Metacognitive Precursors: An Analysis in Children with Different Disabilities
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- Research Issue 1: To determine the reliability and validity indices of the “Scale Measuring Precursor Metacognitive Skills (SMPMS)” in children with disabilities.
- Research Issue 2: To determine the functional relationship between metacognitive precursors in children and different types of disability.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
- Group 1. Global developmental delay (GDD) with GDQ between 55 and 70, as measured with the Brunet-Lezine Revised Test of Psychomotor Development in Early Childhood (BLRT). This group had 29 subjects: 12 boys and 17 girls.
- Group 2. GDD with GDQ between 70 and 80, measured with the BLRT. This group included 16 subjects: 7 boys and 9 girls.
- Group 3. Comprehensive Language Disorder (CLD) with GDQ between 55 and 70, as measured with the Reynell Developmental Language Scales III (RDLS-III) [38]. This group comprised 15 subjects: 6 boys and 9 girls.
- Group 4. Psychomotor Delay (PD) with GDQ between 60 and 80, as measured with the BLRT. This group was made up of 12 subjects: 4 boys and 8 girls.
- Group 5. Expressive language disorder (ELD) with GDQ between 55 and 70, as measured with the RLDS-III. This group included 15 subjects: 7 boys and 8 girls.
2.2. Research Design
2.3. Instruments
2.3.1. Brunet-Lezine Revised Test of Psychomotor Development in Early Childhood (BLRT)
2.3.2. Reynell Developmental Language Scales III (RDLS-III)
2.3.3. Symbolic Play Test (SPT)
2.3.4. Scale Measuring Precursor Metacognitive Skills (SMPMS)
2.4. Procedure
2.5. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Study 1
3.2. Study 2
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Items | Rating | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. The child maintains sustained attention when the adult proposes a task. | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
2. The child understands sentences of six elements and carries out the externally motivated actions. | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
3. The child understands a discourse. | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
4. The child can issue short sentences but without discourse while performing externally motivated actions. | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
5. The child can emit simple sentences with limitations in the topic of conversation. | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
6. The adult’s language can direct short functional actions. | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
7. The child performs autonomous actions of lengthy process without prior planning. | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
8. The child performs externally motivated actions without prior planning. | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
References
- Flavell, J.H. Cognitive monitoring. In Chlidren´s Oral Comunications Skills; Dickson, W., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1981; pp. 35–60. ISBN 9780122154508. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, A.L. Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation and other more mysterious mechanism. In Metacognition, Motivations, Motivation and Understanding; Weinert, F.E., Kluwe, H., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1987; pp. 65–116. ISBN 978-0898595697. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman, B.J. Theories of self-regulated learning-An overview and analysis. In Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Achievement: Theorical Perspectives, 2nd ed.; Zimmerman, B.J., Schunk, D.H., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2001; Volume 1, pp. 191–226. ISBN 1-4106-0103. [Google Scholar]
- Pintrich, P.R. The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In Handbook of Self-Regulation; Pintrich, P.R., Boekaerts, M., Zeidner, M., Eds.; Academic Press: Orlando, FL, USA, 2000; Volume 1, pp. 452–502. ISBN 978-0-12-109890-2. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson, T.O.; Narens, L. Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Bower, G., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1990; Volume 27, pp. 125–173. ISBN 9780125433273. [Google Scholar]
- Gómez, J.C. Apes, Monkeys, Children, and the Growth of Mind; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA; London, UK, 2004; ISBN 0-674-01145-7. [Google Scholar]
