Next Article in Journal
Comparisons of Two Different Treatment Methods for Impacted Maxillary Canines: A Retrospective Study
Previous Article in Journal
Traumatic Spinal Injury in Children; Time to Revise Pre-Hospital and Diagnostic Protocols?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Indirect Exposure to Atrocities and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms among Aid Workers: Hemispheric Lateralization Matters

1
Department of Social Work, Tel Hai College, Qiryat Shemona 1220800, Israel
2
Research Center for Innovation in Social Work, Tel Hai College, Qiryat Shemona 1220800, Israel
3
The Israeli School of Humanitarian Action, Tel Aviv 4632825, Israel
4
Maccabi Healthcare Services, Tel Aviv 6812509, Israel
5
Brain & Behavior Research Institute, Western Galilee Academic College, Akko 2412101, Israel
6
Department of Nursing, Haifa University, Haifa 3498838, Israel
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2024, 13(8), 2373; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm13082373
Submission received: 29 March 2024 / Revised: 13 April 2024 / Accepted: 17 April 2024 / Published: 18 April 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Mental Health)

Abstract

:
Background: Humanitarian aid workers (HAWs) are indirectly exposed to atrocities relating to people of concern (POC). This may result in a risk of secondary traumatization demonstrated by post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSSs). Previous studies have demonstrated that hemispheric lateralization (HL) moderates the relationship between threat exposure and post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSSs). Aims: We hypothesized that indirect exposure to atrocities (IETA) would be positively correlated with PTSSs among HAWs with right and not left HL. Method: Fifty-four HAWs from several countries that provided humanitarian support in Greece and Colombia participated in this correlational and cross-sectional observation study. They completed scales relating to IETA, PTSSs were assessed using a brief, valid scale, and HL was measured. Results: IETA was positively and significantly related to PTSSs (r = 0.39, p < 0.005). Considering HL, IETA was unrelated to PTSSs among people with right HL (r = 0.29, p = 0.14), while IETA was related to PTSSs among people with left HL (r = 0.52, p = 0.008). Right HL emerged as a protective factor in the relationship between IETA and PTSS. Conclusions: An assessment of dominant HL can serve as one consideration among others when deploying HAWs in specific locations and roles, vis à vis IETA. Moreover, those found to have a higher risk for PTSSs based on their HL could be monitored more closely to prevent adverse reactions to IETA.

1. Introduction

Humanitarian settings, which entail responding to atrocities and disasters, often expose humanitarian aid workers (HAWs) to the risk of injuries, assault, kidnapping, robbery, the hostility of local populations, or other threats [1,2]. These threats can also come along with witnessing or hearing others’ stories of suffering. This exposure can lead to secondary traumatic stress (STS), which is defined as “the stress deriving from helping others who are suffering or who have been traumatized” [3]. These various stressors, which may include STS, vicarious traumatization, and compassion fatigue [4], might lead to post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and a sense of burnout among HAWs in the long run.
Indeed, in a systematic review of fourteen studies, the prevalence of mental health problems, including PTSD, among HAWs was similar to or higher than various controls [5]. Another systematic review and two meta-analyses of 15 studies found moderate to severe STS among HAWs working with POC [6]. More specifically, in a study focusing on 376 HAWs in Uganda, 68% were depressed, and 53% were anxious [7]. In another study focusing on 267 HAWs following an earthquake in Pakistan, the prevalence of PTSD was 42.6% [8]. Other studies found a prevalence of 40% among Singaporean relief teams, with symptoms of acute stress reaction (ASR) [9], and 24.2% of Turkish Red Crescent team members showed PTSD symptoms [10]. Both studies were conducted following the 2004 tsunami in Southeast Asia. The same pattern was identified in a study focusing on 446 HAWs from China, where 30% developed PTSD [11]. Other symptoms related to trauma were found in various studies, such as burnout among 40% of HAWs from 44 different countries [12], and alcohol abuse was found among 16.2% of HAWs in Kosovo [13].
Risk factors for such stress responses include exposure to extremely stressful events such as dealing with dead bodies [14], previous exposure to disaster [10], length of mission [15], and low socio-economic status [16], among other factors. Protective factors, on the other hand, included early experience and training [17], perceived organizational support [18], tolerance for ambiguity [19], coping strategies, emotional intelligence [20], and serving as non-local staff [13].
The studies reviewed above suggest that not all HAWs develop PTSD despite direct or indirect exposure to atrocities (IETA). Are there protective factors in this context? Reviewing the nature of both risk and protective factors shows that these factors can be grouped into demographic background (age and socio-economic status), social environment (support of the organization and support of family and friends), or characteristics of a specific humanitarian mission (length of the mission, being local, or past exposure).
Factors that, to the best of our knowledge, have not yet been explored in a humanitarian context are neuro-psychological factors. One emerging neuro-psychological moderating protective factor is hemispheric lateralization (HL). This reflects the general tendencies to activate or use functions of one hemisphere more than the other, e.g., [21].
Briefly, the two hemispheres modulate immunological and psychological responses differently. The left hemisphere is involved in analytic, local thinking, positive affect, anger, behavioral activation and the approach system, and high cellular immune system functioning. Conversely, the right hemisphere is involved in intuitive, parallel and global perception, negative affect (anxiety and depression), behavioral inhibition and the avoidance system, and poor cellular immunity [22,23,24]. In humans, the side of the dominant hand is weakly related to HL. This was supported in a study of 69 French students using the line bisection test detailed below [25].
Several methods for measuring HL exist. The gold standard measure of HL includes EEG and neuroimaging techniques [26,27]. In those tests, brain activity is measured at rest and when exposed to visual or emotional stimuli. In these tests, one could measure the delta of activity levels between the same regions on the left and right hemispheres. Another measure includes the dichotic listening test, which examines the ability to process auditory information in one ear while masking information provided to the other ear. While this method has been used for decades in research, its reliability and validity are questionable [28]. While these measures are direct measures of HL and brain activity, they require a lab, equipment (e.g., neuroimaging), or a sterile and quiet environment (dichotic listening). A simpler measure suitable to humanitarian field work is the line bisection test. One type of this test is the landmark test, where participants are asked to judge which side is longer in a bisected line [29]. Another type is the line bisection test, where participants receive a series of non-bisected horizontal lines, in which they need to bisect in the middle [30]. In both types, left-side bias reflects RHL and vice versa.
These behavioral and emotional differences mediated by the hemispheres are synthesized within a broader theoretical framework, which we now present.

