Gorri (2011) [59], this unisensory dominance in which the most robust modality is employed to tune the others could reflect a process of cross-sensory calibration. As stated in the previous studies, MSI has not reached maturity in children younger than eight years old [27,55,56,57,58].
A number of authors have also put forward the presence of a MSI temporal window over which the strength of multisensory interactions is dependent on the spatial and temporal synchrony between different sensory inputs [60,61,62]. The more remote in space and time two sensory stimuli are, the less likely they are to fuse. A behavioral study by Hillock and colleagues (2011) [63], reported that the audio-visual integration temporal window is still immature in 10- and 11-year-old children, which supports the hypothesis that the underlying plasticity and maturation of MSI continues through development.
Unlike adults, behavioral studies have reported immature multisensory processing capacities in children and adolescents in audio-visual discrimination tasks [63,64,65] and in other sensory modalities [56]. For instance, Barutchu and colleagues [64,65] reported that multisensory facilitation is still immature in 10-year-old children during a simple audio-visual detection task. In accordance with the Late Integration Theory, results from these studies suggest that information acquired through various sensory modalities might not be integrated optimally in very young infants and that optimal MSI only occurs in children older than eight years [27,56] with changes occurring over a prolonged time course that may extend into adolescence.
Animal studies have corroborated the notion that the brain is immature and must learn to combine the various types of sensory information. One of the most studied neural structure in MSI is the cat superior colliculus (SC), a midbrain structure in which neurons located in the deep layers are responsible to converge multisensory inputs [66]. These multisensory neurons are responsible, among other things, to detect and orient one’s behavior to external events and sensory stimuli [67,68,69,70]. Visual, auditory and somatosensory inputs stimulate the SC and each sensory modality is represented in a map-like representation in which all the different maps overlap each other and are in close topographic register. Therefore, the alignment among the sensory maps is fundamental for multisensory neurons to integrate the inputs from various senses manifesting itself into an adequately behavioral response [71]. Nevertheless, according to Wallace and colleagues (1997, 2006) [72,73] multisensory neurons found in the SC are not present at the cortical and sub-cortical levels immediately after birth and it is only after several months of life and exposure to a multisensory environment that the integration-specialized neurons progressively appear and mature in the cat. Although the appearance of the first multisensory neurons in kittens is at about ten days of age, their ability to integrate inputs from multiple sensory modalities that can be considered adult-like is not seen until three post-natal months [72]. As in the cat multisensory SC neurons, the newborn rhesus monkey also fails to integrate coincident cross-modal inputs [74,75]. Thus, this capacity might indeed be strongly dependent on experiences [73,76].
Early post-natal experience is critical and deprivation of sensory information during this time can lead to an inability to integrate signals neurologically [45,77]. Studies conducted on the effects of sensory deprivation have demonstrated the significance of sensory organ stimulation to the proper functioning and development of sensorineural structures. It is possible that MSI capacities increase in precision over the course of human development and that they are progressively enriched through both the maturation of brain systems and the accumulation of experience. Since newborns from all species are first introduced with a complex and multisensorial environment, they ought to possess some neural mechanisms allowing them to adapt to that environment promptly. Although these coping mechanisms remain fragile and rudimentary, it is these mechanisms that allow their survival. This review exposes a discrepancy in the data revealing that newborn animals cannot perform MSI at birth while there is evidence that newborn humans can. This divergence may be accountable in the fact that animal adaptation is specific to natural environments, and “does not occur when the animal is viewing artificial stimuli such as gratings” [78]. Nevertheless, the question whether MSI is innate or a product of our environment remains debatable and requires further investigation.

2.2. Neuroanatomical Correlated of MSI and Pediatric Brain Activity

The recent explosion of EEG’s and neuroimaging techniques has allowed the identification of numerous multisensory convergence zones in the brain. Numerous cortical and subcortical brain regions have been identified to receive afferent inputs from multiple senses. Subcortically, the SC and the basal ganglia as well as association and other cortical regions including the superior temporal sulcus, the parietal, premotor and prefrontal cortex [79,80,81,82] are also implicated in MSI, including feedback as well as feedforward anatomical projections [83,84]. Moreover, recent evidence at the earliest stages of perceptual processing reveals multisensory modulations, activations and connectivity in sensory sensory-specific brain areas [85,86].
To date, knowledge of the development of the neurophysiological processes that underlie MSI comes largely from animal and adult studies. To our knowledge, one recent study using electrophysiology (ERP) [87] has investigated the development of MSI capacities in infants. In this study, Reynolds and colleagues (2014) [87] have shown evidence of enhance neural responsiveness to synchronous audiovisual stimulation compared to asynchronous stimulation in five-month-old infants. Besides, only a few studies have dealt with the underlying cerebral mechanisms of MSI in older children and teenagers. It is in particular the case of Brandwein, Foxe, Russo, Altschuler, Gomes and Molholm (2011) [88] who used ERP to characterize the developmental trajectory of brain processes underlying audiovisual MSI in forty-nine neurotypical children and adolescents aged between 7- and 16-years-old. The data suggest that mature levels of multisensory facilitation are only reached by approximately 15 years of age. Another ERP study in typically developing children aged between 6- and 13-years-old suggests that like adults, they integrate multisensory audio-somatosensory input during multiple stages of sensory information processing [89]. However, a study in magnetoencephalography revealed a robust audio-somatosensory integration response in 11- and 13-month-old infants, which suggest the development of MSI during the first year of life [90].
As outlined above, these studies provide encouraging evidence for cortical MSI in typical children. However, considering the important discrepancy in neurophysiological data, further exploration of MSI in neurotypical children is required. The use of neuroimaging approach is of great interest for the scientific community and could fill the current gap of knowledge on developmental MSI.

3. MSI and Cognitive Development

As previously stated, multisensory stimuli provide an enhanced representation of the environment [91] and MSI processes facilitate the speed and accuracy of a variety of behaviors and perceptual processes. The benefits of multisensory information derive from empirical support with infants in various domains showing that multisensory stimulation can enhance early perceptual, affective, and cognitive discrimination. For instance, the combined use of information from several senses has been found to enhance time discrimination abilities in infants [16,33] as well as affect discrimination [41] and even numerical cognition in infants [92,93] and preschool children [94]. Multisensory stimulation promotes heightened attention, perceptual processing and memory in adults as well as in infants [16,95]. Some authors have put forward that the capacity to perceive a complete and coherent representation of multimodal information provides the groundwork for the development of perception, cognition and behavioral abilities, which, in turn, are critical elements to the development of higher-level perceptual and cognitive functions [96,97]. The acquisition of MSI capacity would influence perceptual learning [16] and the acquisition of global cognitive and intellectual abilities [98,99]. Therefore, these MSI capacities in infancy may provide an important developmental foundation for the emergence of cognitive abilities in adulthood.
The premise that MSI can contribute to cognitive development has been supported by studies showing lower intellectual functioning in children with MSI deficits [99,100,101]. Some studies showed that information transfer across different modalities predict verbal performance in school age children, suggesting that primary MSI skills can impact the acquisition of verbal aptitudes [99,100]. For instance, recent findings showed that children who did not demonstrate good MSI skills had weaker verbal processing scores than what was expected when considering their age and global intellectual quotient [101]. Moreover, sensory integration deficits might result in various difficulties such as learning new skills, getting organized, regulating attention and engaging in positive social experiences in some children [102]. These kinds of difficulties in integrating multisensory information might be associated with particular neurodevelopmental disorders in children.

4. MSI in Neurodevelopmental Disorders

According to the World Health Organization, neurodevelopmental disorders affect one in six children in industrialized countries [103]. Impacting cerebral growth and development, neurodevelopmental disorders encompass a wide range of disorders, such as intellectual disability, Tourette syndrome, autism spectrum disorder, learning disabilities and attention deficit disorder with or without hyperactivity (ADD/ADHD). Children with neurodevelopmental disorders also represent a significant proportion of students who are struggling or failing in school [104,105]. A better understanding of these disorders thus appears to be a crucial concern, particularly when embracing a therapeutic and preventive perspective. Therefore, the following sections will examine the relationship between MSI deficits and two neurodevelopmental disorders commonly found in children: Developmental dyslexia [106] and attention deficit disorder with or without hyperactivity (ADD/ADHD).