- Sáiz, M.C.; Carbonero, M.A.; Flores, V. Precursors of the Theory of Mind in children with disability. Cadmo 2014, 12, 25–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitebread, D.; Coltman, P.; Pasternak, D.P.; Sangster, C.; Grau, V.; Bingham, S.; Demetriou, D. The development of two observational tools for assessing metacognition and self-regulated learning in young children. Metacogn. Learn. 2009, 4, 63–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuvalja, M.; Verma, M.; Whitebread, D. Patterns of co-occurring non-verbal behaviour and self-directed speech: A comparison of three methodological approaches. Metacogn. Learn. 2014, 9, 87–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carbonero, M.A.; Sáiz, M.C.; Flores, V. Effect of a metacognitive training program of mentalist skills. Psicothema 2013, 25, 31–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hanson, L.K.; Atance, C.M.; Paluck, S.W. Is thinking about the future related to theory of mind and executive function? Not in preschoolers. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 128, 120–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bryce, D.; Whitebread, D.; Szücs, D. The relationships among executive functions. Metacognitive skills and educational achievement in 5 and 7-year-old children. Metacogn. Learn. 2015, 10, 181–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Annevirta, T.; Vauras, M. Developmental changes of metacognitive skill in elementary school children. J. Exp. Educ. 2006, 74, 197–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lockl, K.; Schneider, W. Precursors of metamemory in young children: The role of theory of mind and metacognitive vocabulary. Metacogn. Learn. 2006, 1, 15–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sáiz, M.C.; Carbonero, M.A.; Flores, V. Types of responses in 4–5-year-old children in conservation, classification, and theory of mind tasks. Psicothema 2010, 22, 772–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, B.J. Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective. In Handbook of Self-Regulation; Pintrich, P.R., Boekaerts, M., Zeidner, M., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2000; Volume 1, pp. 13–39. ISBN 978-0-12-109890-2. [Google Scholar]
- Sáiz, M.C.; Román, J.M. Estimulación Mentalista en la Primera Infancia, 1st ed.; CEPE: Madrid, Spain, 2011; ISBN 9788478697915. [Google Scholar]
- McConnell, S.R.; Wackerle-Hollman, A.K.W.; Roloff, T.A.; Rodriguez, M. Designing a measurement framework for response to intervention in early childhood programs. J. Early Intervg. 2015, 36, 263–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, B.J.; Moylan, A.R. Self-regulation: Where metacognition and motivation intersect. In Handbook of Metacognition in Education; Hacker, D.J., Dunlosky, J., Graesser, A.C., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 299–315. ISBN 0-203-87642-3. [Google Scholar]
- Whitebread, D.; Basilio, M. The emergence and early development of self-regulation in young children. Revista del Curriculum y Formación del Profesorado 2012, 16, 15–33. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, R.J. Metaphors of the Mind: Conceptions of the Nature of Intelligence; University Press: Cambrige, UK, 1990; ISBN 0-521-35579-6. [Google Scholar]
- Van der Stel, M.; Veenman, M.V.J. Metacognitive skills and intellectual ability of young adolescents: A longitudinal study from a developmental perspective. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2014, 29, 117–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doherty, M.J.; Anderson, J.R.; Howieson, L. The rapid development of explicit gaze judgment ability at 3 years. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2009, 104, 296–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rivière, A.; García Nogales, A.; Nuñez, M. Teoría de la mente en Síndrome de Down: Una revaluación de la hipótesis de la “normalidad mentalista”[Theory of Mind in Down Syndrome. A reappraisal of the hypothesis of “normal mentalist”]. In Ángel Rivière Obras Escogidas Vol III: Metarrepresentación y Semiosis[Ángel Rivière Selected Works Vol III: Metarrepresentation and semiosis]; Ruiz-Vargas, J.M., Belinchón, M., Eds.; Editorial Médica Panamericana: Madrid, España, 2000; Volume 3, pp. 73–88. ISBN 9788479036188. [Google Scholar]