The Approach-Withdrawal Theory

Gray and McNaughton [31] proposed a behavioral activation system (BAS) and a behavioral inhibition system (BIS) in their reinforcement sensitivity theory. The BAS responds to stimuli of reward and non-punishment, elicits positive emotions, and leads to approach behavior and active avoidance. The BIS responds to the stimuli of punishment and non-reward, as well as to novel stimuli and innate fear-eliciting stimuli, wherein it elicits the affective state of anxiety and leads to behavioral inhibition and passive avoidance. Hofman’s [23] review helps to integrate the diverse and, at times, inconsistent, findings into a more coherent framework. He concluded that the right hemisphere represents predominantly the BIS and withdrawal emotions (anxiety and depression). In contrast, the left hemisphere represents the BAS, which reflects approach emotions and behaviors (positive affect, hostility, and impulsiveness).
The psychological differences controlled by the two hemispheres are also related to the impact of stressors on wellbeing. In a series of studies, exposure to stressors was positively correlated with poor mental health only among people with right HL. Furthermore, an experimental stressor increased perceived stress only among those with right but not with left HL [32]. In a following study, the perceived extent of exposure to missiles was positively correlated with PTSD levels among Israelis with right but not left HL [33].
There are inconsistencies in the literature concerning right versus left brain activity and PTSD. Studies associating LHL show that greater right alpha brain asymmetry (reflecting greater left brain activity) is related to PTSD [34]. In contrast, greater right-side activity in the amygdala, a region that processes threat, was related to exposure to trauma in a meta-analysis [35]. The picture may be more complex, such that certain PTSD symptoms, e.g., intrusive thoughts resulting from verbal processing, may be associated with LHL, while pictorial intrusions and arousal might be associated with RHL. Finally, it may be context-dependent and subject to direct versus indirect exposure to traumatic events.
To the best of our knowledge, the moderating role of HL among HAW has never been examined. The purpose of this study was to explore the moderating role of HL in the relationship between indirect exposure to atrocities and PTSD among HAWs. In line with past studies [32,33], we hypothesized that a positive correlation between IETA and PTSD would be found only among aid workers with right HL.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

The sample was recruited by the principal investigator in a humanitarian context and by approaching HAWs of two international humanitarian agencies who were working with the investigator. Those who had worked for at least a year in Greece and Colombia participated. The first agency is NATAN, an Israeli nongovernmental organization (NGO), and the second organization is Cadena, a Mexican NGO. The nature of the humanitarian work was mental health and psychosocial support, the distribution of non-food items, and legal aid provided to forced migrants due to internal conflicts in their country of origin.
In Greece, the HAWs were working with forced migrants from countries in North Africa or the Middle East. These forced migrants fled their homeland due to internal conflict and threats to their lives due to civil unrest. In Colombia, the forced migrants originated from Venezuela, where civil unrest is taking place due to socio-economic and political adversities.
Fifty-four participants took part in the study; all were above 18 years old and proficient in English. All of the participants gave their consent to participate in this study, which was approved by the ethics committee of Tel Hai academic college.

