Executive Dysfunctions in Schizophrenia: A Critical Review of Traditional, Ecological, and Virtual Reality Assessments
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Nature of Executive Functions
3. Brain Correlates of Executive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia
4. Old and New Paradigms for Assessing Executive Functions in Schizophrenia
5. Assessment with Traditional Tools
6. Assessment with Ecological Tools
7. Assessment with Virtual Reality
8. Relationship of Executive Dysfunction with Psychopathological Symptoms
9. Challenges and Future Directions
10. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Dickinson, D.; Ramsey, M.E.; Gold, J.M. Overlooking the obvious: A meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2007, 64, 532–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fioravanti, M.; Bianchi, V.; Cinti, M.E. Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: An updated metanalysis of the scientific evidence. BMC Psychiatry 2012, 12, 64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Freedman, D.; Brown, A.S. The developmental course of executive functioning in schizophrenia. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2011, 29, 237–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Orellana, G.; Slachevsky, A. Executive functioning in schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 2013, 4, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Green, M.F.; Harvey, P.D. Cognition in schizophrenia: Past, present, and future. Schizophr. Res. Cogn. 2014, 1, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association Publishing: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013; ISBN 9780890425596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fett, A.K.; Viechtbauer, W.; Penn, D.L.; van Os, J.; Krabbendam, L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 35, 573–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van der Werf, M.; Hanssen, M.; Köhler, S.; Verkaaik, M.; Verhey, F.R.; van Winkel, R.; van Os, J.; Allardyce, J. Systematic review and collaborative recalculation of 133 693 incident cases of schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 2014, 44, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hay, S.I.; Abajobir, A.A.; Abate, K.H.; Abbafati, C.; Abbas, K.M.; Abd-Allah, F.; Abdulkader, R.S.; Abdulle, A.M.; Abebo, T.A.; Abera, S.F.; et al. Global, regional, and national disability-adjusted life-years (DALYs) for 333 diseases and injuries and healthy life expectancy (HALE) for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2016: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet 2017, 390, 1260–1344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bora, E.; Akdede, B.B.; Alptekin, K. Neurocognitive impairment in deficit and non-deficit schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 2017, 47, 2401–2413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirkpatrick, B.; Galderisi, S. Deficit schizophrenia: An update. World Psychiatry 2008, 7, 143–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Dollfus, S.; Lyne, J. Negative symptoms: History of the concept and their position in diagnosis of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2017, 186, 3–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carpenter, W.T.; Heinrichs, D.W.; Wagman, A.M. Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: The concept. Am. J. Psychiatry 1988, 145, 578–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strauss, G.P.; Harrow, M.; Grossman, L.S.; Rosen, C. Periods of recovery in deficit syndrome schizophrenia: A 20-year multi–follow-up longitudinal study. Schizophr. Bull. 2010, 36, 788–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pełka-Wysiecka, J.; Wroński, M.; Jasiewicz, A.; Grzywacz, A.; Tybura, P.; Kucharska-Mazur, J.; Bieńkowski, P.; Samochowiec, J. BDNF rs 6265 polymorphism and COMT rs 4680 polymorphism in deficit schizophrenia in Polish sample. Pharmacol. Rep. 2013, 65, 1185–1193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galderisi, S.; Quarantelli, M.; Volpe, U.; Mucci, A.; Cassano, G.B.; Invernizzi, G.; Rossi, A.; Vita, A.; Pini, S.; Cassano, P.; et al. Patterns of structural MRI abnormalities in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2008, 34, 393–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, A.S.; Saperstein, A.M.; Gold, J.M.; Kirkpatrick, B.; Carpenter, W.T., Jr.; Buchanan, R.W. Neuropsychology of the deficit syndrome: New data and meta-analysis of findings to date. Schizophr. Bull. 2007, 33, 1201–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yu, M.; Tang, X.; Wang, X.; Zhang, X.; Zhang, X.; Sha, W.; Yao, S.; Shu, N.; Zhang, X.; Zhang, Z. Neurocognitive impairments in deficit and non-deficit schizophrenia and their relationships with symptom dimensions and other clinical variables. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0138357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Polgár, P.; Réthelyi, J.M.; Bálint, S.; Komlósi, S.; Czobor, P.; Bitter, I. Executive function in deficit schizophrenia: What do the dimensions of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test tell us? Schizophr. Res. 2010, 122, 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Réthelyi, J.M.; Czobor, P.; Polgár, P.; Mersich, B.; Bálint, S.; Jekkel, É.; Magyar, K.; Mészáros, A.; Fábián, A.; Bitter, I. General and domain-specific neurocognitive impairments in deficit and non-deficit schizophrenia. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2012, 262, 107–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tyburski, E.; Pełka-Wysiecka, J.; Mak, M.; Samochowiec, A.; Bieńkowski, P.; Samochowiec, J. Neuropsychological profile of specific executive dysfunctions in patients with deficit and non-deficit schizophrenia. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Vakil, E. Neuropsychological assessment: Principles, rationale, and challenges. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2012, 34, 135–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruff, R.M. A friendly critique of neuropsychology: Facing the challenges of our future. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2003, 18, 847–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parsons, T.D.; Carlew, A.R.; Magtoto, J.; Stonecipher, K. The potential of function-led virtual environments for ecologically valid measures of executive function in experimental and clinical neuropsychology. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2017, 27, 777–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Freeman, D.; Reeve, S.; Robinson, A.; Ehlers, A.; Clark, D.; Spanlang, B.; Slater, M. Virtual reality in the assessment, understanding, and treatment of mental health disorders. Psychol. Med. 2017, 47, 2393–2400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neguț, A.; Matu, S.A.; Sava, F.A.; David, D. Virtual reality measures in neuropsychological assessment: A meta-analytic review. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2016, 30, 165–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Macedo, M.; Marques, A.; Queirós, C. Virtual reality in assessment and treatment of schizophrenia: A systematic review. Braz. J. Psychiatry 2015, 64, 70–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rus-Calafell, M.; Garety, P.; Sason, E.; Craig, T.J.; Valmaggia, L.R. Virtual reality in the assessment and treatment of psychosis: A systematic review of its utility, acceptability and effectiveness. Psychol. Med. 2018, 48, 362–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kruk, D.; Mętel, D.; Gawęda, Ł.; Cechnicki, A. Implementation of virtual reality (VR) in diagnostics and therapy of nonaffective psychoses. Psychiatr. Pol. 2020, 54, 951–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dibben, C.R.; Rice, C.; Laws, K.R.; McKenna, P.J. Is executive impairment associated with schizophrenic syndromes? A meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 2009, 39, 381–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Harvey, P.D.; Koren, D.; Reichenberg, A.; Bowie, C.R. Negative symptoms and cognitive deficits: What is the nature of their relationship? Schizophr. Bull. 2005, 32, 250–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Galderisi, S.; DeLisi, L.E.; Borgwardt, S. Neuroimaging of Schizophrenia and Other Primary Psychotic Disorders; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Chan, R.C.; Shum, D.; Toulopoulou, T.; Chen, E.Y. Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2008, 23, 201–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jurado, M.B.; Rosselli, M. The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2007, 17, 213–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luria, A.R. Higher Cortical Functions in Man; Consultants Bureau: New York, NY, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Luria, A.R. The Working Brain; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Tupper, D.E. Introduction: Neuropsychological assessment apres Luria. Neuropsychol. Rev. 1999, 9, 57–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lezak, M.D.; Howieson, D.B.; Loring, D.W. Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. Working memory. Science 1992, 255, 556–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Anderson, V.; Jacobs, R.; Anderson, P.J. Executive Functions and the Frontal Lobes: A Lifespan Perspective; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Gioia, G.A.; Isquith, P.K.; Guy, S.C.; Kenworthy, L. Test review behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000, 6, 235–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barkley, R.A. ADHD and the Nature of Self-Control; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Nigg, J.T. On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol. Bull. 2000, 126, 220–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Testa, R.; Bennett, P.; Ponsford, J. Factor analysis of nineteen executive function tests in a healthy adult population. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2012, 27, 213–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Millan, M.J.; Agid, Y.; Brüne, M.; Bullmore, E.T.; Carter, C.S.; Clayton, N.S.; Connor, R.; Davis, S.; Deakin, B.; DeRubeis, R.J.; et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: Characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 2012, 11, 141–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eisenberg, D.P.; Berman, K.F. Executive function, neural circuitry, and genetic mechanisms in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010, 35, 258–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bressler, S.L.; Menon, V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: Emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010, 14, 277–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Uddin, L.Q.; Yeo, B.T.; Spreng, R.N. Towards a universal taxonomy of macro-scale functional human brain networks. Brain Topogr. 2019, 32, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bressler, S.L. Understanding cognition through large-scale cortical networks. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 11, 58–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Uddin, L.Q.; Menon, V. The anterior insula in autism: Under-connected and under-examined. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2009, 33, 1198–1203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jilka, S.R.; Scott, G.; Ham, T.; Pickering, A.; Bonnelle, V.; Braga, R.M.; Leech, R.; Sharp, D.J. Damage to the salience network and interactions with the default mode network. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 10798–10807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- O’Neill, A.; Mechelli, A.; Bhattacharyya, S. Dysconnectivity of large-scale functional networks in early psychosis: A meta-analysis. Schizophr. Bull. 2019, 45, 579–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhou, Y.; Fan, L.; Qiu, C.; Jiang, T. Prefrontal cortex and the dysconnectivity hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neurosci. Bull. 2015, 31, 207–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Menon, V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 483–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rapoport, J.L.; Giedd, J.N.; Gogtay, N. Neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: Update 2012. Mol. Psychiatry 2012, 17, 1228–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miyata, J.; Sasamoto, A.; Mori, Y.; Isobe, M.; Sakai, Y.; Kochiyama, T.; Urayama, S.; Aso, T.; Murai, T.; Takahashi, H. 144. Structural and Functional Connectivity and Delusional Cognitive Bias: A Multimodal Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study on Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2017, 43, S76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Giraldo-Chica, M.; Rogers, B.P.; Damon, S.M.; Landman, B.A.; Woodward, N.D. Prefrontal-thalamic anatomical connectivity and executive cognitive function in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 2018, 83, 509–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Minzenberg, M.J.; Laird, A.R.; Thelen, S.; Carter, C.S.; Glahn, D.C. Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2009, 66, 811–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Palaniyappan, L.; Mallikarjun, P.; Joseph, V.; White, T.P.; Liddle, P.F. Reality distortion is related to the structure of the salience network in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 2011, 41, 1701–1708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wagner, G.; De la Cruz, F.; Schachtzabel, C.; Güllmar, D.; Schultz, C.C.; Schlösser, R.G.; Bär, K.J.; Koch, K. Structural and functional dysconnectivity of the fronto-thalamic system in schizophrenia: A DCM-DTI study. Cortex 2015, 66, 35–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salgado-Pineda, P.; Fakra, E.; Delaveau, P.; McKenna, P.J.; Pomarol-Clotet, E.; Blin, O. Correlated structural and functional brain abnormalities in the default mode network in schizophrenia patients. Schizophr. Res. 2011, 125, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drakesmith, M.; Caeyenberghs, K.; Dutt, A.; Zammit, S.; Evans, C.J.; Reichenberg, A.; Lewis, G.; David, A.S.; Jones, D.K. Schizophrenia-like topological changes in the structural connectome of individuals with subclinical psychotic experiences. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015, 36, 2629–2643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pu, W.; Li, L.; Zhang, H.; Ouyang, X.; Liu, H.; Zhao, J.; Li, L.; Xue, Z.; Xu, K.; Tang, H.; et al. Morphological and functional abnormalities of salience network in the early-stage of paranoid schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2012, 141, 15–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bracht, T.; Horn, H.; Strik, W.; Federspiel, A.; Razavi, N.; Stegmayer, K.; Wiest, R.; Dierks, T.; Müller, T.J.; Walther, S. White matter pathway organization of the reward system is related to positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2014, 153, 136–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rüsch, N.; Spoletini, I.; Wilke, M.; Bria, P.; Di Paola, M.; Di Iulio, F.; Martinotti, G.; Caltagirone, C.; Spalletta, G. Prefrontal–thalamic–cerebellar gray matter networks and executive functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2007, 93, 79–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nygård, M.; Løberg, E.M.; Craven, A.R.; Ersland, L.; Berle, J.Ø.; Kroken, R.A.; Johnsen, E.; Hugdahl, K. Dichotic listening, executive functions and grey matter cortical volume in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Scand. J. Psychol. 2013, 54, 443–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hatton, S.N.; Lagopoulos, J.; Hermens, D.F.; Hickie, I.B.; Scott, E.; Bennett, M.R. White matter tractography in early psychosis: Clinical and neurocognitive associations. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2014, 39, 417–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Molina, V.; Lubeiro, A.; Soto, O.; Rodriguez, M.; Álvarez, A.; Hernández, R.; de Luis-García, R. Alterations in prefrontal connectivity in schizophrenia assessed using diffusion magnetic resonance imaging. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2017, 76, 107–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tyburski, E.; Mak, M.; Samochowiec, A.; Plichta, P.; Bielecki, M.; Rek-Owodzin, K.; Podwalski, P.; Rudkowski, K.; Pełka-Wysiecka, J.; Kucharska-Mazur, J.; et al. The relationship between cingulum bundle integrity and different aspects of executive functions in chronic schizophrenia. Prog. NeuroPsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2020, 102, e109955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Takei, K.; Yamasue, H.; Abe, O.; Yamada, H.; Inoue, H.; Suga, M.; Muroi, M.; Sasaki, H.; Aoki, S.; Kasai, K. Structural disruption of the dorsal cingulum bundle is associated with impaired Stroop performance in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2009, 114, 119–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levitt, J.J.; Kubicki, M.; Nestor, P.G.; Ersner-Hershfield, H.; Westin, C.F.; Alvarado, J.L.; Kikinis, R.; Jolesz, F.A.; McCarley, R.W.; Shenton, M.E. A diffusion tensor imaging study of the anterior limb of the internal capsule in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2010, 184, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Knöchel, C.; Schmied, C.; Linden, D.E.; Stäblein, M.; Prvulovic, D.; de Carvalho, L.D.; Harrison, O.; Junior, P.O.; Carvalho, A.F.; Reif, A.; et al. White matter abnormalities in the fornix are linked to cognitive performance in SZ but not in BD disorder: An exploratory analysis with DTI deterministic tractography. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 201, 64–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prasad, K.M.; Upton, C.H.; Schirda, C.S.; Nimgaonkar, V.L.; Keshavan, M.S. White matter diffusivity and microarchitecture among schizophrenia subjects and first-degree relatives. Schizophr. Res. 2015, 161, 70–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Zhou, Y.; Liu, J.; Driesen, N.; Womer, F.; Chen, K.; Wang, Y.; Jiang, X.; Zhou, Q.; Bai, C.; Wang, D.; et al. White matter integrity in genetic high-risk individuals and first-episode schizophrenia patients: Similarities and disassociations. BioMed Res. Int. 2017, 2017, 3107845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burgess, P.W.; Alderman, N.; Forbes, C.; Costello, A.; Coates, L.A.; Dawson, D.R.; Anderson, N.D.; Gilbert, S.J.; Dumontheil, I.; Channon, S. The case for the development and use of “ecologically valid” measures of executive function in experimental and clinical neuropsychology. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2006, 12, 194–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Armstrong, C.M.; Reger, G.M.; Edwards, J.; Rizzo, A.A.; Courtney, C.G.; Parsons, T.D. Validity of the Virtual Reality Stroop Task (VRST) in active duty military. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2013, 35, 113–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parsey, C.M.; Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. Applications of technology in neuropsychological assessment. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2013, 27, 1328–1361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chaytor, N.; Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. The ecological validity of neuropsychological tests: A review of the literature on everyday cognitive skills. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2003, 13, 181–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heinrichs, R.W. Current and emergent applications of neuropsychological assessment: Problems of validity and utility. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 1990, 21, 171–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCaffrey, R.J.; Williams, A.D.; Fisher, J.M.; Laing, L.C. The Practice of Forensic Neuropsychology: Meeting Challenges in the Courtroom; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Kvavilashvili, L.; Ellis, J. Ecological validity and twenty years of real-life/laboratory controversy in memory research: A critical (and historical) review. Hist. Philos. Psychol. 2004, 6, 59–80. [Google Scholar]
- Franzen, M.D.; Wilhelm, K.L. Conceptual foundations of ecological validity in neuropsychology. In Ecological Validity of Neuropsychological Testing; Sbordone, R.J., Long, C.J., Eds.; GR Press/St. Lucie Press: Delray Beach, FL, USA, 1996; pp. 91–112. [Google Scholar]
- Strauss, E.; Sherman, E.; Spreen, O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fioravanti, M.; Carlone, O.; Vitale, B.; Cinti, M.E.; Clare, L. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2005, 15, 73–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knowles, E.E.; David, A.S.; Reichenberg, A. Processing speed deficits in schizophrenia: Reexamining the evidence. Am. J. Psychiatry 2010, 167, 828–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Green, M.F. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am. J. Psychiatry 1996, 153, 321–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Green, M.F.; Kern, R.S.; Braff, D.L.; Mintz, J. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: Are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophr. Bull. 2000, 26, 119–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Green, M.F.; Kern, R.S.; Heaton, R.K. Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: Implications for MATRICS. Schizophr. Res. 2004, 72, 41–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shallice, T. Specific impairments of planning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 1982, 298, 199–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knapp, F.; Viechtbauer, W.; Leonhart, R.; Nitschke, K.; Kaller, C.P. Planning performance in schizophrenia patients. Psychol. Med. 2017, 47, 2002–2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rajji, T.K.; Ismail, Z.; Mulsant, B.H. Age at onset and cognition in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. Br. J. Psychiatry 2009, 195, 286–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sullivan, J.R.; Riccio, C.A.; Castillo, C.L. Concurrent validity of the tower tasks as measures of executive function in adults: A meta-analysis. Appl. Neuropsychol. 