4.1. MSI and Dyslexia

Developmental dyslexia is a specific reading and spelling deficit affecting 4% to 10% of the population [107]. According to the Fifth Edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) [106], this neurodevelopmental disorder manifests itself during the years of formal schooling and is characterized by persistent and impairing difficulties with learning foundational academic skills in reading, such as reading fluently with an accurate comprehension, despite average or above-average intelligence [106,108].
Deficits in the phonological domain have consistently been found to be the primary cause of developmental dyslexia [109,110,111,112], although other cognitive deficits, such as in working memory [113,114], executive functions [115,116,117,118], processing speed [119,120,121] and attention [122] have been linked to this disorder. Although a number of conceptual frameworks have been put forward to explain the spectrum of neurological deficits seen in dyslexia, we will review the phonological-deficit hypothesis and the temporal processing hypothesis, two models of particular interest for developmental MSI.

4.1.1. MSI and the Phonological-Deficit and Temporal Processing Hypothesis

The phonological-deficit hypothesis supposes that reading and spelling difficulties result from impaired phonological processes specific to language [107,123,124]. MSI is undoubtedly an important competence in the acquisition of reading skills, since they rely upon rapid and accurate associations between the visual (written) and auditory (verbal) labels [125,126,127]. Regardless of phonological awareness difficulties, authors have suggested that children with dyslexia have difficulties associating verbal labels to the appropriate visual stimuli and thus, establishing appropriate associations between a word and its spelling [127,128]. A number of studies have put forward that unstable letter-speech sound associations could be a critical factor in dyslexia [129,130]. It has been found that children with developmental dyslexia showed significant deficits in processing letter and digit strings (verbal material) [131]. These results receive strong support from a neuroimaging study by Blau and colleagues (2010) [132] showing fundamental deficits in letter-sound integration in children with dyslexia. Similarly, recent findings on the influence of processing speed on reading acquisition in six-year-old children revealed that, whatever the modality (audio-verbal, visuo-verbal, visual, and visuo-visual), children with reading difficulties displayed poor performances in rapid intermodal processing speed in audio-verbal (phonological awareness) and visuo-verbal (rapid naming) tasks [133]. These results support the influence of MSI in rapid naming and phonological processes involved in reading, suggesting that the neural mechanisms underlying audio-visual integration in children with dyslexia differ from normal reading children.
Other studies postulated that not only unimodal [125,134,135] but also multisensory verbal and nonverbal information processing is temporally impaired in children with dyslexia. Some evidence suggests that the core deficit of dyslexia is based on atypical temporal processing of audio-visual multisensory information [125,135,136,137,138]. An interesting paradigm to investigate intermodal temporal processing is the McGurk effect [139]. This effect occurs when an individual sees and hears a speaker producing speech segments that are incongruent with their visual perception. For instance, a classic McGurk effect is hearing the syllable /ta/ while the speaker uttered /ga/. Thus, the auditor usually perceives the sound /pa/. This robust effect in neurotypical individuals was found to be impaired in subjects with dyslexia, suggesting a deficit of MSI. For that matter, instead of perceiving the normal illusion, individuals with dyslexia tend to pronounce the sound perceived visually [140]. Similarly, children with reading difficulties showed low performances in comparing two patterns of brief nonverbal audio-visual stimuli [141]. Moreover, decreased segregation acuity and prolonged cross-modal segregation times were also evident using trains of brief stimuli in three different sensory modalities (audio-visual, audio-tactile and visuo-tactile) in 8- to 12-year-old children with dyslexia, especially in the audio-visual condition [142]. These findings suggest the implication of a general impairment of the nervous system in children with dyslexia when processing audio-visual information rather than a sole deficit in the sensory, motor or phonological systems.

4.1.2. Other Sensory Deficits in Dyslexia

An array of subtle sensory defects unrelated to audio-visual stimuli has also been reported in individuals with dyslexia. For instance, previous studies involving a dual task reported that postural control was highly impaired in children with dyslexia when compared to age- and gender-matched controls [143,144]. Recent findings have also shown that children with dyslexia had difficulty maintaining balance while fixing a point in front of them [145] and that coupling between visual information to body sway was weaker and more variable compare to normal reading children [146]. Poor postural control in children with dyslexia might reflect an inability to integrate multiple sensorimotor inputs, such as proprioceptive signals, that are necessary for proper motor activity. Even though sensory motor deficits were put forth to explain dyslexia, there is a current gap in the literature concerning this hypothesis. Future research on this matter should be considered and would be an interesting avenue to pursue.
Based upon these results, the impairments suggested to underlie the causes of dyslexia could be directly related to atypical audio-visual temporal processing, rapid and accurate associations of visual and auditory stimuli and sensory-motor integration. These findings provide additional information about the neurophysiological causes and evidence of the MSI hypothesis to explain developmental dyslexia.

4.1.3. Anatomical and Structural Differences in Dyslexia and MSI

Recent neuroimaging studies have investigated neuronal activity in multisensory cortical regions of normal readers and individuals with specific learning disabilities in reading. Given its role in language processing, the most consistent finding is altered morphology in the left temporal lobe. For instance, Welcome and colleagues (2011) [147] recently reported reduced asymmetry in gray matter thickness within the temporo-parietal region and smaller brain sizes in the right inferior frontal region in children with reading difficulties. Using fMRI during a phonological task, Temple (2001) [148], reported reduced left temporo-parietal activity in 8- to 12-year-old children with dyslexia compared to normal reading children. Likewise, two magnetoencephalography (MEG) studies from Simos et al. (2000) [149,150] supported these results and found decreased left temporo-parietal activity in children with dyslexia while they were reading words and pseudowords. The temporo-parietal region is known to be involved in the integration of letters and speech sounds [151,152], a crucial ability for reading in beginning readers. In addition to atypical activity within the temporal lobe, pediatric functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have also reported deviant patterns of activation [153,154,155] and morphological brain alterations [156] in frontal brain regions of children with dyslexia. One possible explanation for these cerebral differences might be the reduced specialization for processing letters and letter-sound associations.
Altered connectivity patterns in primary sensory and multisensory processing regions were also found in individuals with dyslexia [157,158]. More precisely, the heteromodal superior temporal cortical regions as well as the auditory cortex (Heschl sulcus and planum temporale) have been identified as integration sites for letters and speech sounds in normal readers [151,159]. However, it has been found that children with dyslexia that failed to recognize speech sounds presented together with visual letters (congruent or incongruent) also showed reduced neural integration in the left planum temporale and Heschl sulcus and the left superior temporal sulcus [132]. Overall, these studies suggest that an interrelated network of visual, auditory and heteromodal brain areas might contribute to the skilled use of letter-speech sound integration necessary for learning to read. Reduced congruency between letters and speech sounds in children with dyslexia are likely to indicate less successful letter-speech sound integration [132]. The presence of deviant patterns of activation and morphological brain alterations associated with MSI may play a critical role in the neural basis of dyslexia. Despite the gap of knowledge on this matter, these results highlight the importance of pursuing such neuroimaging studies in order to better understand the impact of MSI deficits on children with reading disabilities.
Atypical development of MSI capacities during infancy can have a momentous impact on children’s neurodevelopment. The previous findings suggest that children with dyslexia have difficulties in MSI and may suffer from integrating sensory cues coming from multiple sources. The hypothesis that MSI deficits might be responsible for impaired reading is still premature. The impact of such difficulties remains unclear and requires greater attention from researchers. However, the development of this integrative ability may offer a cue that may be useful in identifying children likely to develop later difficulties. Therefore, we can speculate that children with dyslexia would take advantage of intervention protocols directed to improve sensory integration.