2.2. Procedure

After providing their informed consent, the participants completed scales assessing background information, an IETA scale, and a PTSD scale. HL was measured via a neuropsychological test (see below).

2.3. Measures

Sociodemographic and background variables: These included age, gender, country of origin, profession, number of past humanitarian missions, number of days in the mission that they participated in while completing the scales of this study, number of people of concern (POC) that the HAWs were exposed to, position in the team (member or management), and their dominant hand.
Level of indirect exposure to atrocities (IETA): This was assessed using a scale of six items that were developed specifically for this study (see Table 1). Each item was rated on a 1–5 scale, reflecting the number of POC, where 1 = none, 2 = 1–10, 3 = 11–20, 4 = 21–50, and 5 = more than 50 POCs to which the HAWs were exposed to.
These items reflected exposure to reports of physical or mental abuse by adults and children. In the present study, the internal reliability of the scale was high (Cronbach alpha = 0.89). The scores across the six items were summed to reflect the level of IETA. Those levels were positively correlated with the number of POC HAWs worked with (r = 0.65, p < 0.001).
PTSD symptoms: These were assessed using a validated seven-item scale of Yes/ No options [36]. Each item referred to a different symptom of PTSD. For instance, one item was, “Did you avoid being reminded of this experience by staying away from certain places, people, or activities?”. Another example is “Did you begin to feel that there was no point in planning for the future?”. A higher sum reflects a higher probability of PTSD. In this study, the internal reliability was low (Cronbach alpha = 0.54).
Hemispheric Lateralization: This was measured using the line bisection test, which was validated against an EEG measure [30]. Additionally, greater left-side biases in this test (reflecting RHL) were found among anxious patients [37]. This test included fourteen horizontal lines, in which the participants had to indicate, with a vertical line, the perceived center of each of the 14 lines. Deviating to the right of the real middle reflects LHL (scored with positive values), while deviating to the left of the real middle reflects RHL (scored with negative values). Participants’ deviations were averaged across the 14 lines. A higher score reflected LHL. The internal reliability was adequate (Cronbach alpha= 0.81).

2.4. Statistical and Data Analyses

These analyses included descriptive statistics and inferential statistics. The latter included Pearson correlations between the IETA scale and PTSD in the full sample and then in the LHL and RHL subsamples (split at the median HL score) separately. This was repeated with partial correlations controlling for relevant confounders.
To test the interaction effect of HL X exposure, we used hierarchical multivariate regression. We tested whether the interaction of HL X exposure added to explaining the variance in PTSD beyond the effects of exposure and HL’s main effects alone.
All of the statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 27.

3. Results

Table 2 depicts the characteristics of the sample. The majority of participants came from Greece (27.6%) and the Netherlands (17.2%), and the rest came from various other countries. The mean age of the participants was just over 31 and ranged between 19 and 65 years. The mean number of humanitarian missions per individual HAW was just below six. Sixty percent had a clinical profession (physicians, nurses, etc.), and the majority were women.
In the t-test, there was no significant difference between left- and right-handed participants in terms of HL (t(81) = 0.77, p = 0.44).
In the full sample, exposure to atrocities was positively and significantly related to PTSD (r = 0.39, p < 0.005). We conducted a further exploratory analysis to examine which specific atrocity item correlated with PTSD. In this analysis, the scores of the first two items (hearing witness statements from POC about atrocities) were unrelated to PTSD, while the scores of the other four items (directly seeing adults or children POC who had experienced physical/mental abuses) were positively and significantly correlated with PTSD (see Table 2).
In hierarchical multivariate regression, the interaction of HL X exposure added a non-significant 1.1% to the variance in PTSD after controlling for the effects of exposure and HL’s main effects alone (F-change (1.79) = 0.86, p = 0.36). Even though this interaction was not significant, we nevertheless apriori conceptualized HL as a dichotomized variable—left versus right HL.
We then classified the participants according to the median HL. Importantly, there were no significant differences in terms of the extent of exposure to atrocities (t(51) = 0.4, p > 0.05) and PTSD scores (t(51) = 1.3, p > 0.05) between those with LHL and RHL.
The extent of exposure to atrocities was unrelated to PTSD among people with RHL (n = 27, r = 0.29, p = 0.14), while extent of exposure to atrocities was related to PTSD among people with LHL (n = 25, r = 0.52, p = 0.008). When controlling for the effects of age, gender and profession, exposure to atrocities was still unrelated to PTSD among people with RHL (r = 0.24, p = 0.26), while exposure to atrocities remained related to PTSD among people with LHL (r = 0.51, p = 0.03).
The previous analysis was based on a mathematical median HL cutoff, not necessarily reflecting a real HL. Thus, we performed another analysis by splitting the sample according to those with a mean positive deviation from the middle (LHL) and those with a mean negative deviation from the middle (RHL) to reflect HL more precisely. In this classification, approximately 70% of the sample had RHL, and approximately 30% of the sample had LHL. The exposure to atrocities only tended to be related to PTSD among people with RHL (r = 0.28, p = 0.09), while exposure to atrocities was significantly and strongly related to PTSD among people with LHL (r = 0.57, p = 0.03).
Finally, we ran several analyses to rule out additional explanations. There was no significant difference in the percentage of HAW with clinical professions between those with LHL and RHL (x2 (1) = 2.3, p > 0.05). Furthermore, none of the background variables (e.g., age, gender, profession, etc.) were related to PTSD.