2009, 16, 62–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heaton, R.K.; Chelune, G.J.; Talley, J.L.; Kay, G.G.; Curtiss, G. Wisconsin Card Sort Test Manual: Revised and Expanded; Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, TX, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Tyburski, E.; Sokołowski, A.; Chęć, M.; Pełka-Wysiecka, J.; Samochowiec, A. Neuropsychological characteristics of verbal and non-verbal fluency in schizophrenia patients. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 2015, 29, 33–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale; The Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1939. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, J.; Park, S. Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2005, 114, 599–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Forbes, N.F.; Carrick, L.A.; McIntosh, A.M.; Lawrie, S.M. Working memory in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 2009, 39, 889–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Keefe, R.S.; Goldberg, T.E.; Harvey, P.D.; Gold, J.M.; Poe, M.P.; Coughenour, L. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: Reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophr. Res. 2004, 68, 283–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nuechterlein, K.H.; Green, M.F.; Kern, R.S.; Baade, L.E.; Barch, D.M.; Cohen, J.D.; Essock, S.; Fenton, W.S.; Frese, F.J., 3rd; Gold, J.M.; et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: Test selection, reliability, and validity. Am. J. Psychiatry 2008, 165, 203–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised; The Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Katz, N.; Tadmor, I.; Felzen, B.; Hartman-Maeir, A. Validity of the Executive Function Performance Test in individuals with schizophrenia. OTJR 2007, 27, 44–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baum, C.M.; Morrison, T.; Hahn, M.; Edwards, D.F. Test Anual: Executive Function Performance Test; Washington University: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Bulzacka, E.; Delourme, G.; Hutin, V.; Burban, N.; Méary, A.; Lajnef, M.; Leboyer, M.; Schürhoff, F. Clinical utility of the Multiple Errands Test in schizophrenia: A preliminary assessment. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 240, 390–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shallice, T.I.; Burgess, P.W. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain 1991, 114, 727–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caletti, E.; Paoli, R.A.; Fiorentini, A.; Cigliobianco, M.; Zugno, E.; Serati, M.; Orsenigo, G.; Grillo, P.; Zago, S.; Caldiroli, A.; et al. Neuropsychology, social cognition and global functioning among bipolar, schizophrenic patients and healthy controls: Preliminary data. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Burgess, P.W.; Alderman, N.; Evans, J.J.; Wilson, B.A.; Emslie, H.; Shallice, T. Modified Six Element Test. In Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome; Wilson, B.A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P.W., Emslie, H., Evans, J.J., Eds.; Thames Valley Test Company: Bury St Edmunds, UK, 1996; pp. 18–19. [Google Scholar]
- Thai, M.L.; Andreassen, A.K.; Bliksted, V. A meta-analysis of executive dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia: Different degree of impairment in the ecological subdomains of the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. Psychiatry Res. 2019, 272, 230–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vargas, M.L.; Sanz, J.C.; Marín, J.J. Behavioral assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome battery (BADS) in schizophrenia: A pilot study in the Spanish population. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2009, 22, 95–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Evans, J.J.; Chua, S.E.; McKenna, P.J.; Wilson, B.A. Assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 1997, 27, 635–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wilson, B.A.; Evans, J.J.; Emslie, H.; Alderman, N.; Burgess, P. The development of an ecologically valid test for assessing patients with a dysexecutive syndrome. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 1998, 8, 213–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, B.A.; Alderman, N.; Burgess, P.W.; Emslie, H.; Evans, J.J. Zoo map. In Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome; Thames Valley Test Company: Bury St Edmunds, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Klinger, E.; Chemin, I.; Lebreton, S.; Marié, R.M. A virtual supermarket to assess cognitive planning. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 2004, 7, 292–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Josman, N.; Schenirderman, A.E.; Klinger, E.; Shevil, E. Using virtual reality to evaluate executive functioning among persons with schizophrenia: A validity study. Schizophr. Res. 2009, 115, 270–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aubin, G.; Béliveau, M.F.; Klinger, E. An exploration of the ecological validity of the Virtual Action Planning–Supermarket (VAP-S) with people with schizophrenia. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2018, 28, 689–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Han, K.; Kim, I.Y.; Kim, J.J. Assessment of cognitive flexibility in real life using virtual reality: A comparison of healthy individuals and schizophrenia patients. Comput. Biol. Med. 2012, 42, 841–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laloyaux, J.; Van der Linden, M.; Levaux, M.N.; Mourad, H.; Pirri, A.; Bertrand, H.; Domken, M.A.; Adam, S.; Larøi, F. Multitasking capacities in persons diagnosed with schizophrenia: A preliminary examination of their neurocognitive underpinnings and ability to predict real world functioning. Psychiatry Res. 2014, 217, 163–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laloyaux, J.; Van der Linden, M.; Nuechterlein, K.H.; Thonon, B.; Larøi, F. A direct examination of the cognitive underpinnings of multitasking abilities: A first study examining schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2018, 268, 288–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Larøi, F.; Canlaire, J.; Mourad, H.; Van der Linden, M. Relations between a computerized shopping task and cognitive tests in a group of persons diagnosed with schizophrenia compared with healthy controls. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2010, 16, 180–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Funke, J.; Krüger, T. Plan-A-Day (Computer-Programm); Psychologisches Institut der Universität Bonn: Bonn, Germany, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Holt, D.V.; Wolf, J.; Funke, J.; Weisbrod, M.; Kaiser, S. Planning impairments in schizophrenia: Specificity, task independence and functional relevance. Schizophr. Res. 2013, 149, 174–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holt, D.V.; Rodewald, K.; Rentrop, M.; Funke, J.; Weisbrod, M.; Kaiser, S. The Plan-a-Day approach to measuring planning ability in patients with schizophrenia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 327–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ku, J.; Cho, W.; Kim, J.J.; Peled, A.; Wiederhold, B.K.; Wiederhold, M.D.; Kim, I.Y.; Lee, J.H.; Kim, S.I. A virtual environment for investigating schizophrenic patients’ characteristics: Assessment of cognitive and navigation ability. CyberPsychol. Behav. 