4.2. MSI and Attention Deficit Disorder with or without Hyperactivity (ADD/ADHD)

ADHD occurs in most cultures in about 3% to 5% of children [107,160] and accounts for approximately half of all pediatric referrals to mental health services in the United States [161,162]. ADD/ADHD is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by impairing levels of inattention, disorganization, and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity that interferes with functioning or development. Inattention and disorganization entail inability to stay on task, to give close attention to details, to listen when spoken to, to follow through on instructions, to be easily distracted by extraneous stimuli and to have difficulty organizing tasks and activities, at levels that are inconsistent with age or developmental level [107]. Hyperactivity refers to excessive motor activity when it is not appropriate or excessive fidgeting or tapping hands or feet when seated, difficulty to remain seated in situations where it is expected, overactivity, talking excessively, at levels that are inconsistent with age or developmental level [107]. Impulsivity implies a desire for immediate rewards or an inability to delay gratification which can manifest into behaviors such as a difficulty to wait for her/his turn, interrupting or intruding into other people’s activities and making important decisions without consideration of long-term consequences, at levels that are inconsistent with age or developmental level [107]. According to the DSM-V, ADHD symptoms must be present in at least two settings, impact directly on social and academic activities and must be present before the age of 12-years-old [107]. While the predominantly inattentive type (ADHD-I) is the most common subtype in the population (38%–57% of all individuals with ADHD), individuals with the combined inattention-hyperactivity type (ADHD-C) (22%–26%) are more likely to be referred for clinical services [163].
Additionally to attention problems, ADHD is often accompanied by deficits other than those subsumed under the ADHD diagnosis. In terms of cognitive profile, children with ADHD often have difficulty with executive functions (e.g., planning, set shifting, organization, inhibition and regulation of behavior) as well as processing speed and working memory [164,165,166,167,168,169,170,171,172,173,174,175,176]. A high percentage of children with attention disorders also have sensory processing problems, exemplified by behavioral evidence of difficulty modulating sensory responses [177,178,179]. It has been reported that boys with ADHD aged between 6- and 10-year-old have more sensory processing difficulties than neurotypical boys [180]. It has also been suggested that these children may not be perceiving and processing sensory information properly as well as having difficulty producing appropriate responses at school, at home and in the community [181]. Compared with children without neurodevelopmental disorders, children with ADHD exhibit greater difficulties in the sensorimotor domain such as the vestibular and balance control systems [182,183]. For instance, in contrast to neurotypical children, Hassan and Azzam (2012) [184] showed that children with ADHD-C aged between 8- and 10-year-old had lower somatosensory, visual and vestibular ratios by 1%, 9%, and 18%, respectively. According to Guskiewicz and Perrin (1996) [185], this could be the result of a lack of adequate interaction among the three sensory inputs that provide orientation information to the postural control system. Furthermore, children with ADHD also have more difficulties to process tactile [186], visual [187] and auditory stimuli [188,189,190,191]. More precisely, while Hern and Hynd (1992) [186] found that 6- and 12-year-old children with ADHD-C exhibit more soft signs than the normal group on a prototype sensorimotor soft sign battery. Ghanizadeh (2010) [187] demonstrated that children with ADHD give poorer performances on visual acuity and visual field. In the audition realm, a number of studies reported auditory processing problems in children with ADHD [188,189,190,191].

4.3. Neuroanatomical Model for ADHD

Studies investigating the neural basis of ADHD have steadily showed structural and/or functional anomalies in cortical areas, basal ganglia, and cerebellar brain regions in children with ADHD [192,193,194]. This circuit has been extensively investigated using methods such as structural volumetric brain imaging and fMRI and has been linked to ADHD behavioral deficits such as inhibitory response, working memory and executive functioning impairments [194,195,196,197,198,199,200,201,202,203]. For instance, using volumetric brain imaging to examine anatomic brain abnormalities, an extensive study compared regional brain volumes in 152 children and adolescents with ADHD aged 5 to 18 years old and 139 age- and sex-matched controls [204]. Children with ADHD exhibited smaller total cerebral and cerebellar volumes in contrast to control children and these volumetric differences persisted into adulthood. Furthermore, frontal and temporal gray matter, caudate nucleus and cerebellar volumes correlated with the severity of ADHD symptoms [204]. Other studies investigating volumetric differences between children with ADHD and typically developing children also reported smaller cerebral volume in frontal and prefrontal cortices [205,206,207]. Such cortical volume reductions were also associated with altered brain activation in sensory cortices (auditory, visual and somatosensory cortices) [184,208], which could explain sensorimotor deficits in this population. The frontal lobes both receive a multitude of inputs from sensory association areas and have influence over a wide region of the nervous system to direct behavior. Moreover, working memory, planning, and reasoning, often affected in ADHD, are associated to the frontal lobes and depend on the recognition and integration of a vast network of signals.
Research evidence in favor of the existence of bidirectional influences between attention and MSI is thus considerable and allows the possibility to make neuroanatomical connections between ADHD and MSI networks. Attentional processes and MSI share subcortical networks such as the SC [209] and operate with cortical regions, including the fronto-parietal and temporo-parietal networks [210]. Moreover, top-down attention is controlled by a fronto-parietal network of brain areas, which sends signals that modulate the sensitivity of neurons in sensory brain regions [211]. Recently, Koziol et al. (2011,2012) [192,212] integrated SPD/SMD symptoms in relation to their impacts upon the development of inhibitory control, working memory, academic skill, and behavioral automation in children with ADHD, proposing the first neuroanatomical conceptualization model of sensory processing deficits in ADHD. They suggested that an integrative network of specific brain regions is involved in both cognitive development and sensorimotor integration of the environment [212,213]. According to their model, the cortex encodes specific sensory information (visual, auditory, spatial, perceptual and sensory) by interacting with the basal ganglia that act as a selection mechanism and is important to voluntary motor movement, perceptual learning and inhibitory response. Then, the cerebellum receives sensory information inputs from the involved cortical regions and basal ganglia in order to integrate stimuli and modulate or regulate the intensity of experienced stimulation [212,213]. As previously mentioned, similarly to the neurons in the SC and related structures, previous studies have demonstrated the presence of multisensory neurons in the basal ganglia [85]. Neurons sensitive to visual, auditory or somatosensory modalities have been found in both the substantia nigra (SN) and the caudate nucleus (CN) in which a high proportions of neurons with multisensory properties have been described in these neuronal populations [214,215]. MSI deficits in the CN and the SN may lead to a disruption in the processing of complex sensory stimuli which, indirectly will affect the sensory feedback of motor actions controlled by the basal ganglia. Studies have documented anatomical abnormalities, such as volume decrease, asymmetric and connectivity differences, in the basal ganglia [203,208] and cerebellum of children with ADHD [203,216,217,218,219].
Overall, the neural basis of ADHD has been widely documented by functional and structural studies. Anatomical evidence has provided a reasonable framework for the suggestion that ineffective MSI cortical maps underlying sensory-related behaviors as well as physiological reactivity to sensory stimuli found in children with both ADHD and sensory processing problems may possibly explain their comorbidity.

5. Conclusions

One of the most impressive features of the central nervous system is its ability to process information from a variety of stimuli to produce an integrated, comprehensive representation of the external world. Research in neurotypical adults has repeatedly shown behavioral benefits of MSI that might be linked to the underlying ability of the brain to integrate different sensory inputs related to a single event [11]. The benefits of the combined use of information from several senses have been exposed in numerous studies and reveal notably that MSI aids detection and speed response [2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9]. Although MSI capacity has been extensively studied in adults, the literature is scarcer regarding the ability to integrate multisensory information in infants. This review firstly aimed to describe MSI developmental processes of neurotypical infants and children and its neuroanatomic correlates. Moreover, we addressed the question whether the capacity to integrate multisensory information exist from birth or develops as a result of experience. The debate between MSI being innate or acquired remains. Neurophysiological data on animals [59,60,63] and ERP studies on children and adolescents [65,66] suggest that our optimal capacity to integrate multisensory information reaches its maturity late during childhood and is strongly dependent on early experience.
The second section of this article reviewed the presence of sensory integration impairments in children affected with a neurodevelopmental disorder. We first studied dyslexia and paid a particular attention to the phonological-deficit and the temporal processing hypothesis, two models of special interest for developmental MSI. Overall, the literature suggests that an interrelated network of visual, auditory and heteromodal brain areas contributing to the skilled use of letter-speech sound integration might be impaired in children with dyslexia. Studies on ADHD also reported potential alterations of MSI. Children with ADHD often show sensory deficits including sensorimotor [166,167], somatosensory [168], visual [171] and auditory [172,173,174,175] processing impairments. Moreover, anatomical evidence suggested that ineffective MSI cortical maps underlying sensory-related behaviors as well as physiological reactivity to sensory stimuli found in both children with ADHD and sensory processing problems may possibly explain their comorbidity. Although certain neurodevelopmental disorders can cause MSI impairments, research and clinical applications in development are in progress and constitute promising research avenues.

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by scholarships granted by the Fonds de la Recherche du Québec-Santé, the Fonds de recherche du Québec—Nature et technologies (Emmanuelle Dionne-Dostie), the Fondation des Étoiles, CHU Ste-Justine Research Center (Emmanuelle Dionne-Dostie, Natacha Paquette) and the Canadian Institute of Health Research (Natacha Paquette). It was also supported by the Canada Research Chair program held by Maryse Lassonde and grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and the Fonds de la Recherche du Québec Santé held by Anne Gallagher and Maryse Lassonde.

Author Contributions

All authors have been involved in the preparation and have approved the submitted manuscript. Emmanuelle Dionne-Dostie was lead author and responsible for conducting the literature review and writing the manuscript. Natacha Paquette participated in the redaction of the “MSI and neurodevelopmental disorders” section. Maryse Lassonde reviewed and edited the manuscript. As the senior author, Anne Gallagher supervised Emmanuelle Dionne-Dostie and Natacha Paquette’s work, reviewed and edited the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Simon, S.A. Merging of the senses. Front. Neurosci. 2008, 2, 13–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Press, C.; Taylor-Clarke, M.; Kennett, S.; Haggard, P. Visual enhancement of touch in spatial body representation. Exp. Brain Res. 2004, 154, 238–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Diederich, A.; Colonius, H. Why two “Distractors” are better than one: Modeling the effect of non-target auditory and tactile stimuli on visual saccadic reaction time. Exp. Brain Res. 2007, 179, 43–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Lippert, M.; Logothetis, N.K.; Kayser, C. Improvement of visual contrast detection by a simultaneous sound. Brain Res. 2007, 1173, 102–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kennett, S.; Taylor-Clarke, M.; Haggard, P. Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans. Curr. Biol. 2001, 11, 1188–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Gillmeister, H.; Eimer, M. Tactile enhancement of auditory detection and perceived loudness. Brain Res. 2007, 1160, 58–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Taylor-Clarke, M.; Kennett, S.; Haggard, P. Persistence of visual-tactile enhancement in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 2004, 354, 22–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Schürmann, M.; Caetano, G.; Jousmäki, V.; Hari, R. Hands help hearing: Facilitatory audiotactile interaction at low sound-intensity levels. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2004, 115, 830–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Seitz, A.R.; Kim, R.; Shams, L. Sound facilitates visual learning. Curr. Biol. 2006, 16, 1422–1427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Ro, T.; Wallace, R.; Hagedorn, J.; Farne, A.; Pienkos, E. Visual enhancing of tactile perception in the posterior parietal cortex. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 24–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Whiteley, L.; Kennett, S.; Taylor-Clarke, M.; Haggard, P. Facilitated processing of visual stimuli associated with the body. Perception 2004, 33, 307–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Stein, B.E.; Meredith, M. Multisensory integration. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1990, 608, 51–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Robinson, C.W.; Sloutsky, V.M. Development of cross-modal processing. In Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2010; Volume 1, pp. 135–141. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bower, T.G.; Broughton, J.M.; Moore, M.K. The coordination of visual and tactual input in infants. Percept. Psychophys. 1970, 8, 51–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Walker-Andrews, A.; Lewkowicz, D.J.; Lickliter, R. Taxonomy for intermodal relations. In The Development of Intersensory Perception: Comparative Perspectives; Psychology Press: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 2013; pp. 39–56. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bahrick, L.E.; Lickliter, R. Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and perceptual learning in infancy. Dev. Psychol. 2000, 36, 190–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Marks, L.E. The Unity of the Senses: Interrelations among the Modalities; Academic Press: Waltham, MA, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  18. Bahrick, L.E. Infants’ perceptual differentiation of amodal and modality-specific audio-visual relations. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1992, 53, 180–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Bahrick, L.E. The development of infants’ sensitivity to arbitrary intermodal relations. Ecol. Psychol. 1994, 6, 111–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Streri, A. Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes in human newborns. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2003, 20, 13–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Streri, A.; Gentaz, E. Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes and handedness in human newborns. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 1365–1369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Sann, C.; Streri, A. Perception of object shape and texture in human newborns: Evidence from cross-modal transfer tasks. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 399–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Meltzoff, A.N.; Borton, R.W. Intermodal matching in human neonates. Nature 1979, 282, 403–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Gibson, E.J.; Walker, A.S. Developmental of knowledge of visual-tactual affordances of substances. Child Dev. 1984, 55, 453–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Turkewitz, G.; Gardner, J.M.; Lewkowicz, D.J. Sensory/Perceptual functioning during early infancy: The implications of a quantitative basis for responding. In Behavioral Evolution & Integrative Levels; Greenberg, G., Tobach, E., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1984; pp. 167–195. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lewkowicz, D.J.; Turkewitz, G. Cross-modal equivalence in early infancy: Auditory-visual intensity matching. Dev. Psychol. 1980, 16, 597–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gori, M.; del Viva, M.; Sandini, G.; Burr, D.C. Young children do not integrate visual and haptic form information. Curr. Biol. 2008, 18, 694–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Bahrick, L.E. Increasing specificity in perceptual development: Infants’ detection of nested levels of multimodal stimulation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2001, 79, 253–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Bahrick, L.E.; Pickens, J.N. Amodal relations: The basis for intermodal perception and learning. In The Development of Intersensory Perception: Comparative Perspectives; Lewkowicz, D., Lickliter, R., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1994; pp. 205–233. [Google Scholar]
  30. Lewkowicz, D.J. Perception of auditory-visual temporal synchrony in human infants. J. Exp. Psychol. 1996, 22, 1094–1106. [Google Scholar]
  31. Morrongiello, B.A.; Fenwick, K.; Nutley, T. Developmental changes in associations between auditory-visual events. Infant Behav. Dev. 1998, 21, 613–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Slater, A.; Quinn, P.C.; Brown, E.; Hayes, R. Intermodal perception at birth: Intersensory redundancy guides newborn infants’ learning of arbitrary auditory-visual pairings. Dev. Sci. 1999, 2, 333–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Bahrick, L.E.; Flom, R.; Lickliter, R. Intersensory redundancy facilitates discrimination of tempo in 3-month-old infants. Dev. Psychobiol. 2002, 41, 352–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Scheier, C.; Lewkowicz, D.J.; Shimojo, S. Sound induces perceptual reorganization of an ambiguous motion display in human infants. Dev. Sci. 2003, 6, 233–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Spelke, E.S. Perceiving bimodally specified events in infancy. Dev. Psychol. 1979, 15, 626–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kohl, P.K.; Meltzoff, A.N. The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science 1982, 218, 1138–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Walton, G.E.; Bower, T.G.R. Amodal representation of speech in infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 1993, 16, 233–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Patterson, M.L.; Werker, J.F. Matching phonetic information in lips and voice is robust in 4.5-month-old infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 1999, 22, 237–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Patterson, M.L.; Werker, J.F. Two-month-old infants match phonetic information in lips and voice. Dev. Sci. 2003, 6, 191–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Dodd, B. Lip reading in Infants: Attention to speech presented in- & out-of synchrony. Cogn. Psychol. 1979, 11, 478–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Flom, R.; Bahrick, L.E. The development of infant discrimination of affect in multimodal and unimodal stimulation: The role of intersensory redundancy. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 238–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Caron, A.J.; Caron, R.F.; MacLean, D.J. Infant discrimination of naturalistic emotional expressions: The role of face and voice. Child Dev. 1988, 59, 604–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Paus, T. Mapping brain development and aggression. Can. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Rev. 2005, 14, 10–15. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  44. Putzar, L.; Goerendt, I.; Lange, K.; Rösler, F.; Röder, B. Early visual deprivation impairs multisensory interactions in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 2007, 10, 1243–1245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Yu, L.; Rowland, B.A.; Stein, B.E. Initiating the development of multisensory integration by manipulating sensory experience. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 4904–4913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Hooker, D. The Prenatal Origin of Behavior; Lawrence, University of Kansas Press: Oxford, UK, 1952; pp. 56–90. [Google Scholar]
  47. Iyengar, S. Development of the human auditory system. J. Indian Inst. Sci. 2012, 92, 427–440. [Google Scholar]
  48. Atkinson, J. Human visual development over the first 6 months of life: A review and a hypothesis. Hum. Neurobiol. 1983, 3, 61–74. [Google Scholar]
  49. Brown, A.M.; Dobson, V.; Maier, J. Visual acuity of human infants at scotopic, mesopic & photopic luminances. Vis. Res. 1987, 27, 1845–1858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Werner, L.A.; Marean, G.C. Human Auditory Development; Westview Press: Boulder, CO, USA, 1996; pp. 89–131. [Google Scholar]
  51. Morrongiello, B.A.; Humphrey, G.K.; Timney, B.; Choi, J.; Rocca, P.T. Tactual object exploration & recognition in blind and sighted children. Perception 1994, 23, 833–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Rentschler, I.; Jüttner, M.; Osman, E.; Müller, A.; Caelli, T. Development of configural 3D object recognition. Behav. Brain Res. 2004, 149, 107–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Ghazanfar, A.A. The multisensory roles for auditory cortex in primate vocal communication. Hear. Res. 2009, 258, 113–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Neil, P.A.; Chee-Ruiter, C.; Scheier, C.; Lewkowicz, D.J.; Shimojo, S. Development of multisensory spatial integration & perception in humans. Dev. Sci. 2006, 9, 454–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Ernst, M.O.; Banks, M.S. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 2002, 415, 429–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Nardini, M.; Jones, P.; Bedford, R.; Braddick, O. Development of cue integration in human navigation. Curr. Biol. 2008, 18, 689–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Nardini, M.; Bedford, R.; Mareschal, D. Fusion of visual cues is not mandatory in children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2010, 107, 17041–17046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Gori, M.; Sandini, G.; Burr, D. Development of visuo-auditory integration in space and time. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Burr, D.; Gori, M. Multisensory integration develops late in humans. In Frontiers in the Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes; Murray, M.M., Wallace, M.T., Eds.; Taylor & Francis Group: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2012; pp. 345–363. [Google Scholar]
  60. Driver, J.; Spence, C. Multisensory perception: Beyond modularity and convergence. Curr. Biol. 2000, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Shams, L.; Kamitani, Y.; Shimojo, S. Visual illusion induced by sound. Cogn. Brain Res. 2002, 14, 147–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Van Wassenhove, V.; Grant, K.W.; Poeppel, D. Temporal window of integration in auditory-visual speech perception. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 598–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Hillock, A.R.; Powers, A.R.; Wallace, M.T. Binding of sights and sounds: Age-related changes in multisensory temporal processing. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 461–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Barutchu, A.; Crewther, D.P.; Crewther, S.G. The race that precedes coactivation: Development of multisensory facilitation in children. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 464–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Barutchu, A.; Danaher, J.; Crewther, S.G.; Innes-Brown, H.; Shivdasani, M.N.; Paolini, A.G. Audiovisual integration in noise by children and adults. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2010, 105, 38–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Sparks, D.L.; Hartwich-Young, R. The deep layers of the superior colliculus. Rev. Oculomot. Res. 1989, 3, 213–255. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  67. Burnett, L.R.; Stein, B.E.; Chaponis, D.; Wallace, M.T. Superior colliculus lesions preferentially disrupt multisensory orientation. Neuroscience 2004, 124, 535–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Lomber, S.G.; Payne, B.R.; Cornwell, P. Role of the superior colliculus in analyses of space: Superficial and intermediate layer contributions to visual orienting, auditory orienting, and visuospatial discriminations during unilateral and bilateral deactivations. J. Comp. Neurol. 2001, 441, 44–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Jay, M.F.; Sparks, D.L. Sensorimotor integration in the primate superior colliculus. I. Motor convergence. J. Neurophysiol. 1987, 57, 22–34. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  70. Jay, M.F.; Sparks, D.L. Sensorimotor integration in the primate superior colliculus. II. Coordinates of auditory signals. J. Neurophysiol. 1987, 57, 35–55. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  71. Stein, B.E.; Meredith, M.A.; Wallace, M.T. Development and neural basis of multisensory integration. In The Development of Intersensory Perception: Comparative Perspectives; David, J., Lewkowicz, D.J., Lickliter, R., Eds.; Lawrence Elbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1994; pp. 81–105. [Google Scholar]
  72. Wallace, M.T.; Stein, B.E. Development of multisensory neurons & multisensory integration in cat superior colliculus. J. Neurosci. 1997, 17, 2429–2444. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  73. Wallace, M.T.; Carriere, B.N.; Perrault, T.J.; Vaughan, J.W.; Stein, B.E. The development of cortical multisensory integration. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 11844–11849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Wallace, M.T.; McHaffie, J.G.; Stein, B.E. Visual response properties and visuotopic representation in the newborn monkey superior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol. 1997, 78, 2732–2741. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  75. Wallace, M.T.; Stein, B.E. Sensory organization of the superior colliculus in cat and monkey. Prog. Brain Res. 1996, 112, 301–311. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  76. Wallace, M.T.; Stein, B.E. Early experience determines how the senses will interact. J. Neurophysiol. 2007, 97, 921–926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Hensch, T.K. Critical period regulation. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 27, 549–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Streri, A.; de Hevia, M.D.D.; Izard, V.; Coubart, A. What do we know about neonatal cognition? Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 154–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Meredith, M.A.; Stein, B.E. Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science 1983, 221, 389–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Molholm, S.; Sehatpour, P.; Mehta, A.D.; Shpaner, M.; Gomez-Ramirez, M.; Ortigue, S.; Dyke, J.P.; Schwartz, T.H.; Foxe, J.J. Audio-visual multisensory integration in superior parietal lobule revealed by human intracranial recordings. J. Neurophysiol. 2006, 96, 721–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Nagy, A.; Eördegh, G.; Paróczy, Z.; Márkus, Z.; Benedek, G. Multisensory integration in the basal ganglia. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006, 24, 917–924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Sugihara, T.; Diltz, M.D.; Averbeck, B.B.; Romanski, L.M. Integration of auditory and visual communication information in the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 11138–11147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Falchier, A.; Clavagnier, S.; Barone, P.; Kennedy, H. Anatomical evidence of multimodal integration in primate striate cortex. J. Neurosci. 2002, 22, 5749–5759. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  84. Foxe, J.J.; Schroeder, C.E. The case for feedforward multisensory convergence during early cortical processing. Neuroreport 2005, 16, 419–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Driver, J.; Noesselt, T. Multisensory interplay reveals crossmodal influences on “sensory-specific” brain regions, neural responses, and judgments. Neuron 2008, 57, 11–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Ghazanfar, A.A.; Schroeder, C.E. Is neocortex essentially multisensory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 278–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Reynolds, G.D.; Bahrick, L.E.; Lickliter, R.; Guy, M.W. Neural correlates of intersensory processing in 5-month-old infants. Dev. Psychobiol. 2014, 56, 355–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Brandwein, A.B.; Foxe, J.J.; Russo, N.N.; Altschuler, T.S.; Gomes, H.; Molholm, S. The development of audiovisual multisensory integration across childhood and early adolescence: A high-density electrical mapping study. Cereb. Cortex 2011, 21, 1042–1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Brett-Green, B.A.; Miller, L.J.; Gavin, W.J.; Davies, P.L. Multisensory integration in children: A preliminary ERP study. Brain Res. 2008, 1242, 283–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Stephen, J.M.; Romero, L.; Zhang, T.; Okada, Y. Auditory and somatosensory integration in infants. Int. Congr. Ser. 2007, 1300, 107–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Bremner, A.J.; Spence, C. Unimodal experience constrains while multisensory experience enriches cognitive construction. Behav. Brain Sci. 2008, 31, 335–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Jordan, K.E.; Suanda, S.H.; Brannon, E.M. Intersensory redundancy accelerates preverbal numerical competence. Cognition 2008, 108, 210–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. Feigenson, L. Predicting sights from sounds: 6-month-olds’ intermodal numerical abilities. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2011, 110, 347–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Jordan, K.E.; Baker, J. Multisensory information boosts numerical matching abilities in young children. Dev. Sci. 2011, 14, 205–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Bahrick, L.E.; Lickliter, R. Intersensory redundancy guides early perceptual and cognitive development. In Advances in Child Development and Behavior; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2002; Volume 30, pp. 153–187. [Google Scholar]