- Jeans, L.M.; Santos, R.M.; Laxman, D.J.; McBride, B.A.; Dyer, W.J. Early predictors of ASD in young children using a nationally representative data set. J. Early Interv. 2013, 35, 303–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sáiz, M.C.; Montero, E. Metacognition: Self-regulation and assessment in problem-solving processes at university. In Metacognition: Fundaments: Applications and Trends; Peña-Ayala, A., Ed.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2015; pp. 107–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, M.V.J. Alternative assessment of strategy use with self-report instruments: A discussion. Metacog. Learn. 2011, 6, 205–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Case, R. Intellectual Development: Birth to Adulthood; Academic Press: London, UK, 1972; ISBN 9780121628802. [Google Scholar]
- Veenman, M.V.J. Learning to self-monitor and self-regulate. In Handbook of Research on Learning and Instruction; Mayer, R., Alexander, P., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 197–218. ISBN 9780415804608. [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins, E.; Gathercole, S.; Duncan, A. Language Problems and ADHD Symptoms: How Specific Are the Links? Brain Sci. 2016, 6, 50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Karmiloff, K.; Karmiloff-Smith, A. Pathways to Language from Foetus to Adolescent; Harvard University Press: Harvard, MA, USA, 2002; ISBN 9780674008359. [Google Scholar]
- Pino Pasternak, D.; Whitebread, D.; Tolmie, A. A multidimensional analysis of parent-child interactions during academic tasks and their relationships with children. Cogn. Instr. 2010, 28, 219–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, M.V.J.; Bavelaar, L.; De Wolf, L.; Van Haaren, M.G.P. The on-line assessment of metacognitive skills in a computerized learning environment. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2014, 29, 123–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chatzipanteli, A.; Grammatikopoulos, V.; Gregoriadis, A. Development and evaluation of metacognition in early childhood education. Early Child Dev. Care 2014, 184, 1223–1232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sáiz, M.C.; Escolar, C. Observación Sistemática e Investigación en Contextos Educativos [Systematic Observation and Research in Educational Contexts], 1st ed.; Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Burgos: Burgos, Spain, 2013; ISBN 978-84-92681-70-9s. [Google Scholar]
- Perner, J. MiniMeta: In Search of Minimal Criteria for Metacognition. Foundations of Metacognition; Beran, M.J., Brandl, J., Perner, J., Proust, J., Eds.; Oxford Scholarship Online: Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Josse, D. Brunet-Lézine Reviewed. Scale of Psychomotor Development in Early Infancy; Psymtéc: Madrid, Spain, 1997; ISBN 9789210079662. [Google Scholar]
- Reynell, J.K.; Gruber, C.P. Reynell Developmental Language Scales; MEPSA: Madrid, Spain, 1990; ISBN 9783809425908. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Text Revision, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-0-89042-555-8. [Google Scholar]
- International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Revision, Clinical Modification (ICD-10-CM). Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/icd/FY2018_Preface.pdf (accessed on 19 October 2017).
- Campbell, D.T.; Stanley, J.C. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1966; ISBN 9780528614002. [Google Scholar]
- Lowe, M.; Costello, A. Symbolic Play Test; NFER-Nelson: Windsor Britain, UK, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Bollen, K.A.; Stine, R. Bootstrapping Goodness of Fit Measures in Structural Equation Models. In Testing Structural Equation Models; Bollen, K.A., Long, J.S., Eds.; Sage: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 1993; pp. 111–135. ISBN 978-0803945074. [Google Scholar]
- Bandalos, D.L.; Finney, S.J. Item parceling issues in structural equation modeling. In New Development and Techniques in Structural Equation Modeling, 1st ed.; Marcoulides, G.A., Schumacker, R.E., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2001; pp. 269–296. ISBN 978-0805835939. [Google Scholar]
- Desarrollo de un Ejemplo de análisis Factorial Confirmatorio con LISREL. AMOS y SAS. Available online: http://www.benitoarias.com/articulos/afc.pdf (accessed on 19 October 2017).
- Arias, B.; Verdugo, M.A.; Navas, P.; Gómez, L.E. Factor structure of the construct of adaptive behaviour in children with and without intellectual disability. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2013, 13, 155–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Uriel, E.; Aldás, J. Análisis multivariante aplicado; Thomson: Madrid, Spain, 2005; ISBN 9788497323727. [Google Scholar]
- Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Cut off criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jöreskog, K.G.; Sörbom, D. LISREL 8: Structural Equation Modeling with the SIMPLIS Command Language; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1993; ISBN 978-0894980336. [Google Scholar]
- Rial-Boubeta, A.; Varela Mallou, J.; Abalo Piñeiro, J.; Lévy Mangin, J.-P. El Análisis Factorial Confirmatorio. In Modelización con Estructuras de Covarianzas en Ciencias Sociales, 1st ed.; Lévy Mangin, J.-P., Varela Mallou, A., Eds.; Netbiblo: Coruña, Spain, 2006; pp. 119–154. ISBN 9788497451369. [Google Scholar]
Descriptive Statistics and Ranges | Boys | Girls |
---|---|---|
Mean Age | 30.40 months | 30.95 months |
SD Age | 7.02 | 4.55 |
Age range | 25–77 months | 25–77 months |
Mean PDQ | 70.17 | 67.12 |
Mean CDQ | 63.91 | 64.28 |
Mean ELDQ | 59.57 | 66.36 |
Mean CLDQ | 62.81 | 66.86 |
Mean SDQ | 65.63 | 66.71 |
Quotient range | 55–77 | 55–75 |
Items | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1.1 The child maintains sustained attention when the adult emits a message. | ||||||||||||||||
1.2 The child maintains sustained attention when the adult proposes a task. | 0.96 ** | |||||||||||||||
1.3. The child maintains sustained attention when a task is performed by the adult. | 0.92 ** | 0.93 ** | ||||||||||||||
1.4 The child maintains sustained attention when spontaneously starting to perform a task. | 0.91 ** | 0.94 ** | 0.93 ** | |||||||||||||
1.5 The child maintains sustained attention when the adult proposes a task. | 0.88 ** | 0.88 ** | 0.88 ** | 0.90 ** | ||||||||||||
2.1. The child understands simple sentences of three elements. | 0.80 ** | 0.79 ** | 0.78 ** | 0.82 ** | 0.69 ** | |||||||||||
2.2 The child understands sentences of six elements and carries out the externally motivated actions. | 0.38 ** | 0.37 ** | 0.42 ** | 0.44 ** | 0.35 ** | 0.44 ** | ||||||||||
2.3 The child understands a discourse. | 0.64 ** | 0.61 ** | 0.66 ** | 0.57 ** | 0.58 ** | 0.59 ** | 0.20 | |||||||||
2.4 The child can issue short sentences but without discourse while performing externally motivated actions. | 0.44 ** | 0.43 ** | 0.46 ** | 0.48 ** | 0.53 ** | 0.37 ** | 0.65 ** | 0.22 | ||||||||
2.5 The child can emit elements of one phrase or short phrases, but without discourse. | 0.32 * | 0.28 * | 0.34 ** | 0.32 * | 0.33 * | 0.29 * | 0.37 ** | 0.52 ** | 0.06 | |||||||
2.6 The child can emit simple sentences with limitations in the topic of conversation. | 0.68 ** | 0.65 ** | 0.67 ** | 0.69 ** | 0.66 ** | 0.61 ** | 0.26 | 0.61 ** | 0.32 * | 0.44 ** | ||||||
3.1. The child employs expressive language consistent with the proposed task. | −0.003 | 0.08 | −0.02 | 0.09 | −0.09 | 0.19 | 0.30 * | −0.15 | 0.19 | −0.25 | −0.30 * | |||||
3.2. The adult’s language can direct short functional actions. | 0.71 ** | 0.71 ** | 0.69 ** | 0.74 ** | 0.63 ** | 0.80 ** | 0.41 ** | 0.63 ** | 0.50 ** | 0.15 | 0.65 ** | 0.15 | ||||
3.3 The child performs autonomous actions of lengthy process without prior planning. | 0.57 ** | 0.55 ** | 0.54 ** | 0.61 ** | 0.57 ** | 0.57 ** | 0.48 ** | 0.57 ** | 0.49 ** | 0.60 ** | 0.51 ** | −0.08 | 0.55 ** | |||
3.4. The child performs externally motivated actions without prior planning. | 0.54 ** | 0.50 ** | 0.53 ** | 0.55 ** | 0.59 ** | 0.50 ** | 0.28 * | 0.59 ** | 0.20 | 0.51 ** | 0.60 ** | −0.24 | 0.65 ** | 0.54 ** | ||
3.5. The child performs complex activities of lengthy process, with a perfect plan. | 0.37 ** | 0.33 * | 0.37 ** | 0.38 ** | 0.46 ** | 0.26 | −0.04 | 0.44 ** | −0.03 | 0.46 ** | 0.40 ** | −0.16 | 0.36 ** | 0.44 ** | 0.66 ** | |
Mean (M) | 2.46 | 2.45 | 2.32 | 2.34 | 2.27 | 2.45 | 2.05 | 2.09 | 1.98 | 1.82 | 2.05 | 2.16 | 2.23 | 1.96 | 1.84 | 1.77 |
Standard Desviation (SD) | 0.93 | 0.89 | 0.93 | 0.94 | 0.92 | 1.14 | 0.75 | 0.96 | 0.86 | 0.69 | 0.92 | 1.00 | 0.95 | 0.80 | 0.73 | 0.79 |
Items | F1 | F2 |
---|---|---|
1.5 The child maintains sustained attention when the adult proposes a task. | 0.82 | 0.07 |
2.2 The child understands sentences of six elements and carries out the externally motivated actions. | 0.43 | 0.64 |
2.3 The child understands a discourse. | 0.63 | 0.39 |
2.4 The child can issue short sentences but without discourse while performing externally motivated actions. | 0.46 | 0.70 |
2.5 The child can emit elements of one phrase or short phrases, but without discourse. | 0.40 | −0.15 |
2.6 The child can emit simple sentences with limitations in the topic of conversation. | 0.79 | −0.11 |
3.2. The adult’s language can direct short functional actions. | 0.79 | 0.19 |
3.3 The child performs autonomous actions of lengthy process without prior planning. | 0.80 | −0.22 |
3.4 The child performs externally motivated actions without prior planning. | 0.59 | −0.43 |
Items | Minimum | Maximum | Skewness | CI | Kurtosis | CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1.5 The child maintains sustained attention when the adult proposes a task. | 1 | 5 | 0.56 | 1.77 | 0.16 | 0.24 |
2.2 The child understands sentences of six elements and carries out the externally motivated actions | 1 | 4 | 0.44 | 1.34 | 0.08 | 0.12 |
2.3 The child understands a discourse. | 1 | 5 | 1.07 | 3.27 | 1.34 | 2.05 |
2.4 The child can issue short sentences but without discourse while performing externally motivated actions. | 1 | 4 | 0.72 | 2.20 | 0.02 | 0.04 |
2.6 The child can emit simple sentences with limitations in the topic of conversation. | 1 | 5 | 1.01 | 3.10 | 1.00 | 1.53 |
3.2 The adult’s language can direct short functional actions | 1 | 5 | 1.17 | 3.59 | 1.24 | 1.89 |
3.3 The child performs autonomous actions of lengthy process without prior planning. | 1 | 4 | 0.90 | 2.74 | 0.79 | 1.21 |
3.4 The child performs externally motivated actions without prior planning. | 1 | 4 | 1.09 | 3.34 | 1.96 | 3.00 |
Two-Factor Model (Pre-Determined Model) | One-Factor Model | Accepted Value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
df | 20 | 19 | ||
χ2/df | 28.59 | - | ||
Residual-based indices | CMIN/df | 1.50 | 7.18 | |
RMSEA | 0.06 | 0.21 | (0.05, 0.08) | |
RMSEA confidence interval | (0.00, 0.10) | (0.19, 0.24) | ||
SRMR | 0.06 | - | 0.05–0.08 | |
Comparative fit index | NFI | 0.90 | 0.00 | 0.90–0.95 |
Proportion of variance indices | CFI | 0.96 | 0.00 | 0.95–0.97 |
TLI | 0.91 | 0.00 | 0.85–0.90 | |
Indices of degree of parsimony | AIC | 78.59 | 274.56 | The lowest value |
ECVI | 0.59 | 2.06 | ||
ECVI interval (90%) | 0.52–0.73 | 1.70–2.47 |
Items | Group 1 GDD 55–70 | Group 2 GDD 70–80 | Group 3 CLD | Group 4 PD | Group 5 ELD | F(4, 82) | p | η2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n = 29 | n = 16 | n = 15 | n = 12 | n = 15 | ||||
M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | ||||
1.5. The child maintains sustained attention when the adult proposes a task. | 2.10 (0.77) | 2.75 (1.00) | 2.33 (0.97) | 2.81 (0.87) | 2.70 (1.54) | 2.50 | 0.05 | 0.11 |
2.2. The child understands sentences of six elements and carries out the externally motivated actions. | 2.06 (1.03) | 2.06 (0.68) | 1.73 (0.45) | 2.00 (0.77) | 2.64 (0.49) | 2.60 | 0.04 * | 0.11 |
2.3. The child understands a discourse. | 1.86 (0.78) | 2.37 (0.88) | 2.40 (1.35) | 2.81 (0.87) | 2.42 (1.08) | 2.30 | 0.07 | 0.10 |
2.4. The child can issue short sentences but without discourse while performing externally motivated actions. | 2.06 (1.03) | 2.18 (1.04) | 2.06 (0.80) | 1.45 (0.52) | 2.35 (0.92) | 1.60 | 1.83 | 0.07 |
2.6. The child can emit simple sentences with limitations in the topic of conversation. | 1.55 (0.46) | 2.56 (1.09) | 2.20 (0.94) | 2.00 (0.00) | 3.00 (1.10) | 5.70 | 0.00 ** | 0.22 |
3.2. The adult’s language can direct short functional actions. | 1.93 (0.59) | 1.75 (0.57) | 3.33 (0.57) | 2.20 (1.09) | 2.83 (1.16) | 4.99 | 0.01 ** | 0.20 |
3.3. The child performs autonomous actions of lengthy process without prior planning. | 2.00 (0.75) | 2.12 (0.81) | 1.93 (0.96) | 2.54 (0.93) | 2.57 (1.01) | 1.81 | 0.13 | 0.08 |
3.4. The child performs externally motivated actions without prior planning. | 1.68 (0.47) | 2.18 (0.91) | 1.80 (0.77) | 2.00 (0.00) | 2.07 (0.82) | 1.82 | 0.13 | 0.08 |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sáiz Manzanares, M.C.; Carbonero Martín, M.Á. Metacognitive Precursors: An Analysis in Children with Different Disabilities. Brain Sci. 2017, 7, 136. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7100136
Sáiz Manzanares MC, Carbonero Martín MÁ. Metacognitive Precursors: An Analysis in Children with Different Disabilities. Brain Sciences. 2017; 7(10):136. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7100136
Chicago/Turabian StyleSáiz Manzanares, María Consuelo, and Miguel Ángel Carbonero Martín. 2017. "Metacognitive Precursors: An Analysis in Children with Different Disabilities" Brain Sciences 7, no. 10: 136. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7100136
APA StyleSáiz Manzanares, M. C., & Carbonero Martín, M. Á. (2017). Metacognitive Precursors: An Analysis in Children with Different Disabilities. Brain Sciences, 7(10), 136. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7100136