2003, 6, 397–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Siemerkus, J.; Irle, E.; Schmidt-Samoa, C.; Dechent, P.; Weniger, G. Egocentric spatial learning in schizophrenia investigated with functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage Clin. 2012, 1, 153–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Canty, A.L.; Fleming, J.; Patterson, F.; Green, H.J.; Man, D.; Shum, D.H. Evaluation of a virtual reality prospective memory task for use with individuals with severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2014, 24, 238–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Man, D.W.; Ganesan, B.; Yip, C.C.; Lee, C.O.; Tsang, S.Y.; Yu, P.W.; Young, J.G.; Shum, D.H. Validation of the virtual-reality prospective memory test (Hong Kong Chinese version) for individuals with first-episode schizophrenia. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2018, 28, 1197–1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plechatá, A.; Hejtmánek, L.; Fajnerová, I. Virtual supermarket shopping task for cognitive rehabilitation and assessment of psychiatric patients: Validation in chronic schizophrenia. Cesk. Psychol. 2021, 65, 14–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kerns, J.G.; Nuechterlein, K.H.; Braver, T.S.; Barch, D.M. Executive functioning component mechanisms and schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 64, 26–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kim, J.; Glahn, D.C.; Nuechterlein, K.H.; Cannon, T.D. Maintenance and manipulation of information in schizophrenia: Further evidence for impairment in the central executive component of working memory. Schizophr. Res. 2004, 68, 173–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reichenberg, A.A. The assessment of neuropsychological functioning in schizophrenia. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2010, 12, e383. [Google Scholar]
- Thornton, A.E.; Kristinsson, H.; DeFreitas, V.G.; Thornton, W.L. The ecological validity of everyday cognition in hospitalized patients with serious mental illness. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2010, 32, 299–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nilsson, N.C.; Nordahl, R.; Serafin, S. Immersion revisited: A review of existing definitions of immersion and their relation to different theories of presence. Hum. Technol. 2016, 12, 108–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Slater, M. Immersion and the illusion of presence in virtual reality. Br. J. Psychol. 2018, 109, 431–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jansari, A.S.; Devlin, A.; Agnew, R.; Akesson, K.; Murphy, L.; Leadbetter, T. Ecological assessment of executive functions: A new virtual reality paradigm. Brain Impair 2014, 15, 71–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Bennekom, M.J.; de Koning, P.P.; Denys, D. Virtual reality objectifies the diagnosis of psychiatric disorders: A literature review. Front. Psychiatry 2017, 8, 163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jeonghun Ku, W.G.; Kim, J.H.; Kim, K.U. The development of a VR system for the cognitive & behavioral assessment of schizophrenia. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 2004, 98, 180–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sorkin, A.; Weinshall, D.; Modai, I.; Peled, A. Improving the accuracy of the diagnosis of schizophrenia by means of virtual reality. Am. J. Psychiatry 2006, 163, 512–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Henry, M.; Joyal, C.C.; Nolin, P. Development and initial assessment of a new paradigm for assessing cognitive and motor inhibition: The bimodal virtual-reality Stroop. J. Neurosci. Methods 2012, 210, 125–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parsons, T.D.; Courtney, C.G.; Dawson, M.E. Virtual reality Stroop task for assessment of supervisory attentional processing. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2013, 35, 812–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parsons, T.D. Ethical Challenges of Using Virtual Environments in the Assessment and Treatment of Psychopathological Disorders. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laguna, K.; Babcock, R.L. Computer anxiety in young and older adults: Implications for human-computer interactions in older populations. Comput. Hum. Behav. 1997, 13, 317–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heydebrand, G.; Weiser, M.; Rabinowitz, J.; Hoff, A.L.; DeLisi, L.E.; Csernansky, J.G. Correlates of cognitive deficits in first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2004, 68, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waters, F.A.; Badcock, J.C.; Maybery, M.T.; Michie, P.T. Inhibition in schizophrenia: Association with auditory hallucinations. Schizophr. Res. 2003, 62, 275–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Good, K.P.; Rabinowitz, J.; Whitehorn, D.; Harvey, P.D.; DeSmedt, G.; Kopala, L.C. The relationship of neuropsychological test performance with the PANSS in antipsychotic naıve, first-episode psychosis patients. Schizophr. Res. 2004, 68, 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Leary, D.S.; Flaum, M.; Kesler, M.L.; Flashman, L.A.; Arndt, S.; Andreasen, N.C. Cognitive correlates of the negative, disorganized, and psychotic symptom dimensions of schizophrenia. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2000, 12, 4–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nieuwenstein, M.R.; Aleman, A.; de Haan, E.H. Relationship between symptom dimensions and neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of WCST and CPT studies. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2001, 35, 119–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amann, B.L.; Gomar, J.J.; Ortiz Gil, J.; McKenna, P.J.; Sans-Sansa, B.; Sarró, S.; Moro, M.; Madre, M.; Landin-Romero, R.; Vieta, E.; et al. Executive dysfunction and memory impairment in schizoaffective disorder: A comparison with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia and healthy controls. Psychol. Med. 2012, 42, 2127–2135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Semkovska, M.; Bédard, M.A.; Godbout, L.; Limoge, F.; Stip, E. Assessment of executive dysfunction during activities of daily living in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2004, 69, 289–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shafer, A.; Dazzi, F. Meta-analysis of the positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) factor structure. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2019, 115, 113–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burgess, N.; Maguire, E.A.; Spiers, H.J.; O’Keefe, J. A temporoparietal and prefrontal network for retrieving the spatial context of lifelike events. NeuroImage 2001, 14, 439–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Folley, B.S.; Astur, R.; Jagannathan, K.; Calhoun, V.D.; Pearlson, G.D. Anomalous neural circuit function in schizophrenia during a virtual Morris water task. NeuroImage 2010, 49, 3373–3384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hawco, C.; Buchy, L.; Bodnar, M.; Izadi, S.; Dell’Elce, J.; Messina, K.; Joober, R.; Malla, A.; Lepage, M. Source retrieval is not properly differentiated from object retrieval in early schizophrenia: An fMRI study using virtual reality. NeuroImage Clin. 2015, 7, 336–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Salgado-Pineda, P.; Landin-Romero, R.; Portillo, F.; Bosque, C.; Pomes, A.; Spanlang, B.; Franquelo, J.C.; Teixido, C.; Sarró, S.; Salvador, R.; et al. Examining hippocampal function in schizophrenia using a virtual reality spatial navigation task. Schizophr. Res. 2016, 172, 86–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leong, V.; Schilbach, L. The promise of two-person neuroscience for developmental psychiatry: Using interaction-based sociometrics to identify disorders of social interaction. Br. J. Psychiatry 2019, 215, 636–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyer, L.; Lakatos, P.; He, Y. Language Dysfunction in Schizophrenia: Assessing Neural Tracking to Characterize the Underlying Disorder (s)? Front. Neurosci. 2020, 15, 163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gainsford, K.; Fitzgibbon, B.; Fitzgerald, P.B.; Hoy, K.E. Transforming treatments for schizophrenia: Virtual reality, brain stimulation and social cognition. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 288, e112974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Duff, K. Evidence-based indicators of neuropsychological change in the individual patient: Relevant concepts and methods. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2012, 27, 248–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bauer, R.M.; Iverson, G.L.; Cernich, A.N.; Binder, L.M.; Ruff, R.M.; Naugle, R.I. Computerized neuropsychological assessment devices: Joint position paper of the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology and the National Academy of Neuropsychology. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2012, 27, 362–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Type | Tool | Diagnostic Validity | Ecological Validity | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Verisimilitude | Veridicality | |||||||
Examined | Confirmed | Developed | Construct | Technological Demands | Examined | Confirmed | ||
Traditional | SCWT [85,86] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [1,87,88]; several meta-analyses | No | Laboratory task: paper-pencil or computerized task | Computer version as non-immersive technology | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on SCWT and scales of community functioning [89,90,91]; several meta-analyses |
Traditional | TOL [85,92] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [93,94,95]; several meta-analyses | No | Laboratory task: paper-pencil or computerized task | Computer version as non-immersive technology | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on TOL and scales of community functioning, scales of social behavior in the milieu, scales of social problem-solving, scales of social skills [7]; one meta-analysis |
Traditional | TMT [85] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [1,87,88]; several meta-analyses | No | Laboratory task: paper-pencil or computerized task | Computer version as non-immersive technology | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on TMT and scales of community functioning [89,91]; two meta-analyses |
Traditional | WCST [85,96] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [1,2,87,88]; several meta-analyses | No | Laboratory task: paper-pencil or computerized task | Computer version as non-immersive technology | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on WCST and scales of community functioning, scales of social behavior in the milieu, scales of social problem-solving, scales of social skills [7]; one meta-analysis |
Traditional | VFT [85,97] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [1,87,88]; several meta-analyses | No | Laboratory task: paper-pencil or computerized task | Lack of computer version | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on VFT and scales of community functioning [7]; one meta-analysis |
Traditional | DS [85,98] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [1,87,99,100]; several meta-analyses | No | Laboratory task: verbal or computerized task | Computer version as non-immersive technology | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on DS and scales of community functioning [7,89,90,91]; several meta-analyses |
Traditional | DST [101] | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [10]; one meta-analysis | No | Laboratory task: verbal or computerized task | Lack of computer version | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on DST and scales of community functioning [7]; one meta-analysis | |
Traditional | LNST [102] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [1]; one meta-analysis | No | Laboratory task: verbal or computerized task | Computer version as non-immersive technology | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on LNST and scales of community functioning [91]; one meta-analysis |
Traditional | SS [85,103] | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [99,100]; two meta-analyses | No | Laboratory task: manual or computerized task | Computer version as non-immersive technology | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on SS and scales of community functioning [89,90,91]; several meta-analyses | |
Ecological | EFPT [104,105] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [104]; single study | Yes | Independent living as manual task: simple cooking, telephone use, medication management, and bill payment | Computer version as non-immersive technology | No | No information |
Ecological | MET [106,107] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [106,108]; two studies | Yes | Manual task: shopping center | Computer version as non-immersive technology | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on MET and two scales of real-world functioning [106]; single study No: no significant correlation between results in MET and two scales of real-world functioning [108]; single study |
Ecological | MSET [109] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [110]; one meta-analysis | Yes | Manual tasks: six realistic problems | Lack of computer version | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between MSET and scale of real-world functioning [111]; single study No: no significant correlation between results on MSET and questionnaire ratings of executive impairment [112]; single study |
Ecological | ZMT [113,114] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [110]; one meta-analysis | Yes | Manual task: map of zoo | Lack of computer version | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between ZMT and questionnaire ratings of executive impairment and scale of real-world functioning [111,112]; two studies |
Virtual reality | VAP-S [115] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [116]; single study | Yes | Virtual reality task: supermarket with aisles displaying items and static virtual people (avatars) | Computer non-immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment, graphic representations of real items | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on VAP-S and results of naturalistic behavioral observations of real-life tasks [117]; single study |
Virtual reality | VCFMT [118] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [118]; single study | Yes | Virtual reality task: city, station, bus and virtual people (avatars) | Computer non-immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment, graphic representations of real items | No | No information |
Virtual reality | CMPT a [119] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [119]; single study | Yes | Virtual reality task: office in a company and virtual people (avatars) | Computer non-immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment, graphic representations of real items | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on CMPT and two scales of real-world functioning [119]; single study |
Virtual reality | CMPT b [120] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [120]; single study | Yes | Virtual reality task: office in a company and virtual people (avatars) | Computer non-immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment, graphic representations of real items | Yes | No: no significant between results on CMPT and two scales of real-world functioning [120]; single study |
Virtual reality | CST [121] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [121]; single study | Yes | Virtual reality task: grocery and virtual people (avatars) | Computer non-immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment, graphic representations of real items | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on CST and scale of real-world functioning [121]; single study |
Virtual reality | PDT [122] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [123,124]; two studies | Yes | Virtual reality task: workplace environment | Computer non-immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment, graphic representations of real items | Yes | Yes: significant correlation between results on PDT and scale of real-world functioning [123,124]; two studies |
Virtual reality | VEP [125] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [125]; single study | Yes | Virtual reality task: rooms with three doors in each and corridors | Computer immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment, graphic representations of real items | No | No information |
Virtual reality | VMT [126] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [126]; single study | Yes | Virtual reality task: maze with walls and green grass | Computer immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment | No | No information |
Virtual reality | VRPMT [127,128] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [128]; single study | Yes | Virtual reality task: shopping center with aisles displaying items and static virtual people (avatars) | Computer non-immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment, graphic representations of real items | No | No information |
Virtual reality | VSST [129] | Yes | Yes: significant difference between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [129]; single study | Yes | Virtual reality task: supermarket with aisles displaying items and static virtual people (avatars) | Computer non-immersive technology, scenario, participants’ activities in the environment, graphic representations of real items | Yes | No: no significant correlation between results on VSST and scale of real-world functioning [129] |
Aspect of Executive Dysfunction | Definition |
---|---|
Monitoring deficits | Problems with the detection and resolution of conflict or detection and correction of errors. |
Inhibition deficits | Diminished ability to withhold automatic reactions or associations that are inappropriate to the currently executed task. |
Attention shifting deficits | Inability to flexibly transition between at least two processes or tasks. |
Planning deficits | Reduced ability to set sequences of steps that lead to the achievement of a particular goal. |
Rule generation deficits | Impaired formation of mental representations of rules concerning ways to solve problems in new situations. |
Abstract thinking deficits | Limitations in ability to break away from the current situational context and to transition to a different aspect that may not be directly related to the current state. |
Working memory deficits | Storing and manipulating verbal or non-verbal information in a short period of time in order to carry out a task. |
Tool | Description | Components |
---|---|---|
Stroop Color Word Test (SCWT [85,86]) | Traditionally three independent tasks constitute the SCWT: quickly identifying the names of colors in black ink; quickly identifying colors depicted as rectangles; and quickly identifying colored words printed in ink of an incongruent color. Manual and computer versions. | Working memory, verbal attention, cognitive inhibition |
Tower of London (TOL [85,92]) | Participants are shown a wooden tower with three pins (small, medium, large) and three balls (green, blue, red); from a predetermined initial state, they are asked to move the balls, one by one, to match a desired end position shown on a picture (12 configuations in total), using the minimum necessary moves (between 2 and 5); if the participant cannot finish or uses more moves, they are asked to start over. Manual and computer versions. | Planning, working memory, visual attention |
Trail Making Test (TMT [85]) | This task is split into two parts. In part A, the participant connects 25 irregularly arranged circles containing numbers from 1 to 25; part B requires alternation between circles arranged irregularly with numbers (1 to 13) and letters (A to L) to form a continuous line. Paper-and-pencil test. | Non-verbal attention, working memory, non-verbal cognitive flexibility |
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST [85,96]) | WCST requires two identical decks of cards (each containing 64 cards) and four reference cards; a participant must place subsequent cards according to their shape, color, and number using the researcher’s feedback. Manual and computer versions. | Working memory, switching, perseveration, problem-solving |
Verbal Fluency Test (VFT [85,97]) | A letter (phonemic) or a category (semantic) task is used. Participants are asked to produce as many unique words as possible within 60 s from each category in the semantic task or words starting with a given letter in the phonemic task. | Working memory, verbal flexibility, word production, verbal attention |
Digit Span (DS [85,98]) | Digit Span is a core subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales. It consists of two parts: (a) Digits Forward characterized as a simple span test in which participants are asked to repeat a series of digits in a correct order, (b) Digits Backward that requires repeating digits in the reverse order of presentation. | Verbal sustained attention, storage and manipulation of information in verbal working memory |
Digit Sequencing Test (DST [101]) | Participants are asked to order series of numbers from lowest to highest. Number of correct responses and the longest sequence recalled correctly are recorded. | Storage and manipulation of information in non-verbal working memory |
Letter Number Sequencing Test (LNST [102]) | Participants are asked to sequence a random order of numbers and letters (numbers in ascending order followed by letters in alphabetical order. | Storage and manipulation of information in non-verbal working memory |
Spatial Span (SS [85,103]) | Task consists of two parts: (a) Spatial Forward which requires participants to point the blocks in a given order, (b) Spatial Backward which requires pointing the blocks in the reverse order. | Visual sustained attention, storage and manipulation of information in non-verbal working memory |
Tool | Description | Components |
---|---|---|
Executive Function Performance Test (EFPT [104,105]) | Completing four tasks related to self-maintenance and independent living: simple cooking, using a telephone, managing medications, and paying bills. | Working memory, multitasking, planning, activities of daily living |
Multiple Errands Test (MET [106,107]) | This test measures everyday executive functioning with real-world activities (e.g., purchasing specific items, arriving at a destination, collecting and writing down information). The sum and type of errors (such as rule breaks and omissions) are registered. | Strategy allocation, planning, working memory |
Modified Six Elements Test (MSET [109]) | This test consists of six tasks: two sets of arithmetic problems two sets of pictures to be named, and two dictation tasks; the participant attempts to solve them according to set rules within 10 min. Tasks of the same type cannot be completed one after the other. | Working memory, strategy application, performance monitoring, planning |
Zoo Map Test (ZMT [113,114]) | Participants plan a route for a visit to the zoo according to rules provided (e.g., starting point, number of possible locations, using paths just once). | Working memory, planning, multitasking |
Tool | Description | Components |
---|---|---|
Virtual Action Plan—Supermarket (VAP-S [115]) | This task emulates a medium size supermarket and the goal is to make purchases based on the list provided. Total distance, time, number of products, correct and incorrect actions, and pauses are recorded. Computer version. | Cognitive planning, working memory, sustained attention |
Virtual Cognitive Flexibility Measurement Task (VCFMT [118]) | The goal of this task is to get to a meeting place by bus; the participant is provided with a destination and recommended bus route. The participant can review a bus route map and choose one from a number of options (wrong, slow, pre-informed, or quick). Computer version. | Decision making, cognitive flexibility, prospective memory, working memory |
Computerized Meeting Preparation Task (CMPT [119]) | The task consists of two parts: learning and meeting preparation. The learning phase familiarizes the participant with the design of the task, which involves progressive learning (total time and number of incorrect actions are recorded). During the meeting preparation phase, the participant is asked to organize a meeting according to the provided rules. The task requires, inter alia, placing guests, bringing office equipment, and preparing drinks. Total time, number of times the instructions were checked, incorrectly placed/missed objects, respect for rules, planning score, and initiation are scored. Computer and real-life version. | Planning, multitasking, working memory |
Computerized Shopping Task (CST [121]) | This task consists of two parts. The learning phase aims to make the participant familiar with the functions and actions available. For the main shopping task, the participant is presented with a list of seven items belonging to distinct categories. Total time, correct items, intrusions, aisle redundancy, corrected errors, number of times in a non-relevant aisle, number of times a non-relevant aisle was visualized, number of times the shopping list was checked, time spent checking the shopping list, and checking the shopping cart are recorded. Computer version. | Planning, multitasking, organizing, prospective memory, working memory |
Plan-a-Day Test (PDT [122]) | This task emulates a workplace environment. The goal of the task is to schedule a list of daily work-related activities based on a given list of tasks. Total time, planning ratio, and number of solved problems without correction are recorded. Computer version. | Planning, organizing, prospective memory, working memory |
Virtual Egyptian Pyramids (VEP [125]) | A participant walks through a series of rooms and corridors in a pyramid. One of three doors needs to be chosen in each room. Doors are labeled with a shape, a color, and a sound. The task mechanics resemble the WCST. Computer version. | Switching, perseveration, problem-solving, working memory, integration of multimodal stimuli |
Virtual Maze Task (VMT [126]) | The maze consists of six identical two-way intersections. Corridors lead either to another intersection or to one of seven cul-de-sacs. Only one dead end contains money (the goal). Five trials with a 5 min time limit are given to reach the goal. Computer version. | Spatial learning, decision making, working memory |
Virtual Reality Prospective Memory Test (VRPMT [127,128]) | An adaptation of the Virtual Reality Shopping Task [127]. It imitates a shopping center with 20 stores. Obtaining items from a shopping list is used as a distraction task. Execution of time-based and event-based prospective memory tasks are recorded. Computer version. | Prospective memory |
Virtual Supermarket Shopping Task (VSST [129]) | Simulates shopping activity in a supermarket. Participants are asked to memorize and collect items from a list. A shopping list cannot be consulted and there is no time limit. Number of correct items, errors, trial time, and trial distance are scored. Computer version. | Planning, multitasking, working memory |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Tyburski, E.; Mak, M.; Sokołowski, A.; Starkowska, A.; Karabanowicz, E.; Kerestey, M.; Lebiecka, Z.; Preś, J.; Sagan, L.; Samochowiec, J.; et al. Executive Dysfunctions in Schizophrenia: A Critical Review of Traditional, Ecological, and Virtual Reality Assessments. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 2782. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10132782
Tyburski E, Mak M, Sokołowski A, Starkowska A, Karabanowicz E, Kerestey M, Lebiecka Z, Preś J, Sagan L, Samochowiec J, et al. Executive Dysfunctions in Schizophrenia: A Critical Review of Traditional, Ecological, and Virtual Reality Assessments. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2021; 10(13):2782. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10132782
Chicago/Turabian StyleTyburski, Ernest, Monika Mak, Andrzej Sokołowski, Anna Starkowska, Ewa Karabanowicz, Magdalena Kerestey, Zofia Lebiecka, Joanna Preś, Leszek Sagan, Jerzy Samochowiec, and et al. 2021. "Executive Dysfunctions in Schizophrenia: A Critical Review of Traditional, Ecological, and Virtual Reality Assessments" Journal of Clinical Medicine 10, no. 13: 2782. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10132782
APA StyleTyburski, E., Mak, M., Sokołowski, A., Starkowska, A., Karabanowicz, E., Kerestey, M., Lebiecka, Z., Preś, J., Sagan, L., Samochowiec, J., & Jansari, A. S. (2021). Executive Dysfunctions in Schizophrenia: A Critical Review of Traditional, Ecological, and Virtual Reality Assessments. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10(13), 2782. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10132782