4. Discussion

The objective of this study was to examine the relationships between the newly developed IETA experienced by POC and the PTSD symptoms of HAWs as a function of HL.
For the purpose of the present study, we developed a new Indirect Exposure to Atrocities scale. The internal reliability of the scale and its correlations with both the number of POC that the participants worked with and their PTSD level support the reliability and validity of this newly developed scale.
In the full sample, exposure to the atrocities experienced by POC was positively and significantly correlated to PTSD among HAWs. Importantly, this correlation was significant only in HAWs with LHL and not with RHL. These correlations remained intact after statistically controlling for age, gender, and profession. These findings suggest that mere exposure to humanitarian crises affects the psychological wellbeing of HAWs, especially among those with LHL.
These findings contradict our hypothesis, in which we anticipated that among HAWs with RHL there would be a positive correlation between levels of IETA and PTSD, similar to past studies among other populations [32,33]. In those studies, exposure to stressful life events or acute stress positively correlated with poor mental health in people with RHL. However, in the current study, surprisingly, these correlations were only observed among those with LHL.
The correlation between the level of IETA and PTSD, specifically in HAWs with LHL, can be explained through the lens of brain asymmetry and its impact on the indirect processing of traumatic experiences. Studies have shown that the left hemisphere is typically associated with language processing and analytical thinking, while the right hemisphere is linked to emotional processing and arousal [38]. In the context of PTSD, the verbal re-experiencing component of the disorder (intrusive thoughts), which involves the persistent activation of neural networks related to traumatic memories, may be more pronounced in individuals with left hemispheric dominance [39]. Research has indicated that individuals with left hemispheric dominance may exhibit altered functional connectivity patterns, particularly in the frontal areas, which could contribute to the manifestation of PTSD symptoms [40]. Furthermore, right alpha asymmetry (greater LHL) is associated with PTSD severity [34]. Additionally, structural abnormalities in brain regions such as the corpus callosum and the cerebellum have been associated with PTSD, suggesting that interhemispheric communication and cerebellar function play a role in the disorder [41,42]. The protective role of right hemispheric lateralization in mitigating the impact of indirect exposure to atrocities on PTSD among HAWs observed in the present study could be attributed to the right hemisphere’s involvement in visual–spatial processing and emotion regulation [43]. HAWs in a humanitarian context may process and re-experience the atrocities more verbally due to the indirect nature of the exposure. The right hemisphere may facilitate more adaptive coping mechanisms in terms of processing traumatic experiences, thereby reducing the risk of developing severe PTSD symptoms [44] when indirectly exposed to atrocities. In conclusion, the observed correlation between IETA and PTSD, specifically in individuals with LHL, among HAWs underscores the importance of considering hemispheric lateralization in understanding the psychological impact of indirect exposure to atrocities. The findings suggest that individuals with left hemispheric dominance may be more vulnerable to the development of PTSD symptoms following indirect exposure to traumatic experiences, highlighting the need for tailored interventions and support strategies based on an individual’s hemispheric dominance.
An additional explanation could be derived from the differences in terms of approach versus avoiding coping mechanisms between RHL and LHL individuals. One of the characteristics of LHL is an approach coping mechanism that includes the tendency to approach other people compared to those with RHL, who are characterized by more avoidance behavior [45,46]. Consequently, among HAWs with LHL, approach coping may result in greater attachment to the POC reporting their atrocities, with the outcome being an increased risk of PTSD during their mission. Though the right hemisphere is associated with empathy [47], a term relevant to our results, the behavioral–emotional patterns of approach coping (LHL) versus avoidance coping (RHL) may have had a more important role, thus explaining our pattern of results.