  96. Gibson, E.J. Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development; Appleton: New York, NY, USA, 1969. [Google Scholar]
  97. Thelen, E.; Smith, L. A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  98. Birch, H.G.; Belmont, L. Auditory-visual integration in normal and retarded readers. Am. J. Orthopsychiatr. 1964, 34, 852–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Rose, S.A.; Feldman, J.F.; Wallace, I.F. Infant information processing in relation to six-year cognitive outcomes. Child Dev. 1992, 63, 1126–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Rose, S.A.; Feldman, J.F.; Futterweit, L.R.; Jankowski, J.J. Continuity in tactual-visual cross-modal transfer: Infancy to 11 years. Dev. Psychol. 1998, 34, 435–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Barutchu, A.; Crewther, S.G.; Fifer, J.; Shivdasani, M.N.; Innes-Brown, H.; Toohey, S.; Danaher, J.; Paolini, A.G. The relationship between multisensory integration and IQ in children. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 47, 877–885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Ayres, A.J. Sensory Integration and Learning Disabilities; Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1972. [Google Scholar]
  103. Children and Neurodevelopmental Behavioral Disorders (NDBID). World Health Organization Training Package for the Health Sector. Available online: http://www.who.int/ceh/capacity/neurodevelopmental.pdf (accessed on 20 May 2014).
  104. McManus, B.M.; Robert, S.A.; Albanese, A.; Sadek-Badawi, M.; Palta, M. Relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and social function of Wisconsin 2-and 3-year-olds born at very low birth weight. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2011, 165, 119–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Waber, D.P. Rethinking Learning Disabilities: Understanding Children Who Struggle in School; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 3–241. [Google Scholar]
  106. American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V), 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013; pp. 5–947. [Google Scholar]
  107. Shaywitz, S.E. Dyslexia. N. Engl. J. Med. 1998, 338, 307–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Silverman, L.K. Upside-Down Brilliance: The Visual-Spatial Learner; The Institute for the Study of Advanced Development: Denver, CO, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  109. Fletcher, J.M. Dyslexia: The evolution of a scientific concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2009, 15, 501–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Ramus, F. Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2003, 13, 212–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Snowling, M.J. From language to reading and dyslexia1. Dyslexia 2001, 7, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Vellutino, F.R.; Fletcher, J.M.; Snowling, M.J.; Scanlon, D.M. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2004, 45, 2–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Berninger, V.W.; Raskind, W.; Richards, T.; Abbott, R.; Stock, P. A multidisciplinary approach to understanding developmental dyslexia within working-memory architecture: Genotypes, phenotypes, brain, and instruction. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2008, 33, 707–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Menghini, D.; Finzi, A.; Carlesimo, G.A.; Vicari, S. Working memory impairment in children with developmental dyslexia: Is it just a phonological deficity? Dev. Neuropsychol. 2011, 36, 199–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Helland, T.; Asbjørnsen, A. Executive functions in dyslexia. Child Neuropsychol. 2000, 6, 37–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Brosnan, M.; Demetre, J.; Hamill, S.; Robson, K.; Shepherd, H.; Cody, G. Executive functioning in adults and children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 2002, 40, 2144–2155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Reiter, A.; Tucha, O.; Lange, K.W. Executive functions in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia 2005, 11, 116–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Altemeier, L.E.; Abbott, R.D.; Berninger, V.W. Executive functions for reading and writing in typical literacy development and dyslexia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2008, 30, 588–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Thomson, M. Monitoring dyslexics’ intelligence and attainments: A follow-up study. Dyslexia 2003, 9, 3–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Willcutt, E.G.; Pennington, B.F.; Chhabildas, N.A.; Olson, R.K.; Hulslander, J.L. Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: In search of the common deficit. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2005, 27, 35–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Shanahan, M.A.; Pennington, B.F.; Yerys, B.E.; Scott, A.; Boada, R.; Willcutt, E.G.; Olson, R.K.; DeFries, J.C. Processing speed deficits in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2006, 34, 585–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Marzocchi, G.M.; Ornaghi, S.; Barboglio, S. What are the causes of the attention deficits observed in children with dyslexia? Child Neuropsychol. 2009, 15, 567–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Stanovich, K.E. Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. J. Learn. Disabil. 1988, 21, 590–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Pennington, B.F.; Gilger, J.W.; Pauls, D.; Smith, S.A.; Smith, S.D.; DeFries, J.C. Evidence for major gene transmission of developmental dyslexia. JAMA 1991, 266, 1527–1534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Tallal, P.; Miller, S.; Fitch, R.H. Neurobiological basis of speech: A case for the preeminence of temporal processing. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1993, 682, 27–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Massaro, D.W.; Cohen, M.M.; Smeele, P.M. Perception of asynchronous and conflicting visual and auditory speech. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1996, 100, 1777–1786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Mayringer, H.; Wimmer, H. Pseudoname learning by German-speaking children with dyslexia: Evidence for a phonological learning deficit. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2000, 75, 116–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Windfuhr, K.L.; Snowling, M.J. The relationship between paired associate learning and phonological skills in normally developing readers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2001, 80, 160–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  129. Share, D.L. Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 1995, 55, 151–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Ehri, L.C. Development of sight word reading: Phases and findings. In The Science of Reading: A Handbook; Snowling, M.J., Hulme, C., Eds.; Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UK, 2005; pp. 135–145. [Google Scholar]
  131. Ziegler, J.C.; Pech-Georgel, C.; Dufau, S.; Grainger, J. Rapid processing of letters, digits and symbols: What purely visual-attentional deficit in developmental dyslexia? Dev. Sci. 2010, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Blau, V.; Reithler, J.; van Atteveldt, N.; Seitz, J.; Gerretsen, P.; Goebel, R.; Blomert, L. Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. Brain 2010, 133, 868–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Plaza, M.; Cohen, H. Influence of auditory-verbal, visual-verbal, visual, and visual-visual processing speed on reading and spelling at the end of Grade 1. Brain Cogn. 2005, 57, 189–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  134. Stein, J.; Walsh, V. To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends Neurosci. 1997, 20, 147–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  135. Fischer, B.; Hartnegg, K.; Mokler, A. Dynamic visual perception of dyslexic children. Perception 2000, 29, 523–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Laasonen, M.; Virsu, V.J. Temporal order and processing acuity of visual, auditory, and tactile perception in developmentally dyslexic young adults. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2001, 1, 394–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  137. Stein, J. The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 2001, 7, 12–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Fischer, B.; Hartnegg, K. On the development of low-level auditory discrimination and deficits in dyslexia. Dyslexia 2004, 10, 105–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. McGurk, H.; MacDonald, J. Hearing lips & seeing voices. Nature 1976, 264, 746–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  140. Hayes, E.A.; Tiippana, K.; Nicol, T.G.; Sams, M.; Kraus, N. Integration of heard and seen speech: A factor in learning disabilities in children. Neurosci. Lett. 2003, 351, 46–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  141. Rose, S.A.; Feldman, J.F.; Jankowski, J.J.; Futterweit, L.R. Visual and auditory temporal processing, cross-modal transfer, and reading. J. Learn. Disabil. 1999, 32, 256–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  142. Laasonen, M.; Tomma-Halme, J.; Lahti-Nuuttila, P.; Service, E.; Virsu, V. Rate of information segregation in developmentally dyslexic children. Brain Lang. 2000, 75, 66–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. Vieira, S.; Quercia, P.; Michel, C.; Pozzo, T.; Bonnetblanc, F. Cognitive demands impair postural control in developmental dyslexia: A negative effect that can be compensated. Neurosci. Lett. 2009, 462, 125–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  144. Bucci, M.P.; Gerard, C.L.; Bui-Quoc, E. The effect of a cognitive task on the postural control of dyslexic children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 3727–3735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  145. Barela, J.A.; Dias, J.L.; Godoi, D.; Viana, A.R.; de Freitas, P.B. Postural control and automaticity in dyslexic children: The relationship between visual information and body sway. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 1814–1821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Quercia, P.; Demougeot, L.; Dos Santos, M.; Bonnetblanc, F. Integration of proprioceptive signals and attentional capacity during postural control are impaired but subject to improvement in dyslexic children. Exp. Brain Res. 2011, 209, 599–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  147. Welcome, S.E.; Chiarello, C.; Thompson, P.M.; Sowell, E.R. Reading skill is related to individual differences in brain structure in college students. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011, 32, 1194–1205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Temple, E.; Poldrack, R.A.; Salidis, J.; Deutsch, G.K.; Tallal, P.; Merzenich, M.M.; Gabrieli, J.D. Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: An fMRI study. Neuroreport 2001, 12, 299–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  149. Simos, P.G.; Breier, J.I.; Fletcher, J.M.; Bergman, E.; Papanicolaou, A.C. Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: A magnetic source imaging approach. Cereb. Cortex 2000, 10, 809–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  150. Simos, P.G.; Breier, J.I.; Fletcher, J.M.; Foorman, B.R.; Bergman, E.; Fishbeck, K.; Papanicolaou, A.C. Brain activation profiles in dyslexic children during non-word reading: A magnetic source imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 2000, 290, 61–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Van Atteveldt, N.; Formisano, E.; Goebel, R.; Blomert, L. Integration of letters and speech sounds in the human brain. Neuron 2004, 43, 271–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Van Atteveldt, N.M.; Formisano, E.; Blomert, L.; Goebel, R. The effect of temporal asynchrony on the multisensory integration of letters and speech sounds. Cereb Cortex 2007, 4, 962–974. [Google Scholar]
  153. Georgiewa, P.; Rzanny, R.; Gaser, C.; Gerhard, U.J.; Vieweg, U.; Freesmeyer, D.; Mentzel, H.J.; Kaiser, W.A.; Blanz, B. Phonological processing in dyslexic children: A study combining functional imaging and event related potentials. Neurosci. Lett. 2002, 318, 5–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  154. Maisog, J.M.; Einbinder, E.R.; Flowers, D.L.; Turkeltaub, P.E.; Eden, G.F. A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of dyslexia. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1145, 237–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  155. Gabrieli, J.D. Dyslexia: A new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science 2009, 325, 280–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  156. Eckert, M.A.; Leonard, C.M.; Richards, T.L.; Aylward, E.H.; Thomson, J.; Berninger, V.W. Anatomical correlates of dyslexia: Frontal and cerebellar findings. Brain 2003, 126, 482–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  157. Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A.; Pugh, K.R.; Fulbright, R.K.; Constable, R.T.; Mencl, W.E.; Shankweiler, D.P.; Liberman, A.M.; Skudlarski, P.; Fletcher, J.M.; et al. Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 1998, 95, 2636–2641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  158. Pugh, K.R.; Mencl, W.E.; Shaywitz, B.A.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Fulbright, R.K.; Constable, R.T.; Skudlarski, P.; Marchione, K.E.; Jenner, A.R.; Fletcher, J.M.; et al. The angular gyrus in developmental dyslexia: Task-specific differences in functional connectivity within posterior cortex. Psychol. Sci. 2000, 11, 51–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  159. Hashimoto, R.; Kuniyoshi, L.S. Learning letters in adulthood: Direct visualization of cortical plasticity for forming a new link between orthography and phonology. Neuron 2004, 42, 311–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  160. Schachar, R. The MTA Study: Implications for medication management. ADHD Rep. 2000, 8, 2–6. [Google Scholar]
  161. Goldman, L.S.; Genel, M.; Bezman, R.J.; Slanetz, P.J. Diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. JAMA 1998, 279, 1100–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  162. Mangeot, S.D.; Miller, L.J.; McIntosh, D.N.; McGrath-Clarke, J.; Simon, J.; Hagerman, R.J.; Goldson, E. Sensory modulation dysfunction in children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2001, 43, 399–406. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  163. Willcutt, E.G. The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics 2012, 9, 490–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  164. Pennington, B.F.; Ozonoff, S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1996, 37, 51–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  165. Barkley, R.A. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 1997, 121, 65–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  166. Oosterlaan, J.; Logan, G.D.; Sergeant, J.A. Response inhibition in ADHD, CD, comorbid ADHD + CD, anxious, and control children: A meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1998, 39, 411–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  167. Tannock, R. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advances in cognitive, neurobiological, and genetic research. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1998, 39, 65–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  168. Schulz, K.P.; Himelstein, J.; Halperin, J.M.; Newcorn, J.H. Neurobiological models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A brief review of the empirical evidence. CNS Spectr. 2000, 5, 34–44. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  169. Nyden, A.; Billstedt, E.; Hjelmquist, E.; Gillberg, C. Neurocognitive stability in Asperger syndrome, ADHD, and reading and writing disorder: A pilot study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2001, 43, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  170. Mayes, S.D.; Calhoun, S.L. The gordon diagnostic system and WISC-III freedom from distractibility index: Validity in identifying clinic-referred children with and without ADHD. Psychol. Rep. 2002, 91, 575–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  171. Rucklidge, J.J.; Tannock, R. Neuropsychological profiles of adolescents with ADHD: Effects of reading difficulties and gender. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2002, 43, 988–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  172. Vance, A.L.; Maruff, P.; Barnett, R. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, combined type: Better executive function performance with longer-term psychostimulant medication. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2003, 37, 570–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  173. Pliszka, S.; Glahn, D.; Semrud-Clikeman, M.; Franklin, C.; Perez, R., III; Liotti, M. Neuroimaging of inhibitory control in treatment naive and chronically treated children with ADHD. Am. J. Psychiatry 2006, 163, 1052–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  174. Semrud-Clikeman, M.; Plizska, S.; Liotti, M.; Higgins, K.; Lancaster, J.L. Neuroimaging in children with ADHD, treated and treatment naive. Neurology 2006, 67, 1023–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  175. Ek, U.; Fernell, E.; Westerlund, J.; Holmberg, K.; Olsson, P.O.; Gillberg, C. Cognitive strengths and deficits in schoolchildren with ADHD. Acta Paediatr. 2007, 96, 756–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  176. Semrud-Clikeman, M.; Pliszka, S.; Liotti, M. Executive functioning in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Combined type with and without a stimulant medication history. Neuropsychology 2008, 22, 329–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  177. Cermak, S.A. Somatodyspraxia. In Sensory Integration: Theory and Practice; Fisher, A.G., Murray, E.A., Bundy, A.C., Eds.; F.A. Davis Company: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1991; pp. 137–170. [Google Scholar]
  178. Parush, S.; Sohmer, H.; Steinberg, A.; Kaitz, M. Somatosensory functioning in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 1997, 39, 464–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  179. Miller, L.J.; Reisman, J.; McIntosh, D.N.; Simon, J. An ecological model of sensory modulation. In The Nature of Sensory Integration with Diverse Population; Smith-Roley, S., Imperatore-Blanche, E., Schaaf, R.C., Eds.; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2001; pp. 57–88. [Google Scholar]
  180. Engel-Yeger, B.; Ziv-On, D. The relationship between sensory processing difficulties and leisure activity preference of children with different types of ADHD. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 1154–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  181. Dunn, W.; Bennett, D. Patterns of sensory processing in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Occup. Ther. J. Res. 2002, 22, 4–15. [Google Scholar]
  182. Ayres, A.J. Sensory Integration and the Child; Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  183. Fisher, A. Vestibular-proprioceptive processing and bilateral integration and sequencing deficits. In Sensory Integration: Principles and Theory; Fisher, A., Murray, E., Bundy, A., Eds.; F.A. Davis: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1991; pp. 71–107. [Google Scholar]
  184. Hassan, D.M.; Azzam, H. Sensory Integration in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Implications to Postural Control. In Contemporary trends in ADHD research; Norvilitis, J.M., Ed.; INTECH Open Access: Rijeka, Croatia, 2012; pp. 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  185. Guskiewicz, K.; Perrin, D. Research and clinical applications of assessing balance. J. Sport Rehabil. 1996, 5, 45–53. [Google Scholar]
  186. Hern, K.L.; Hynd, G.W. Clinical differentiation of the attention deficit disorder subtypes: Do sensorimotor deficits characterize children with ADD/WO? Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1992, 7, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  187. Ghanizadeh, A. Visual fields in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder before and after treatment with stimulants. Acta Ophthalmol. 2010, 88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  188. Cook, J.R.; Mausbach, T.; Burd, L.; Gascon, G.G.; Slotnick, H.B.; Patterson, B.; Johnson, R.D.; Hankey, B.; Reynolds, B.W. A preliminary study of the relationship between central auditory processing disorder and attention deficit disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 1993, 18, 130–137. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  189. Riccig, C.A.; Hynd, G.W. Relationship between ADHD and central auditory processing disorder a review of the literature. Sch. Psychol. Int. 1996, 17, 235–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  190. Ghanizadeh, A. Screening signs of auditory processing problem: Does it distinguish attention deficit hyperactivity disorder subtypes in a clinical sample of children? Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2009, 73, 81–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  191. Cheung, P.P.; Siu, A.M. A comparison of patterns of sensory processing in children with and without developmental disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2009, 30, 1468–1480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  192. Koziol, L.F.; Budding, D. ADHD and sensory processing disorders: Placing the diagnostic issues in context. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 2012, 1, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  193. Posner, M. Progress in attention research. In Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, 2nd ed.; Posner, M., Ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 3–8. [Google Scholar]
  194. Krain, A.L.; Castellanos, F.X. Brain development and ADHD. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 26, 433–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  195. Faraone, S.V.; Biederman, J. Neurobiology of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 1998, 44, 951–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  196. Bush, G.; Valera, E.M.; Seidman, L.J. Functional neuroimaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review and suggested future directions. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 1273–1284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  197. Krienen, F.M.; Buckner, R.L. Segregated fronto-cerebellar circuits revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Cereb. Cortex 2009, 19, 2485–2497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  198. Seidman, L.J.; Valera, E.M.; Bush, G. Brain function and structure in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2004, 27, 323–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  199. Seidman, L.J.; Valera, E.M.; Makris, N. Structural brain imaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 1263–1272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  200. Sergeant, J.A.; Geurts, H.; Oosterlaan, J. How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behav. Brain Res. 2002, 130, 3–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  201. Spencer, T.J.; Biederman, J.; Mick, E. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Diagnosis, lifespan, comorbidities, and neurobiology. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2007, 32, 631–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  202. Stevens, M.C.; Kiehl, K.A.; Pearlson, G.D.; Calhoun, V.D. Functional neural networks underlying response inhibition in adolescents and adults. Behav. Brain Res. 2007, 181, 12–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  203. Stevens, M.C.; Kiehl, K.A.; Pearlson, G.D.; Calhoun, V.D. Brain network dynamics during error commission. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 24–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  204. Castellanos, F.X.; Lee, P.P.; Sharp, W.; Jeffries, N.O.; Greenstein, D.K.; Clasen, L.S.; Blumenthal, J.D.; James, R.S.; Ebens, C.L.; Walter, J.M.; et al. Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA 2002, 288, 1740–1748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  205. Castellanos, F.X.; Giedd, J.N.; Marsh, W.L.; Hamburger, S.D.; Vaituzis, A.C.; Dickstein, D.P.; Sarfatti, S.E.; Vauss, Y.C.; Snell, J.W.; Lange, N.; et al. Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1996, 53, 607–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  206. Yeo, R.A.; Hill, D.E.; Campbell, R.A.; Vigil, J.; Petropoulos, H.; Hart, B.; Zamora, L.; Brooks, W.M. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy investigation of the right frontal lobe in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2003, 42, 303–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  207. Durston, S.; Pol, H.E.H.; Schnack, H.G.; Buitelaar, J.K.; Steenhuis, M.P.; Minderaa, R.B.; Kahn, R.S. Magnetic resonance imaging of boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2004, 43, 332–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  208. Tian, L.; Jiang, T.; Liang, M.; Zang, Y.; He, Y.; Sui, M.; Wang, Y. Enhanced resting-state brain activities in ADHD patients: A fMRI study. Brain Dev. 2008, 30, 342–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  209. Kim, Y.H.; Gitelman, D.R.; Nobre, A.C.; Parrish, T.B.; LaBar, K.S.; Mesulam, M. The large-scale neural network for spatial attention displays multifunctional overlap but differential asymmetry. Neuroimage 1999, 9, 269–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  210. Corbetta, M.; Shulman, G.L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 201–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  211. Talsma, D.; Senkowski, D.; Soto-Faraco, S.; Woldorff, M.G. The multifaceted interplay between attention and multisensory integration. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010, 14, 400–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  212. Koziol, L.F.; Budding, D.E.; Chidekel, D. Sensory integration, sensory processing, and sensory modulation disorders: Putative functional neuroanatomic underpinnings. Cerebellum 2011, 10, 770–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  213. Koziol, L.F.; Budding, D.E.; Chidekel, D. From movement to thought: Executive function, embodied cognition, and the cerebellum. Cerebellum 2012, 11, 505–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  214. Chudler, E.H.; Sugiyama, K.; Dong, W.K. Multisensory convergence and integration in the neostriatum and globus pallidus of the rat. Brain Res. 1995, 674, 33–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  215. Nagy, A.; Paróczy, Z.; Norita, M.; Benedek, G. Multisensory responses and receptive field properties of neurons in the substantia nigra and in the caudate nucleus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005, 22, 419–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  216. Hill, D.E.; Yeo, R.A.; Campbell, R.A.; Hart, B.; Vigil, J.; Brooks, W. Magnetic resonance imaging correlates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children. Neuropsychology 2003, 17, 496–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  217. Casey, B.J.; Epstein, J.N.; Buhle, J.; Liston, C.; Davidson, M.C.; Tonev, S.T.; Spicer, J.; Niogi, S.; Millner, A.J.; Reiss, A.; et al. Frontostriatal connectivity and its role in cognitive control in parent-child dyads with ADHD. Am. J. Psychiatry 2007, 164, 1729–1736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  218. Mackie, S.; Shaw, P.; Lenroot, R.; Pierson, R.; Greenstein, D.K.; Nugent, T.F., III; Sharp, W.S.; Giedd, J.N.; Rapoport, J. Cerebellar development and clinical outcome in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2007, 164, 647–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  219. Bledsoe, J.; Semrud-Clikeman, M.; Pliszka, S.R. A magnetic resonance imaging study of the cerebellar vermis in chronically treated and treatment-naive children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder combined type. Biol. Psychiatry 2009, 65, 620–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Dionne-Dostie, E.; Paquette, N.; Lassonde, M.; Gallagher, A. Multisensory Integration and Child Neurodevelopment. Brain Sci. 2015, 5, 32-57. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci5010032

AMA Style

Dionne-Dostie E, Paquette N, Lassonde M, Gallagher A. Multisensory Integration and Child Neurodevelopment. Brain Sciences. 2015; 5(1):32-57. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci5010032

Chicago/Turabian Style

Dionne-Dostie, Emmanuelle, Natacha Paquette, Maryse Lassonde, and Anne Gallagher. 2015. "Multisensory Integration and Child Neurodevelopment" Brain Sciences 5, no. 1: 32-57. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci5010032

APA Style

Dionne-Dostie, E., Paquette, N., Lassonde, M., & Gallagher, A. (2015). Multisensory Integration and Child Neurodevelopment. Brain Sciences, 5(1), 32-57. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci5010032

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop