Bilingual and Monolingual First Language Acquisition Experience Differentially Shapes Children’s Property Term Learning: Evidence from Behavioral and Neurophysiological Measures
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Whole Object Constraint, the Mutual Exclusivity Constraint, and Property Term Learning
1.2. Two Fundamentally Different Labeling Experiences: Bilingual and Monolingual First Language Acquisition
1.3. Learning New Nouns: Differences and Similarities in MEC Adherence in Bilinguals and Monolinguals
1.4. Learning New Property Terms: Differences and Similarities in MEC Adherence in Bilinguals and Monolinguals
1.5. Neurophysiological Methods to Better Understand MEC Adherence in Young Children: Deductive Reasoning
1.6. The Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Overview
2.2. Participants
2.3. Behavioral Assessment
2.4. Experimental Setting and Procedure
2.5. Stimulus Material
2.6. Analysis of Behavioral Data
2.7. Assessment of Cerebral Oxygenation Changes (fNIRS)
2.8. fNIRS Datasets
2.9. Stimulation Period Analyzed by fNIRS
2.10. Analysis of fNIRS-Data
3. Results
3.1. Behavioral Data
3.1.1. German Version: Effect of GROUP and AGE
3.1.2. Bilingual Children: Effect of LANGUAGE and AGE
3.2. fNIRS Data (Limited to Five-Year-Olds)
3.2.1. German Version: Effect of GROUP and ROI
3.2.2. Bilingual Children: Effect of LANGUAGE and ROI
3.3. Summary of Results
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Input situation on working days | G >> S 3 | G > S 46 | G = S 7 | G < S 4 | G << S 0 | missing 3 |
Input situation on weekends | G >> S 1 | G > S 20 | G = S 23 | G < S 13 | G << S 3 | missing 3 |
Children’s reported verbal skills | G >> S 19 | G > S 11 | G = S 20 | G < S 4 | G << S 5 | missing 4 |
Parental language use with children | one-G one-S 40 | one-G one-G & S 6 | one-S/G & S one-G & S 9 | both-S others-G 4 | one-S one-other 2 | missing 2 |
References
- Yow, W.Q.; Markman, E.M. Young bilingual children’s heightened sensitivity to referential cues. J. Cogn. Dev. 2011, 12, 12–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brojde, C.L.; Ahmed, S.; Colunga, E. Bilingual and monolingual children attend to different cues when learning new words. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Verhagen, J.; Grassmann, S.; Küntay, A.C. Monolingual and bilingual children’s resolution of referential conflicts: Effects of bilingualism and relative language proficiency. Cognitive. Dev. 2017, 41, 10–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yow, W.Q.; Li, X.; Lam, S.; Gliga, T.; Chong, Y.S.; Kwek, K.; Broekman, B.F.P. A bilingual advantage in 54-month-olds’ use of referential cues in fast mapping. Developmental. Sci 2017, 20, e12482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Groba, A.; De Houwer, A.; Mehnert, J.; Rossi, S.; Obrig, H. Bilingual and monolingual children process pragmatic cues differently when learning novel adjectives. Biling-Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 384–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davidson, D.; Raschke, V.R.; Pervez, J. Syntactic awareness in young monolingual and bilingual (Urdu–English) children. Cognitive Dev. 2010, 25, 166–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yow, W.Q.; Markman, E.M. A bilingual advantage in how children integrate multiple cues to understand a speaker’s referential intent. Biling-Lang. Cogn. 2015, 18, 391–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E.; Barac, R.; Blaye, A.; Poulin-Dubois, D. Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. J. Cogn. Dev. 2010, 11, 485–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crivello, C.; Kuzyk, O.; Rodrigues, M.; Friend, M.; Zesiger, P.; Poulin-Dubois, D. The effects of bilingual growth on toddlers’ executive function. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2016, 141, 121–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foy, J.G.; Mann, V.A. Bilingual children show advantages in nonverbal auditory executive function task. Int. J. Bilingual. 2014, 18, 717–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kovács, A.M.; Mehler, J. Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 6556–6560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brito, N.H.; Sebastián-Gallés, N.; Barr, R. Differences in language exposure and its effects on memory flexibility in monolingual, bilingual, and trilingual infants. Biling-Lang. Cogn. 2015, 18, 670–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hollich, G.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K. Young children associate novel words with complex objects rather than salient parts. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 1051–1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Markman, E.M. Constraints on word meaning in early language acquisition. Lingua 1994, 92, 199–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dixon, R.M. Where Have All the Adjectives Gone? De Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Hall, D.G.; Quantz, D.H.; Persoage, K.A. Preschoolers’ use of form class cues in word learning. Dev. Psychol. 2000, 36, 449–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, D.G.; Williams, S.G.; Bélanger, J. Learning count nouns and adjectives: Understanding the contributions of lexical form class and social-pragmatic cues. J. Cogn. Dev. 2010, 11, 86–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hiramatsu, K.; Rulf, K.E.; Epstein, S.D. When knowledge causes failure: Children’s extension of novel adjectives and the interpretation of one. Lingua 2010, 120, 1209–1218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mintz, T.H. Linguistic and conceptual influences on adjective acquisition in 24- and 36-month-olds. Dev. Psychol. 2005, 41, 17–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- O’Neill, D.K.; Topolovec, J.; Stern-Cavalcante, W. Feeling sponginess: The importance of descriptive gestures in 2- and 3-year-old children’s acquisition of adjectives. J. Cogn. Dev. 2002, 3, 243–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markman, E.M. Constraints children place on word meanings. In Language Acquisition: Core Readings; Bloom, P., Ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1993; pp. 154–174. [Google Scholar]
- Merriman, W.E.; Bowman, L.L.; MacWhinney, B. The mutual exclusivity bias in children’s word learning. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child. 1989, 54, 1–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bion, R.A.H.; Borovsky, A.; Fernald, A. Fast mapping, slow learning: disambiguation of novel word-object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30months. Cognition 2013, 126, 39–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Halberda, J. Is this a dax which I see before me? Use of the logical argument disjunctive syllogism supports word-learning in children and adults. Cognitive. Psychol. 2006, 53, 310–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kalashnikova, M.; Mattock, K.; Monaghan, P. Disambiguation of novel labels and referential facts: A developmental perspective. First Lang. 2014, 34, 125–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalashnikova, M.; Mattock, K.; Monaghan, P. Mutual exclusivity develops as a consequence of abstract rather than particular vocabulary knowledge. First Lang. 2016, 36, 451–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markman, E.M.; Wachtel, G.F. Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive. Psychol. 1988, 20, 121–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kandhadai, P.; Hall, D.G.; Werker, J.F. Second label learning in bilingual and monolingual infants. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20, e12429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yoshida, H.; Tran, D.N.; Benitez, V.; Kuwabara, M. Inhibition and adjective learning in bilingual and monolingual children. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Houwer, A. The Acquisition of Two Languages from Birth: A Case Study; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Meisel, J. Early differentiation of languages in bilingual children. In Bilingualism Across the Lifespan: Aspects of Acquisition, Maturity and Loss; Hyltenstam, K., Obler, L.K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1989; pp. 13–40. [Google Scholar]
- Pearson, B.Z.; Fernández, S.; Oller, D.K. Cross-language synonyms in the lexicons of bilingual infants: One language or two? J. Child. Lang. 1995, 22, 345–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quay, S. The bilingual lexicon: Implications for studies of language choice. J. Child. Lang. 1995, 22, 369–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Houwer, A.; Bornstein, M.H.; De Coster, S. Early understanding of two words for the same thing: A CDI study of lexical comprehension in infant bilinguals. Int. J. Bilingual. 2006, 10, 331–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henderson, A.M.E.; Scott, J.C. She called that thing a mido, but should you call it a mido too? Linguistic experience influences infants’ expectations of conventionality. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Byers-Heinlein, K.; Chen, K.H.; Xu, F. Surmounting the Tower of Babel: Monolingual and bilingual 2-year-olds’ understanding of the nature of foreign language words. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2014, 119, 87–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Byers-Heinlein, K.; Werker, J.F. Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: Evidence from bilingual infants. Cognition 2013, 128, 407–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kalashnikova, M.; Mattock, K.; Monaghan, P. The effects of linguistic experience on the flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning. Biling-Lang. Cogn. 2015, 18, 626–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Houwer, A. Bilingual First Language Acquisition; Multilingual Matters: Bristol, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Diesendruck, G. The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: An empirical examination. Dev. Psychol. 2005, 41, 451–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bhagwat, J.; Casasola, M. Infant sensitivity to speaker and language in learning a second label. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2014, 118, 41–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piccin, T.B.; Blewitt, P. Resource conservation as a basis for the mutual exclusivity effect in children’s word learning. First Lang. 2007, 27, 5–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Savage, S.L.; Au, T.K.-F. What word learners do when input contradicts the mutual exclusivity assumption. Child. Dev. 1996, 67, 3120–3134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Matthews, D.; Lieven, E.; Tomasello, M. What’s in a manner of speaking? Children’s sensitivity to partner-specific referential precedents. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 749–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Byers-Heinlein, K. Bilingualism affects 9-month-old infants’ expectations about how words refer to kinds. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20, e12486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Byers-Heinlein, K.; Werker, J.F. Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: Infants’ language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 815–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Houston-Price, C.; Caloghiris, Z.; Raviglione, E. Language experience shapes the development of the mutual exclusivity bias. Infancy 2010, 15, 125–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalashnikova, M.; Escudero, P.; Kidd, E. The development of fast-mapping and novel word retention strategies in monolingual and bilingual infants. Dev. Sci. 2018, e12674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rowe, L.; Jacobson, R.; Saylor, M.M. Differences in how monolingual and bilingual children learn second labels for familiar objects. J. Child. Lang. 2015, 42, 1219–1236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aravind, A.; de Villiers, J.; Pace, A.; Valentine, H.; Golinkoff, R.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Iglesias, A.; Sweig Wilson, M. Fast mapping word meanings across trials: Young children forget all but their first guess. Cognition 2018, 177, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Davidson, D.; Jergovic, D.; Imami, Z.; Theodos, V. Monolingual and bilingual children’s use of the mutual exclusivity constraint. J. Child. Lang. 1997, 24, 3–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Davidson, D.; Tell, D. Monolingual and bilingual children’s use of mutual exclusivity in the naming of whole objects. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2005, 92, 25–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hung, W.-Y.; Patrycia, F.; Yow, W.Q. Bilingual children weigh speaker’s referential cues and word-learning heuristics differently in different language contexts when interpreting a speaker’s intent. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Houwer, A. The speech of fluent child bilinguals. In Multilingual Aspects of Fluency Disorders; Howell, P., van Borsel, J., Eds.; Multilingual Matters: Bristol, UK, 2011; pp. 3–23. [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer, A. The role of language input environments for language outcomes and language acquisition in young bilingual children. In Bilingual Cognition and Language: The State of the Science Across Its Subfields; Miller, D., Bayram, F., Rothman, J., Serratrice, L., Eds.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2018; pp. 127–153. [Google Scholar]
- De Neys, W. Dual processing in reasoning: two systems but one reasoner. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 428–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grassmann, S.; Schulze, C.; Tomasello, M. Children’s level of word knowledge predicts their exclusion of familiar objects as referents of novel words. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goel, V. Anatomy of deductive reasoning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 435–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prado, J.; Chadha, A.; Booth, J.R. The brain network for deductive reasoning: A quantitative meta-analysis of 28 neuroimaging studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 3483–3497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reverberi, C.; Cherubini, P.; Rapisarda, A.; Rigamonti, E.; Caltagirone, C.; Frackowiak, R.S.J.; Macaluso, E.; Paulesu, E. Neural basis of generation of conclusions in elementary deduction. Neuroimage 2007, 38, 752–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rodriguez-Moreno, D.; Hirsch, J. The dynamics of deductive reasoning: An fMRI investigation. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 949–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Monti, M.M.; Osherson, D.N. Logic, language and the brain. Brain Res. 2012, 1428, 33–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Overwalle, F. A dissociation between social mentalizing and general reasoning. Neuroimage 2011, 54, 1589–1599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tsujii, T.; Watanabe, S. Neural correlates of belief-bias reasoning under time pressure: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuroimage 2010, 50, 1320–1326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tsujii, T.; Watanabe, S. Neural correlates of dual-task effect on belief-bias syllogistic reasoning: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain Res. 2009, 1287, 118–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poirel, N.; Borst, G.; Simon, G.; Rossi, S.; Cassotti, M.; Pineau, A.; Houdé, O. Number conservation is related to children’s prefrontal inhibitory control: An fMRI study of a piagetian task. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e40802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsujii, T.; Masuda, S.; Akiyama, T.; Watanabe, S. The role of inferior frontal cortex in belief-bias reasoning: An rTMS study. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 2005–2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsujii, T.; Sakatani, K.; Masuda, S.; Akiyama, T.; Watanabe, S. Evaluating the roles of the inferior frontal gyrus and superior parietal lobule in deductive reasoning: An rTMS study. Neuroimage 2011, 58, 640–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aron, A.R.; Robbins, T.W.; Poldrack, R.A. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Houdé, O.; Borst, G. Evidence for an inhibitory-control theory of the reasoning brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shallice, T.; Cipolotti, L. The prefrontal cortex and neurological impairments of active thought. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2018, 69, 157–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Banich, M.T.; Depue, B.E. Recent advances in understanding neural systems that support inhibitory control. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2015, 1, 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Janssen, T.W.P.; Heslenfeld, D.J.; van Mourik, R.; Logan, G.D.; Oosterlaan, J. Neural correlates of response inhibition in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A controlled version of the stop-signal task. Psychiatry Res. 2015, 233, 278–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mehnert, J.; Akhrif, A.; Telkemeyer, S.; Rossi, S.; Schmitz, C.H.; Steinbrink, J.; Wartenburger, I.; Obrig, H.; Neufang, S. Developmental changes in brain activation and functional connectivity during response inhibition in the early childhood brain. Brain Dev. 2013, 35, 894–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Obrig, H.; Villringer, A. Beyond the visible—Imaging the human brain with light. J. Cereb. Blood F. Met. 2003, 23, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lloyd-Fox, S.; Blasi, A.; Elwell, C.E. Illuminating the developing brain: The past, present and future of functional near infrared spectroscopy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 34, 269–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, D.G. Preschoolers’ default assumptions about word meaning: Proper names designate unique individuals. Dev. Psychol. 1996, 32, 177–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Imai, M.; Haryu, E. Learning proper nouns and common nouns without clues from syntax. Child. Dev. 2001, 72, 787–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hall, D.G. Semantic constraints on word learning: Proper names and adjectives. Child. Dev. 1994, 65, 1299–1317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Álvarez, B.; Cuetos, F. Objective age of acquisition norms for a set of 328 words in Spanish. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 377–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schröder, A.; Kauschke, C.; De Bleser, R. Messungen des Erwerbsalters für konkrete Nomina. Neurolinguistik 2003, 17, 83–114. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, L.B.; Jones, S.S.; Landau, B. Count nouns, adjectives, and perceptual properties in children’s novel word interpretations. Dev. Psychol. 1992, 28, 273–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sandhofer, C.M.; Smith, L.B. Perceptual complexity and form class cues in novel word extension tasks: How 4-year-old children interpret adjectives and count nouns. Dev. Sci. 2004, 7, 378–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pomper, R.; Saffran, J.R. Familiar object salience affects novel word learning. Child. Dev. 2018, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cope, M.; Delpy, D.T. System for long-term measurement of cerebral blood and tissue oxygenation on newborn infants by near infra-red transillumination. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 1988, 26, 289–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fox, P.T.; Raichle, M.E. Focal physiological uncoupling of cerebral blood flow and oxidative metabolism during somatosensory stimulation in human subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1986, 83, 1140–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kleinschmidt, A.; Obrig, H.; Requardt, M.; Merboldt, K.D.; Dirnagl, U.; Villringer, A.; Frahm, J. Simultaneous recording of cerebral blood oxygenation changes during human brain activation by magnetic resonance imaging and near-infrared spectroscopy. J. Cereb. Blood. Flow Met. 1996, 16, 817–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Obrig, H.; Neufang, M.; Wenzel, R.; Kohl, M.; Steinbrink, J.; Einhäupl, K.; Villringer, A. Spontaneous low frequency oscillations of cerebral hemodynamics and metabolism in human adults. Neuroimage 2000, 12, 623–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gervain, J.; Mehler, J.; Werker, J.F.; Nelson, C.A.; Csibra, G.; Lloyd-Fox, S.; Shukla, M.; Aslin, R.N. Near-infrared spectroscopy: A report from the McDonnell infant methodology consortium. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 1, 22–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Steinbrink, J.; Villringer, A.; Kempf, F.; Haux, D.; Boden, S.; Obrig, H. Illuminating the BOLD signal: Combined fMRI-fNIRS studies. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2006, 24, 495–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Telkemeyer, S.; Rossi, S.; Koch, S.P.; Nierhaus, T.; Steinbrink, J.; Poeppel, D.; Obrig, H.; Wartenburger, I. Sensitivity of newborn auditory cortex to the temporal structure of sounds. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 14726–14733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rossi, S.; Jürgenson, I.B.; Hanulíková, A.; Telkemeyer, S.; Wartenburger, I.; Obrig, H. Implicit processing of phonotactic cues: Evidence from electrophysiological and vascular responses. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 1752–1764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Obrig, H.; Mock, J.; Stephan, F.; Richter, M.; Vignotto, M.; Rossi, S. Impact of associative word learning on phonotactic processing in 6-month-old infants: A combined EEG and fNIRS study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 25, 185–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boynton, G.M.; Engel, S.A.; Heeger, D.J. Linear systems analysis of the fMRI signal. Neuroimage 2012, 62, 975–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eriksson, M.; Marschik, P.; Tulviste, T.; Almgren, M.; Pérez Pereira, M.; Wehberg, S.; Marjanovič-Umek, L.; Gayraud, F.; Kovacevic, M.; Gallego, C. Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 30, 326–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rojo, D.P.; Echols, C.H. Non-native language exposure promotes children’s willingness to accept labels in two languages. J. Cogn. Dev. 2018, 19, 107–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Menjivar, J.; Akhtar, N. Language experience and preschoolers’ foreign word learning. Biling-Lang. Cogn. 2017, 20, 642–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halberda, J. The development of a word-learning strategy. Cognition 2003, 87, B23–B34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Song, H.-J.; Baillargeon, R.; Fisher, C. The development of infants’ use of novel verbal information when reasoning about others’ actions. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e92387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bulgarelli, F.; Lebkuecher, A.L.; Weiss, D.J. Bilingualism and statistical learning. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 740–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gathercole, V.C.M.; Min, H. Word meaning biases or language-specific effects?: Evidence from English, Spanish and Korean. First Lang. 1997, 17, 31–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colunga, E.; Smith, L.B.; Gasser, M. Correlation versus prediction in children’s word learning: Cross-linguistic evidence and simulations. Lang. Cogn. 2009, 1, 197–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hahn, E.R.; Cantrell, L. The shape-bias in Spanish-speaking children and its relationship to vocabulary. J. Child Lang. 2012, 39, 443–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Subrahmanyam, K.; Chen, H.-H.N. A crosslinguistic study of children’s noun learning: The case of object and substance words. First Lang. 2006, 26, 141–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haryu, E. Effects of knowledge about cross-language equivalents on children’s use of mutual exclusivity in interpreting novel labels. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 1998, 40, 82–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merriman, W.E.; Kutlesic, V. Bilingual and monolingual children’s use of two lexical acquisition heuristics. Appl. Psycholinguist. 1993, 14, 229–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landau, B.; Smith, L.B.; Jones, S. Syntactic context and the shape bias in children’s and adults’ lexical learning. J. Mem. Lang. 1992, 31, 807–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayas Tanaka, M.E. Knowledge of adjective reference by monolingual Spanish- and English-speaking children. Entrehojas: Revista de Estudios Hispánicos 2014, 4, 1–24. [Google Scholar]
- Song, L.; Nazzi, T.; Moukawane, S.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Stahl, A.; Ma, W.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Connell, M. Sleepy vs. Sleeping: Preschoolers’ sensitivity to morphological cues for adjectives and verbs in English and French. In BUCLD 34: Proceedings of the 34th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development; Franich, K., Iserman, K.M., Keil, L.L., Eds.; Cascadilla: Somerville, MA, USA, 2010; pp. 409–420. [Google Scholar]
- Hall, D.G.; Moore, C.E. Red bluebirds and black greenflies: Preschoolers’ understanding of the semantics of adjectives and count nouns. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 1997, 67, 236–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Group & Language | 3.5-Year-Olds | Five-Year-Olds | All |
---|---|---|---|
bilingual, Spanish | 29 | 30 | 59 |
bilingual, German | 28 | 30 | 58 |
monolingual, German | 29 | 28 | 57 |
All | 86 | 88 | 174 |
Block | Trials |
---|---|
Block 1 | First 2 focus trials 1 property focus trial |
1 category focus trial (order balanced across trials) | |
Followed by 4 test trials (→ end of short version) | |
Block 2 | First 2 focus trials 1 property focus trial |
1 category focus trial (order balanced across trials) | |
Followed by 3 test trials (→ end of long version) |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Groba, A.; De Houwer, A.; Obrig, H.; Rossi, S. Bilingual and Monolingual First Language Acquisition Experience Differentially Shapes Children’s Property Term Learning: Evidence from Behavioral and Neurophysiological Measures. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 40. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9020040
Groba A, De Houwer A, Obrig H, Rossi S. Bilingual and Monolingual First Language Acquisition Experience Differentially Shapes Children’s Property Term Learning: Evidence from Behavioral and Neurophysiological Measures. Brain Sciences. 2019; 9(2):40. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9020040
Chicago/Turabian StyleGroba, Agnes, Annick De Houwer, Hellmuth Obrig, and Sonja Rossi. 2019. "Bilingual and Monolingual First Language Acquisition Experience Differentially Shapes Children’s Property Term Learning: Evidence from Behavioral and Neurophysiological Measures" Brain Sciences 9, no. 2: 40. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9020040