In contrast, among HAWs with RHL, avoidance behavior may distance them from the POC, hence minimizing the risk of PTSD even if exposed to atrocities. Furthermore, it is possible that the global perception, together with the avoidance of people with RHL, could be manifested in the ability to perceive their experience as part of a broader mission and humanitarian goal. This could help to minimize and put their IETA in perspective, a cognition found to regulate emotion in a meta-analysis of 306 to regulate emotion [48].
Risk factors for adverse psychological symptoms among HAWs can be grouped into pre-deployment factors, such as training and personality; peri-deployment factors, such as trauma exposure; and post-deployment factors, such as social support and media [49]. The current study focused on a peri-deployment factor of POC reported atrocities and identified a pre-deployment factor, namely HL, which moderates the effect of atrocities on PTSD symptoms.
We explored which specific items of the IETA scale correlated with PTSD. Indeed, four of the six items reflecting witnessing damage to adults or children among POC positively correlated with PTSD. In contrast, indirectly witnessing (hearing and not seeing) items were unrelated to PTSD. These results suggest that visually processing atrocities may have a greater impact on HAWs than hearing about atrocities alone. These results point to the gradation of the IETA experience. These results are in line with past studies that found differences in PTSD prevalence subject to direct vs. indirect exposure to stressful events or atrocities [50] and that indirect exposure might lead to PTSD, yet with lower probability [51].
The present study had several limitations. First, the size of the sample was relatively small. This may have masked potentially significant results should we have had a larger sample. However, a larger sample may not change the effect sizes observed in the present study. Importantly, there was no a priori sample size calculation since we did not know in advance how many people would participate from a humanitarian context. It should be noted that the relatively small sample size resulted from the nature and the context in which the study was performed—an emergency humanitarian disaster.
Second, we did not include a neurological measure of HL, such as EEG. Furthermore, factors influencing resilience and stress, such as familiarity with peers or pre-deployment training, were not explored in this study. In addition, secondary traumatization was not formally assessed with a suitable instrument. Instead, we assessed PTSSs because we anticipated a more direct reaction in HAWs when viewing actual physical manifestations of atrocities relating to POC. Finally, this was a cross-sectional study; therefore, no directionality of association can be inferred. HL has state and trait characteristics; therefore, it is possible that, due to the IETA, participants’ HL may have shifted to the right side [52]. Future studies should measure HL several times, including during pre-deployment, as a baseline. These results need to be replicated in a larger sample using neurological measures and a prospective design together with an assessment of secondary traumatization.
Future studies can explore not only the dominant hemisphere but also the extent of this dominance. Secondly, humanitarian agencies can measure the HL of their staff or their newly recruited workers. An assessment of the dominant HL can serve as one consideration among others in deploying HAWs in specific locations and roles, vis à vis IETA. Moreover, those found with a higher risk for PTSD based on their HL can be monitored more closely to prevent adverse reactions to IETA.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization—E.L., Y.G., D.H. and C.H.R.; Writing—original draft preparation, E.L. and Y.G.; Writing—review and editing—E.L., Y.G., D.H. and C.H.R.; Visualization—E.L., Y.G. and D.H.; Supervision, Y.G.; Data collection, E.L. and R.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

OLAM TOGEHER organization and Natan worldwide disaster relief supported this study.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by ethics committee of the Tel Hai academic college No. 05/2021 and date of approval 5 April 2021.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

If required, our data can be submitted.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

ASRAcute stress reaction
HAWHumanitarian aid workers
IETAIndirect exposure to atrocities
LHLLeft hemispheric lateralization
RHLRight hemispheric lateralization
NG0Nongovernmental organization
POCPeople of concern
PTSDPost-traumatic stress disorder
PTSSPost-traumatic stress symptoms
STSSecondary traumatic stress

References

  1. Connorton, E.; Perry, M.J.; Hemenway, D.; Miller, M. Humanitarian relief workers and trauma-related mental illness. Epidemiol. Rev. 2012, 34, 145–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Macpherson, R.I.; Burkle, F.M. Humanitarian aid workers: The forgotten first responders. Prehosp. Disaster Med. 2021, 36, 111–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Figley, C.R. Compassion fatigue: Toward a new understanding of the cost of caring. In Secondary Traumatic Stress; Stamm, B.H., Ed.; Sidran Institute: Towson, MD, USA, 1999; pp. 3–28. [Google Scholar]
  4. Greinacher, A.; Derezza-Greeven, C.; Herzog, W.; Nikendei, C. Secondary traumatization in first responders: A systematic review. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 2019, 10, 1562840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Strohmeier, H.; Scholte, W.F. Trauma-related mental health problems among national humanitarian staff: A systematic review of the literature. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 2015, 6, 28541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Roberts, F.; Teague, B.; Lee, J.; Rushworth, I. The Prevalence of Burnout and Secondary Traumatic Stress in Professionals and Volunteers Working With Forcibly Displaced People: A Systematic Review and Two Meta-Analyses. J. Trauma. Stress 2021, 34, 773–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Ager, A.; Pasha, E.; Yu, G.; Duke, T.; Eriksson, C.; Cardozo, B.L. Stress, mental health, and burnout in national humanitarian aid workers in Gulu, Northern Uganda. J. Trauma. Stress 2012, 25, 713–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Ehring, T.; Razik, S.; Emmelkamp, P.M. Prevalence and predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder, anxiety, depression, and burnout in Pakistani earthquake recovery workers. Psychiatry Res. 2011, 185, 161–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Chan, A.O.; Huak, C.Y. Emotional impact of 2004 Asian tsunami on Singapore medical relief workers. Int. J. Disaster Med. 2004, 2, 152–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Armagan, E.; Engindeniz, Z.; Devay, A.O.; Erdur, B.; Ozcakir, A. Frequency of post-traumatic stress disorder among relief force workers after the tsunami in Asia: Do rescuers become victims? Prehosp. Disaster Med. 2006, 21, 168–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zhen, Y.; Huang, Z.Q.; Jin, J.; Deng, X.Y.; Zhang, L.P.; Wang, J.G. Posttraumatic stress disorder of Red Cross nurses in the aftermath of the 2008 Wenchuan China earthquake. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 2012, 26, 63–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Eriksson, C.B.; Bjorck, J.P.; Larson, L.C.; Walling, S.M.; Trice, G.A.; Fawcett, J.; Abernethy, A.D.; Foy, D.W. Social support, organisational support, and religious support in relation to burnout in expatriate humanitarian aid workers. Ment. Health Relig. Cult. 2009, 12, 671–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cardozo, B.L.; Holtz, T.H.; Kaiser, R.; Gotway, C.A.; Ghitis, F.; Toomey, E.; Salama, P. The mental health of expatriate and Kosovar Albanian humanitarian aid workers. Disasters 2005, 29, 152–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Dobashi, K.; Nagamine, M.; Shigemura, J.; Tsunoda, T.; Shimizu, K.; Yoshino, A.; Nomura, S. Psychological effects of disaster relief activities on Japan ground self-defense force personnel following the 2011 Great East Japan Earthquake. Psychiatry 2014, 77, 190–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Holtz, T.H.; Salama, P.; Cardozo, B.L.; Gotway, C.A. Mental health status of human rights workers, Kosovo, June 2000. J. Trauma Stress Off. Publ. Int. Soc. Trauma Stress Stud. 2002, 15, 389–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Shah, S.A.; Garland, E.; Katz, C. Secondary traumatic stress: Prevalence in humanitarian aid workers in India. Traumatology 2007, 13, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Ghodsi, H.; Sohrabizadeh, S.; Jazani, R.K.; Kavousi, A. Factors affecting resiliency among volunteers in disasters: A systematic literature review. Disaster Med. Public Health Prep. 2020, 16, 398–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Aldamman, K.; Tamrakar, T.; Dinesen, C.; Wiedemann, N.; Murphy, J.; Hansen, M.; Vallières, F. Caring for the mental health of humanitarian volunteers in traumatic contexts: The importance of organisational support. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 2019, 10, 1694811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Dagan, K.; Itzhaky, H.; Ben-Porat, A. Therapists working with trauma victims: The contribution of personal, environmental, and professional-organizational resources to secondary traumatization. J. Trauma Dissociation 2015, 16, 592–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Akinsulure-Smith, A.M.; Espinosa, A.; Chu, T.; Hallock, R. Secondary traumatic stress and burnout among refugee resettlement workers: The role of coping and emotional intelligence. J. Trauma. Stress 2018, 31, 202–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Davidson, R.J. What does the prefrontal cortex “do” in affect: Perspectives on frontal EEG asymmetry research. Biol. Psychol. 2004, 67, 219–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Goel, V.; Tierney, M.; Sheesley, L.; Bartolo, A.; Vartanian, O.; Grafman, J. Hemispheric specialization in human prefrontal cortex for resolving certain and uncertain inferences. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 2245–2250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Hofman, D. The frontal laterality of emotion: A historical overview. Neth. J. Psychol. 2008, 64, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sumner, R.C.; Parton, A.; Nowicky, A.V.; Kishore, U.; Gidron, Y. Hemispheric lateralisation and immune function: A systematic review of human research. J. Neuroimmunol. 2011, 240, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Amiri, C.; Lelorain, S.; Herzog, D.; Gidron, Y. Daily Hassles, Coping and Well-Being: The Moderating Role of Hemispheric Lateralization. Neuropsychiatry 2018, 8, 739–744. [Google Scholar]
  26. Horato, N.; Quagliato, L.A.; Nardi, A.E. The relationship between emotional regulation and hemispheric lateralization in depression: A systematic review and a meta-analysis. Transl. Psychiatry 2022, 12, 162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Metzen, D.; Genç, E.; Getzmann, S.; Larra, M.F.; Wascher, E.; Ocklenburg, S. Frontal and parietal EEG alpha asymmetry: A large-scale investigation of short-term reliability on distinct EEG systems. Brain Struct. Funct. 2022, 227, 725–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Westerhausen, R. A primer on dichotic listening as a paradigm for the assessment of hemispheric asymmetry. Laterality Asymmetries Body Brain Cogn. 2019, 24, 740–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Toraldo, A.; McIntosh, R.D.; Dijkerman, H.C.; Milner, A.D. A revised method for analysing neglect using the landmark task. Cortex 2004, 40, 415–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Nash, K.; McGregor, I.; Inzlicht, M. Line bisection as a neural marker of approach motivation. Psychophysiology 2010, 47, 979–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gray, J.A.; McNaughton, N. The neuropsychology of anxiety: Reprise. In Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1995: Perspectives on Anxiety, Panic, and Fear; Hope, D.A., Ed.; University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln, NE, USA, 1996; pp. 61–134. [Google Scholar]
  32. Herzog, D.; Killianova, T.; Pauwels, S.; Germeys, F.; Gidron, Y. Hemispheric lateralization moderates the life events–distress relationship. Stress Health 2016, 32, 47–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Herzog, D.; Farchi, M.; Gidron, Y. The relationship between exposure to missiles and PTSD symptoms as a function of hemispheric preference in Israelis. J. Trauma Dissociation 2018, 19, 59–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Butt, M.; Espinal, E.; Aupperle, R.L.; Nikulina, V.; Stewart, J.L. The electrical aftermath: Brain signals of posttraumatic stress disorder filtered through a clinical lens. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 451154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hosseini-Kamkar, N.; Farahani, M.V.; Nikolic, M.; Stewart, K.; Goldsmith, S.; Soltaninejad, M.; Rajabli, R.; Lowe, C.; Nicholson, A.A.; Morton, J.B.; et al. Adverse life experiences and brain function: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging findings. JAMA Netw. Open 2023, 6, e2340018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Breslau, N.; Peterson, E.L.; Kessler, R.C.; Schultz, L.R. Short screening scale for DSM-IV posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 1999, 156, 908–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Wei, H.E.; Hao, C.H.; Zhang, Y.; Shaohua, Y.U.; Wei, C.H.; Wei, W.A. Line bisection performance in patients with generalized anxiety disorder and treatment-resistant depression. Int. J. Med. Sci. 2010, 7, 224. [Google Scholar]
  38. Raja Beharelle, A.; Dick, A.S.; Josse, G.; Solodkin, A.; Huttenlocher, P.R.; Levine, S.C.; Small, S.L. Left hemisphere regions are critical for language in the face of early left focal brain injury. Brain 2010, 133, 1707–1716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Engdahl, B.; Leuthold, A.C.; Tan, H.M.; Lewis, S.M.; Winskowski, A.M.; Dikel, T.N.; Georgopoulos, A.P. Post-traumatic stress disorder: A right temporal lobe syndrome? J. Neural Eng. 2010, 7, 066005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Misaki, M.; Phillips, R.; Zotev, V.; Wong, C.K.; Wurfel, B.E.; Krueger, F.; Feldner, M.; Bodurka, J. Connectome-wide investigation of altered resting-state functional connectivity in war veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. NeuroImage Clin. 2018, 17, 285–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Saar-Ashkenazy, R.; Cohen, J.E.; Guez, J.; Gasho, C.; Shelef, I.; Friedman, A.; Shalev, H. Reduced corpus-callosum volume in posttraumatic stress disorder highlights the importance of interhemispheric connectivity for associative memory. J. Trauma Stress 2014, 27, 18–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Baldaçara, L.; Borgio, J.G.F.; Araújo, C.; Nery-Fernandes, F.; Lacerda, A.L.T.; Moraes, W.A.D.S.; Montaño, M.B.M.M.; Rocha, M.; Quarantini, L.C.; Schoedl, A.; et al. Relationship between structural abnormalities in the cerebellum and dementia, posttraumatic stress disorder and bipolar disorder. Dement. Neuropsychol. 2012, 6, 203–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Brookshire, G.; Casasanto, D. Motivation and motor control: Hemispheric specialization for approach motivation reverses with handedness. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e36036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Vasterling, J.J.; Duke, L.M.; Tomlin, H.R.; Lowery, N.; Kaplan, E. Global–local visual processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2004, 10, 709–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Harmon-Jones, E.; Gable, P.A.; Peterson, C.K. The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biol. Psychol. 2010, 84, 451–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Miller, G.A.; Crocker, L.D.; Spielberg, J.M.; Infantolino, Z.P.; Heller, W. Issues in localization of brain function: The case of lateralized frontal cortex in cognition, emotion, and psychopathology. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Hecht, D. Cerebral lateralization of pro-and anti-social tendencies. Exp. Neurobiol. 2014, 23, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Webb, T.L.; Miles, E.; Sheeran, P. Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Brooks, S.K.; Dunn, R.; Sage, C.A.; Amlôt, R.; Greenberg, N.; Rubin, G.J. Risk and resilience factors affecting the psychological wellbeing of individuals deployed in humanitarian relief roles after a disaster. J. Ment. Health 2015, 24, 385–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Zimering, R.; Gulliver, S.B.; Knight, J.; Munroe, J.; Keane, T.M. Posttraumatic stress disorder in disaster relief workers following direct and indirect trauma exposure to Ground Zero. J. Trauma Stress Off. Publ. Int. Soc. Trauma Stress Stud. 2006, 19, 553–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. May, C.L.; Wisco, B.E. Defining trauma: How level of exposure and proximity affect risk for posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 2016, 8, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lewis, R.S.; Weekes, N.Y.; Wang, T.H. The effect of a naturalistic stressor on frontal EEG asymmetry, stress, and health. Biol. Psychol. 2007, 75, 239–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. The table shows the correlations between atrocities items and PTSD.
Table 1. The table shows the correlations between atrocities items and PTSD.
VariableR
How many adults have told you about their physical atrocities?0.25
How many adults told you about their mental atrocities and abuses?0.22
How many adults have you seen with physical injuries?0.36 **
How many adults have you seen suffering?0.32 *
How many children (<age 18) have you seen with injuries?0.34 *
How many children have you seen suffering?0.32 *
PTSD—post-traumatic stress disorder * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 2. The table shows the sample characteristics.
Table 2. The table shows the sample characteristics.
a.
Categorical variables
VariablePercent
Gender
Men25.9%
Women74.1%
Profession
Technical/Logistics40%
Clinical60%
Dominant hand
Right94.4%
Left5.6%
b.
Continuous
VariableMean (SD)
Age31.58 (9.56)
Number of missions5.86 (16.19)
Hemispheric lateralization−0.19 (0.41)
Exposure18.82 (6.93)
Perceived threat1.96 (0.96)
PTSD1.06 (1.25)
PTSD—post-traumatic stress disorder.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Levy, E.; Herzog, D.; Ryder, C.H.; Grunstein, R.; Gidron, Y. Indirect Exposure to Atrocities and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms among Aid Workers: Hemispheric Lateralization Matters. J. Clin. Med. 2024, 13, 2373. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm13082373

AMA Style

Levy E, Herzog D, Ryder CH, Grunstein R, Gidron Y. Indirect Exposure to Atrocities and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms among Aid Workers: Hemispheric Lateralization Matters. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2024; 13(8):2373. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm13082373

Chicago/Turabian Style

Levy, Einav, Daniela Herzog, Chen Hanna Ryder, Rachel Grunstein, and Yori Gidron. 2024. "Indirect Exposure to Atrocities and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms among Aid Workers: Hemispheric Lateralization Matters" Journal of Clinical Medicine 13, no. 8: 2373. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm13082373

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop