Next Article in Journal
Fast Tac Metabolizers at Risk—It is Time for a C/D Ratio Calculation
Next Article in Special Issue
Lifetime Psychotic Symptoms, Subthreshold Depression and Cognitive Impairment as Barriers to Functional Recovery in Patients with Bipolar Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Poor Efficacy of L-Acetylcarnitine in the Treatment of Asthenozoospermia in Patients with Type 1 Diabetes
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health (CRASH): Its Validity and Reliability

by
Silvia Amoretti
1,2,3,
Bibiana Cabrera
1,2,3,
Carla Torrent
2,3,4,5,
Caterina del Mar Bonnín
2,3,4,5,
Gisela Mezquida
1,2,3,4,
Marina Garriga
2,3,4,5,
Esther Jiménez
2,3,4,5,
Anabel Martínez-Arán
2,3,4,5,
Brisa Solé
2,3,4,5,
Maria Reinares
2,3,4,5,
Cristina Varo
2,3,4,5,
Rafael Penadés
1,2,3,4,
Iria Grande
2,3,4,5,
Estela Salagre
2,3,4,5,
Eduard Parellada
1,2,3,4,
Miquel Bioque
1,2,3,4,
Clemente Garcia-Rizo
1,2,3,4,
Ana Meseguer
1,2,
Gerard Anmella
1,3,5,
Adriane R Rosa
6,7,8,
Fernando Contreras
2,9,
Gemma Safont
2,3,10,
Eduard Vieta
2,3,4,5 and
Miquel Bernardo
1,2,3,4,*
add Show full author list remove Hide full author list
1
Barcelona Clinic Schizophrenia Unit, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, Neuroscience Institute, 08036 Barcelona, Spain
2
Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health (CIBERSAM), 28029 Madrid, Spain
3
University of Barcelona, 08036 Barcelona, Spain
4
August Pi I Sunyer Biomedical Research Institute (IDIBAPS), 08036 Barcelona, Spain
5
Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Institute of Neurosciences, 08036 Barcelona, Spain
6
Laboratory of Molecular Psychiatry, Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre 90035-003, Brazil
7
Postgraduate Program: Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Porto Alegre 90040-060, Brazil
8
Department of Pharmacology and Postgraduate Program: Pharmacology and Therapeutics, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Porto Alegre 90040-060, Brazil
9
Psychiatry Department, Bellvitge University Hospital-IDIBELL, 08907 L’Hospitalet de Llobregat, Spain
10
Hospital Universitari Mutua Terrassa, 08221 Terrassa, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2019, 8(5), 586; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8050586
Submission received: 29 March 2019 / Revised: 18 April 2019 / Accepted: 25 April 2019 / Published: 28 April 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Improving Cognitive Functioning in People with Schizophrenia)

Abstract

:
(1) Background: The cognitive reserve (CR) concept has not been precisely defined in severe mental disorders and has been estimated using heterogeneous methods. This study aims to investigate and develop the psychometric properties of the Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health (CRASH), an instrument designed to measure CR in people with severe mental illness; (2) Methods: 100 patients with severe mental illness (non-affective psychoses and affective disorders) and 66 healthy controls were included. The internal consistency and convergent validity of CRASH were assessed. Spearman’s correlations coefficients were also performed to examine the relationship between CRASH and neuropsychological tests, psychosocial functioning, and clinical course; (3) Results: The internal consistency was high (Cronbach’s alpha coefficient = 0.903). The CRASH global score had a large positive correlation with the Cognitive reserve questionnaire total score (r = 0.838, p < 0.001), demonstrating good convergent validity. The correlation coefficients between the CRASH total scores and clinical, functional, and neuropsychological performance were different between groups. In order to provide clinical interpretation, severity classification based on diagnosis (non-affective psychotic disorders, affective disorders, and healthy controls) have been created; (4) Conclusions: CRASH is the first CR measure developed specifically for patients with severe mental illness, facilitating reliable and valid measurement of this construct. The scale may aid in the stratification of patients and the implementation of personalized interventions.

1. Introduction

The concept of cognitive reserve (CR) has been defined as the ability of the brain to make flexible and efficient use of cognitive networks in order to minimize the clinical manifestation of the pathology of dementia [1]. At the beginning, the concept of CR was developed in the context of aging and dementia. In this field, it is hypothesized that CR includes the capacity to withstand damage, the ability to compensate for damage through the use of alternate networks, and remodelling and plasticity [2]. Stern proposes at least two types of reserve: the first relates to the use of brain structures or networks not normally used in order to compensate for brain damage and the second concerns the use of brain networks or cognitive paradigms that are less susceptible to disruption when coping with task demands (used by healthy individuals as a normal process) [2]. Therefore, this second type could explain the variation in the performance of the healthy individual, particularly when they must perform to the maximum of their capacity [2].
In the field of neuropsychiatric disorders, there have been attempts to associate CR with clinical expression and there is an increasing evidence showing that CR may be considered as a resilience factor [3]. Some studies in patients with a first episode of psychosis (FEP), schizophrenia, or bipolar disorder suggest that high CR is associated with a later onset of psychosis and better recovery, and it is considered to be a positive moderator of the impact of pathology on clinical course, functional outcome, and cognitive performance [3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12]. However, in mental disorders the way to conceptualize CR remains open to debate. To date, the most common proposed proxies of CR include estimated premorbid intellectual quotient (IQ), educational level, occupational attainment, and leisure activities [4,8,9,10,12]. Nevertheless, CR has been estimated using heterogeneous methods [4,8,9,10], which makes it difficult to compare studies. Therefore, there is a need to create a specific scale for the assessment of this relevant construct. For example, in order to assess the proxy “Education-Occupation”, de la Serna et al. [4] took into account the number of years of obligatory education that subjects had completed; school performance before the beginning of the disorder; parents’ educational level; and questions about the children’s development in terms of language, reading, writing, and motor functions. Instead, Forcada et al. [8] included quantifications of educational and work attainment during youth and adulthood. However, Anaya et al. [9] used the number of years of formal education and the Scale for Occupational Prestige PRESCA-2. In a FEP sample, we included the number of years of completed obligatory education, parent’s educational level, and the life time school performance [10]. Overall, the lack of a standardizing procedure to quantify CR in mental disorders makes it difficult to optimally compare studies.
Different instruments are available to assess CR in both healthy populations and those with dementia, including the “Lifetime of Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ)” [13], “cognitive reserve questionnaire (CRQ)” [14], “CR Index questionnaire (CRIq)” [15] and “cognitive reserve scale (CRS)” [16]. Nevertheless, taking into account that the emergence of a FEP usually occurs during late adolescence or early adulthood (between 15 and 30 years of age), the aforementioned instruments were not specifically created to assess populations with a severe mental illness (SMI) such as a first episode of psychosis (FEP), schizophrenia, or bipolar disorder.
More specifically, the LEQ [13] consists of 42 questions, and it was designed for individuals over the age of 65 years or those already retired. The LEQ measures educational, occupational, and cognitive lifestyle activities at different stages of life. It is not suitable for people with a severe mental illness. In addition, it takes approximately 30 min, which makes it difficult to implement in clinical practice.
The CRQ [14] consists of eight items that measure different aspects of intellectual activity: schooling, training courses, parents’ schooling, the occupation performed throughout life, musical training, and language proficiency. In addition, it inquires about the approximate frequency of the cognitively stimulating activities. In spite of being a quick scale and widely used in dementia, it has the disadvantage of not valuing the different previous stages of life. The variety of intellectual, psychical, and leisure activities have not been taken into account, nor the type of courses carried out. Thus, it has never been tested in severe mental illness.
As far as the CRS [15] is concerned, it consists of 24 items that measure participation in cognitively stimulating activities throughout a person’s lifetime and is divided into three different life stages. The CRS is divided into four categories: activities of daily life, training, information, hobbies, and social life, and the mean score on each item is corrected for the possible effect of the additional period, called late adulthood, for elderly adults. The educational and professional attainments items are not included in the scale despite being frequently used in the measurement of CR [4,7,8,9,10,12,13,14,16].
Finally, the CRIq [16] includes 20 items divided into 3 sections: CRI—education, CRI—working activity, and CRI—leisure time. It does not value continued studies as an occupation (which is important for SMI patients), or the educational level reached. Instead, this scale values the number of years of education, which makes it not possible to differentiate between a person who has passed a particular level of studies and someone who has repeated a year of schooling and may not have passed. The working activity and leisure time sections record the years rounded off on a five-year scale (0–5–10–15–20), rounded up to the nearest 5-year period, and the years for which the frequency of leisure activities is to be stated are those of the person’s whole adult life, making it unsuitable for patients with a SMI. In addition, neither does it separately assess the different stages of life.
Overall, considering that the age of onset of the more prevalent SMI is usually during the final adolescence and young adulthood, the previous questionnaires are not adapted to the characteristics that should be assessed in a young population with an SMI. Critical issues when assessing CR on SMI populations might be benefited of considering, for example, whether the person is working or the maximum level reached at work, while also scoring training occupational programs. Other questionnaires do not value the accumulation of CR in different age stages, or items that have been considered relevant such as language trainings. Modifiable factors such as intellectual and leisure activities (and the variety and frequency of activities) have not always been considered.
The onset of a SMI supposes an interruption in life development at early stages that is closely related to the later functional outcomes. Furthermore, neuropsychiatric developmental disorders (such as the SMI) must be differentiated from other neurodegenerative pathologies when evaluating CR. Unlike severe brain damage or dementia, in cases of SMI, especially in schizophrenia, CR is considered to be mediated by the own nature of the pathology’s neurodevelopment [3], hence the necessity for a specific assessment instrument. Although assessing CR could benefit in the understanding of the two illnesses (neurodegenerative and neuropsychiatric), the fact that the onset of the SMI is many years before the onset of a neurodegenerative illness gives an add-on value on the field of SMI. People with SMI present a prolonged illness course and the application of personalized intervention on CR could, to a certain extent, alter the progression of the illness. Accordingly the Schizophrenia and the Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Units at the Hospital Clinic de Barcelona developed the Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health (CRASH), a specific instrument for the assessment of CR in SMI populations. This study aimed to develop and investigate the psychometric properties of the CRASH.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

For the present study a sample of 100 patients with SMI and 66 healthy controls (HC) were recruited. Participants were enrolled through the Schizophrenia and the Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Units at the Hospital Clinic de Barcelona. Both Units belong to a University Hospital and collaborate in the context of the Spanish Biomedical Research Networking Center on Mental Health (CIBERSAM) [17]. HC were recruited via advertisement.
Inclusion criteria for all participants were (1) aged between 18 and 50 years old, (2) fluent Spanish, and (3) signed informed consent. In the case of patients, they should also present a diagnosis of severe mental illness (diagnosis of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorders, and bipolar I or II, according to DSM-IV criteria). Exclusion criteria were (1) mental retardation according to DSM-IV criteria; (2) history of head trauma with loss of consciousness; (3) organic disease with mental repercussions; and (4) significant physical or neurological condition that could affect neuropsychological performance, substance abuse, or dependence in the last 12 months, and electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) within the preceding year. The patients were matched with HC by age (± 10%), gender, and parental socioeconomic status (SES) (± 1 level) determined using Hollingshead’s Two-Factor Index of Social Position [18]. The exclusion criteria for controls were the same as for the patients, with some additional criteria such as the presence of a current or past psychotic disorder, major depression or other serious psychiatric illnesses, or having a first-degree relative with a history of psychotic or affective disorders.
Patients were divided into two groups according to their diagnostic category. Diagnoses of schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorders were categorized into “non-affective”, whereas bipolar disorder I and II were grouped as “affective”.
This study was conducted in accordance with the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and Good Clinical Practice and the Hospital Clinic Ethics and Research Board. The research project was approved by the Clinical Research Ethics Committee of the Hospital Clínic of Barcelona (Protocol number ID: HCB/2016/0579, Date: April 2016). All participants provided written informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study.

2.2. Assessments

2.2.1. Clinical and Sociodemographic Assessment

Clinical and sociodemographic data were systematically obtained for all participants. Diagnoses were determined with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM (SCID-I and II) [19,20] according to DSM-IV criteria.
Patients were assessed with a total of three clinical scales: the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) [21,22], the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) [23,24], and the Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) [25,26]. On each scale, the items were summed to obtain a total score. Higher scores indicate greater severity of the assessed symptomatology.

2.2.2. Functional Assessment

The functional outcome was assessed by means of the Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST) [27] and the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) [28]. The FAST is a scale used to evaluate six functional domains (autonomy, occupational functioning, cognitive functioning, financial issues, interpersonal relationships, and leisure time). Higher scores indicate worse functioning. The GAF is a scale used to assess the severity of symptoms and the level of functioning, on a numeric scale from 1 to 100. Higher scores indicate a better functioning.

2.2.3. Neuropsychological Assessment

The premorbid IQ was estimated using the Vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) [29]. To assess verbal memory, the Verbal Learning Test Spain Complutense for adults (TAVEC) [30] was used. Sustained attention was tested with the Continuous Performance Test–II (CPT-II) [31], version 5, corrected by age and educational level. To assess processing speed, Trail Making Test-part A (TMT-A) [32] and the Digit Symbol-Coding Subtest of the WAIS-III were used. Working memory was assessed by the Letter–Number Sequencing Subtest of the WAIS-III. Verbal fluency was tested with the animal fluency test [33]. Higher scores correspond to better performance in all cognitive domains except for attention.

2.2.4. Cognitive Reserve Assessment

To assess CR, the CRQ [14] and CRASH were used. In both scales, the higher the score, the greater the CR. The CRQ has been chosen over all the other listed instruments due to its short time of administration (2 min), which is more similar to CRASH (10 min) than other questionnaires; the fact that it is validated in the Spanish language; and the fact that it takes into account the educational level.
The CRASH was developed by the Schizophrenia and the Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Units at the Hospital Clinic de Barcelona in order to measure CR in SMI populations (see Appendix A). To generate potential items, an in-depth review of the scientific literature and existing measures [13,14,15,16] was initially carried out. Efforts were made to assess how to define more precisely the large blocks that make up the concept of CR (premorbid IQ, education/occupation, and leisure activities) and the constraints of each measure of CR available, taking into account that the reference population was people with a SMI. Once the first draft of CRASH was prepared, it was presented to twelve health professionals on mental health (eight psychologists and four psychiatrists) through discussion groups. They were asked to evaluate the items of the CRASH scale, including the suitability, clarity, and expression of the wording, and any items omitted that they considered relevant. The scale was modified and adapted to the different comments received (provided that at least 50% of the discussion group agreed). Finally, the discussion groups focused on the review of each of the measuring items. Items rated as essential, suitable, and clear by at least 80 percent of the health professionals were included; the other items were reformulated and re-rated, confirming the face validity.
The CRASH scale provides a global score and a score for each of the domains that form it (education, occupation, and intellectual and leisure activities). The scale’s maximum total score is 90, and it can be calculated using a formula, created with the intention that all domains have the same weighting in the final score. The score for each domain is obtained by adding the scores of the items it contains. For all scores, the higher the result, the better the level of CR.
Education: It measures the degree of schooling attained and passed by the subject and his parents. It also measures academic performance during childhood and adolescence and the number of languages in which the subject is able to maintain a conversation.
Occupation: It includes five different levels of working activities taking into account the occupation of the highest level (without work experience, unskilled and skilled work, professional occupation, and highly intellectual occupation) and five levels of training courses that must have been carried out in the last year to be scored.
Intellectual and leisure activities: The frequency and variety of four activities (physical, intellectual, artistic, and cultural) are counted over three life stages of the subject: childhood and adolescence, adulthood, and the previous year. To be considered an activity, it must be characteristic of the person’s life and it must have been performed within at least one year or academic year. Regarding frequency, a Likert-type scale of 0–3 points was used. In this section, sociability and withdrawal (type and quantity of relationships) are also evaluated.
A manual has been prepared including a semi-structured interview and a scoring guide that indicates how to score each item. Unlike a neurocognitive assessment, which should only be administered by a neuropsychologist, the CRASH can be carried out by professionals from different areas (nurses, psychologists, therapists, and medical doctors, among others), which facilitates its application. Furthermore, the time required to administer the test is approximately 10 min, which makes it highly feasible.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Data were analyzed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 18 (v for Windows, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Descriptive analyses were conducted using chi-square for categorical variables and Student’s test for continuous variables. Group differences were examined using unpaired t-tests for normally distributed variables, or using Mann–Whitney U tests for non-normal data. Significance level was set at p < 0.05.
The internal consistency reliability of the CRASH was assessed using Cronbach’s alpha. Concurrent validity for the CRASH and CRQ was assessed by examining Spearman’s correlation. Spearman’s correlation coefficient was also performed to examine the relationship between CRASH and CRQ, neuropsychological tests, psychosocial functioning, and clinical course.
Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was applied to each items of the scale. The Keiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) test for sampling adequacy and Bartlett’s test for sphericity were performed to ensure that the EFA was adequate for principal component factor solution.
To test the predictive capacity of CRASH and CRQ for each group, a logistic regression analysis was conducted. Additionally, receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves were analyzed to assess the ability of the CRASH and CRQ to distinguish subjects with a SMI from HC.
In order to help guide clinical interpretation, severity levels have been created based on diagnosis. The authors have divided the CRASH score into high and low in each group (healthy control, non-affective and affective patients). Subjects with a value above the median were considered to have a high CRASH score, and those with a score below median were assumed to have a low CRASH score [11,12,34], providing ease of interpretation that indicated where the score lay in comparison to the norm group.

3. Results

3.1. Sociodemographic Characteristics of the Sample

A total of 100 patients with a SMI and 66 HC were enrolled. A summary of the baseline sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of patients and HCs is shown in Table 1. Significant differences in functional outcomes (GAF and FAST) and neuropsychological outcomes (premorbid IQ, verbal memory, processing speed, and working memory) were found between patients and HC. There were no differences in terms of gender, age, SES, tobacco, cannabis use, and some cognitive performance variables such as sustained attention and verbal fluency.
When only analyzing the clinical group, there were significant differences in sociodemographic, clinical, functional, and neuropsychological variables among non-affective psychoses and affective disorders. The affective group was older, presented better clinical profile (positive, negative, general, and total PANSS) and performed better in different cognitive measures (premorbid IQ, verbal memory, and processing speed), except in sustained attention that performed worse (see Table 2).

3.2. Internal Consistency and Convergent Validity

Regarding inter-rater reliability of CRASH, there was agreement amongst 90% of health professionals, surpassing the two phases of reliability in all the tests. Thus, the inter-judge reliability study guarantees that the professionals carried out a correct application and correction of the tests.
The entirety of the CRASH score demonstrated adequate internal consistency (α = 0.903). The CRASH tool was significantly correlated with the CRQ in patients (rs = 0.838, p < 0.001) and in HC (rs = 0.816, p < 0.001) that persisted after controlling for potencial confounders such as premorbid IQ. When considering clinical groups amongst patients, this association was present for both non-affective psychotic disorder and affective disorders (rs = 0.827, p < 0.001, rs = 0.795, p < 0.001; respectively).

3.3. Factorial Analysis

The construct validity of CRASH was determined by conducting a factor analysis. The study of the internal structure of CRASH determined a four-factor structure, explaining 70.26% of total variance. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy (KMO) index was 0.728 (over 0.6), while Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity yielded an acceptable result (p < 0.001).
The principal components factor solution for CRASH is shown in Table 3. Factor I accounted for 36.23% of the variance and consisted of items related to “sociability”. Factor II accounted for 13.78% of the variance and consisted of “leisure activities”. Factor III accounted for 11.31% of the variance and consisted of “occupation”. Factor IV accounted for 8.95% of the variance and consisted of “education”.

3.4. CRASH Score Comparisons Among the Subject Samples and Severity Levels

Significant differences between the total CRASH score of patients and control groups have been found. The patient group obtained lower scores compared to the control group (39.82 ± 10.64 vs. 52.10 ± 9.40, p < 0.001). Between diagnostic groups, affective patients displayed higher CRASH scores than non-affective patients (44.78 ± 11.09 vs. 37.02 ± 9.25, p < 0.001). The affective patients showed lower CRASH scores than HC (p < 0.001). After performing a logistic regression to assess the predictive power of CRASH for each group (patients/controls), the model explained between 31.3% (Cox & Snell R Square) and 42.4% (Nagelkerke R Square) of the variance and correctly classified 78.9% of the cases (B = 0.131; p < 0.001; Exp(B) = 1.140). The predictive power of CRQ was also assessed and correctly classified 69.90% of the cases (B = 0.329; p < 0.001; Exp(B) = 1.389).
An ROC curve was generated to reveal the diagnostic accuracies of the CRASH and CRQ in discriminating patients with SMI from HC subjects. The analysis showed that CRASH, at a cutoff value of 47.58, had higher sensitivity (79%) and specificity (80.30%). The area under the curve was 0.842 (0.779–0.905) (see Figure 1). As far as the CRQ is concerned, at a cutoff value of 13.50, it had higher sensitivity (71.20%) and specificity (71%). The area under the curve was 0.773 (0.701–0.844).

3.5. Association Between CRASH and CRQ Scales and Clinical, Functionality, and Neuropsychological Test

Correlation analyses between CRASH and CRQ scales and clinical, functional, and neuropsychological outcomes are shown in Table 4. In the control group, the CRASH score was not associated with any measure. Nevertheless, the CRQ was related to some cognitive measures such as attention, processing speed, and premorbid IQ. In patients, the CRASH and CRQ total score were associated with clinical, functional, and almost all cognitive domains (except sustained attention). There were differences between non-affective psychotic disorders and affective disorders in these associations. In the non-affective psychosis group, the CRASH score was associated with premorbid IQ (rs = 0.387, p = 0.002), attention (rs = −0.302, p = 0.021), verbal fluency (rs = 0.504, p < 0.001), verbal memory (rs = 0.288, p = 0.026), processing speed (rs = 0.514, p = 0.014), GAF (rs = 0.292, p = 0.049), FAST (rs = −0.287, p = 0.048), and PANSS total score (rs = −0.318, p = 0.031). The CRQ was associated with age (rs = 0.294, p = 0.020) and three cognitive domains: verbal fluency (rs = 0.354, p = 0.006), verbal memory (rs = 0.217, p = 0.036), and premorbid IQ (rs = 0.500, p < 0.001). It was not associated with functionality or clinical outcomes in non-affective psychosis group. Instead, in patients with an affective disorder there were no associations between CRASH and neuropsychological, functional, and clinical outcomes, except for verbal memory (rs = 0.742, p < 0.001). The CRQ was also associated only with verbal memory (rs = 0.556, p < 0.001).
After dividing the CRASH into low or high level in each group (healthy control, non-affective, and affective patients), the differences on sociodemographic, clinical, functional, and neuropsychological variables were analyzed. In controls, there were no significant differences between those with higher and lower CRASH scores (above or below a score of 54). In the non-affective psychosis group, a significantly better performance was determined in patients with higher CRASH scores (above a score of 37) in all the cognitive domains evaluated, except for working memory, and lower negative and total symptoms assessed by PANSS scale. There were no differences in terms of age; gender; psychosocial functioning; and depressive, manic, or positive symptoms. In the affective disorder group, comparing those with high and low CRASH (above or below a score of 45), there were significant differences only in verbal memory performance (p < 0.001) (see Table 5).
An additional analysis was performed in order to clarify the necessity to divide the CRASH score in high and low above the median for each group, considering that subjects with a value above the cut-off point of 48 had a higher CRASH score. When comparing those with higher and lower CRASH scores, there were no significant differences in the non-affective psychosis group and in the affective disorder group there was only one significant difference for verbal memory performance (p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

This study confirmed the reliability and validity of the CRASH scale for patients with a SMI. The advantages of this new measure over existing measures include (a) the scale is adapted to the characteristics of this population, especially in the occupation domain, taking into account both training undertaken in the last year and the highest level occupation; (b) dividing it by periods of life facilitates the memory recall of the participants and provides information that, later, can be analyzed to know which period has been the most active in a person’s life and what kind of relationship it might have with other variables, such as cognitive impairment or psychosocial functioning; (c) it gives importance to modifiable factors such as intellectual and leisure activities; and (d) it is susceptible to being enhanced through psychosocial interventions.
Psychometric testing revealed excellent internal consistency and its statistically significant correlation with the CRQ confirms that the CRASH scale measures the intended construct of CR. The high association (0.8 and above) between CRASH and CRQ may be due to a high relationship between variables. However, as previously mentioned, it might be interesting to separately evaluate different activities and stages of life to explore the accumulation of CR and to know which period has been the most active in a person’s life, as well as which activities are most relevant. It could also be used to analyze what type of relationship CR might have with other variables, such as cognitive impairment. The principal components factor solution for CRASH extracted four factors: “education”, “occupation”, “leisure activities”, and “sociability”. These factors are foreseeable as the most common proposed CR indicator variables include educational level, occupational attainment, and leisure activities [4,8,9,10,12].
As expected, the CRASH score was higher for HC than for patients. This result is in accordance with previous studies that compared CR levels in patients and control subjects [4,8,10,12]. Amongst patients, the clinical group with an affective disorder showed a higher CRASH score compared to those with non-affective psychoses. This result is in line with our recently published study that explored the implications of CR in a FEP sample depending on whether the diagnosis was affective or non-affective FEP [12]. Indeed, the current findings suggest that the CRASH may have the ability to discriminate between patients with SMI and HC, with a high sensitivity (79%) and specificity (80.30%). The predictive power of CRASH is higher than CRQ (78.9% vs. 69.90%, respectively), indicating that the CRASH scale probably takes into account differential characteristics of the population with SMI.
The CR level was different between groups, and a serious limitation of norm-reference tests is that the reference group may not represent the population of interest. For this reason and in order to help guide clinical interpretation, the authors have divided the CRASH score into high and low in each group (HC, non-affective, and affective patients). The cut-off point value clearly differentiated patients from HC. However, it did not serve to differentiate non-affective patients with worse cognitive performance from those with higher cognitive performances.
In HC, the CRASH score was not associated with any measure and there were no differences between those with a higher CRASH score and those with a lower CRASH score. This is consistent with other studies that have shown that CR, after controlling for tobacco use, cannot predict cognitive performance nor functional outcomes [12]. As mentioned, one possible explanation of these results could be the CR concept itself [1,12]. HC are free from any pathology that might affect their cognitive performance or functionality, and CR refers to the brain’s capacity to face a pathology using alternative, or more efficient, cerebral networks in order to minimize symptoms [1]. Hence, we might not be able to detect the potential protective effects of CR in HC. Another possibility, taking into account that CRQ is associated with attention, processing speed and premorbid IQ, could be that the CRASH scale may be helpful in discriminating patients from healthy population. CRASH may have the ability to discriminate between patients with SMI and HC with higher sensitivity and specificity than CRQ. The predictive power of CRASH is also higher than CRQ, indicating that the CRASH scale probably takes into account differential characteristics of the population with SMI, especially for non-affective patients. This is advantageous for the evaluation of people with SMI, but it may not differentiate HC with greater cognitive abilities. Finally, it could be because, as observed by Stern, there are at least two types of reserve [2] (the use of brain networks or cognitive paradigms that are less susceptible to disruption or the use of brain structures or networks not normally used by individuals with intact brains in order to compensate for brain damage), and these may need to be measured differently.
In patient groups, as expected, associations between higher CRASH and CRQ scores and better clinical, functional, and cognitive performance could be confirmed. However, there were differences in the associations according to the diagnosis. CRQ was associated with better clinical, psychosocial, and cognitive functioning in patients; however, it was not associated with functionality or clinical outcomes in non-affective psychosis group. As far as the CRASH is concerned, it was associated with functional, clinical, and neuropsychological outcomes in non-affective psychotic disorders. The results suggested that people with a lower CRASH score had greater cognitive impairment and worse clinical and psychosocial functioning. Specifically, comparing those with higher and lower CRASH scores, a significantly better performance was determined in patients with higher CR on different cognitive measures (premorbid IQ, verbal memory, sustained attention, processing speed, and verbal fluency) and lower symptom severity, especially the negative ones. These results are in line with previous studies [4,10,12,35,36,37] indicating that non-affective patients with lower CRASH scores could benefit from a cognitive rehabilitation program [38], oriented to improve performance in cognitive domains. This recommendation was already mentioned in our study of first psychotic episodes [12], specifically in those with a non-affective psychosis. In affective disorders, having a lower or higher level of CRASH was only associated with verbal memory performance. Some studies have shown that CR was significantly predictive of functioning and different cognitive domains [8,9,11], while others have reported that capacity only for verbal memory [12]. In fact, we have found a strong correlation between verbal memory and CRASH in affective patients, probably indicating the important role of this cognitive domain in the CR and as a ‘core’ deficit of bipolar disorder, even in the early stages of the illness [39,40,41,42]. There are aspects to keep in mind that could explain these differences such as the variables used to estimate the CR or the sample size. As recommended in previous reports from our group [12], for patients with affective disorders and lower CRASH scores a functional remediation therapy [43] can be suggested, since it is the intervention that has been shown to be the most effective in improving psychosocial functioning and verbal memory, the only altered domain found in our study [44,45]. Regardless of diagnosis, we consider that although there are important premorbid variables in the CR concept that are difficult to modify (such as premorbid IQ or education), interventions centered on CR stimulation and engaging lifestyle (such as physical, social, or leisure activities) could be beneficial to preventing or reducing the impact of illness [12,46,47], especially those conducted in the early stages of the illness, or even in people with a high risk of suffering psychosis.
Thus, with these preliminary results, authors believe that the CRASH scale may aid in the classification of patients while helping in the implementation of personalized interventions focused on improving core symptoms of SMI [46,47,48]. Is expected that it will allow for the homogeneous assessment of the concept of CR in the psychiatric population, and that this will be translated into more consistent, uniform, and replicable data. Further research should be conducted to use the CRASH scale as a stratification tool and a potential sensitive measure of change. There are already neuropsychological and functional interventions aimed at improving cognitive and psychosocial functioning, but until now the impact of intervening on the different areas of CR such as social, mental, physical activities, and hobbies in people with SMI has not been explored.
Some limitations of our work have to be taken into consideration before translating these findings into clinical practice. Firstly, the difference between the clinical sample size of non-affective and affective psychotic disorder groups (62 vs. 38). The small sample size may have interfered with the results (low statistical power), and further larger studies are required to confirm these findings. Secondly, executive function has not been assessed with specific tests, and we have used verbal fluency as a variable that expresses executive processes. The final limitation would be the cross-sectional study designs, which does not allow one to measure stability. However, it is not expected to change without an intervention. Despite these limitations, our study has significant strengths. We have developed a new instrument that measures CR level in people with SMI and can improve our understanding of individual differences in the course of SMI helping the implementation of personalized interventions.

5. Conclusions

In summary, CRASH showed strong internal consistency. It is the first measure developed specifically for patients with severe mental illness with optimal psychometric properties, facilitating reliable and valid measurement of cognitive reserve. The scale may aid in the stratification of patients and the implementation of personalized interventions.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and methodology, S.A., B.C., C.T., C.d.M.B., G.M., M.G., E.J., A.M.-A., B.S., M.R., C.V., R.P., I.G., E.S., M.B. (Miquel Bioque), C.G.-R., and G.A.; formal analysis, S.A., C.d.M.B., and A.R.R.; investigation, S.A., B.C., C.T., C.d.M.B., G.M., M.G., E.J., A.M.-A., B.S., M.R., and C.V.; writing—original draft preparation S.A., B.C., and C.T.; writing—review and editing, I.G., E.S., A.M., E.P., A.R.R., F.C., G.S., E.V., and M.B. (Miquel Bernardo); supervision, E.V. and M.B. (Miquel Bernardo). All authors reviewed, wrote, and approved the final version.

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all participants. Una manera de hacer Europa, Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de salud Mental, CIBERSAM, by the CERCA Programme/Generalitat de Catalunya and Secretaria d’Universitats i Recerca del Departament d’Economia I Coneixement (2014SGR441). Departament de Salut de la Generalitat de Catalunya, en la convocatoria corresponent a l’any 2017 de concessió de subvencions del Pla Estratègic de Recerca i Innovació en Salut (PERIS) 2016–2020, modalitat Projectes de recerca orientats a l’atenció primària, amb el codi d’expedient SLT006/17/00345. Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (PI08/0208; PI11/00325; PI14/00612), Instituto de Salud Carlos III—Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional. Unión Europea. C.T. is funded by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, Instituto Carlos III, through a ‘Miguel Servet’ postdoctoral contract (CPI14/00175) with a FIS (PI 17/01066) and a PERIS from the Generalitat de Catalunya (SLT006/17/352). I.G. thanks the suport of the Spanish Ministry of Economy, Industry, and Competitiveness (PI16/00187), integrated into the Plan Nacional de I+D+I y cofinanciado por el ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación y el Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER). A.M.A. thanks the support of the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (PI15/00330), integrated into the Plan Nacional de I+D+I y cofinanciado por el ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación y el Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER). E.V. thanks the support of the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (PI15/00283), integrated into the Plan Nacional de I+D+I y cofinanciado por el ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación y el Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER); CIBERSAM; and the Comissionat per a Universitats i Recerca del DIUE de la Generalitat de Catalunya to the Bipolar Disorders Group (2017 SGR 1365) and the project SLT006/17/00357, from PERIS 2016-2020 (Departament de Salut). CERCA Programme/Generalitat de Catalunya.

Conflicts of Interest

A.M.A. has received funding for research projects and/or honoraria as a consultant or speaker for the following companies and institutions: Otsuka, Pfizer, AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Siquibb, Lundbeck, Brain and Behaviour Foundation (NARSAD Independent Investigator), the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, and Instituto de Salud Carlos III. I.G. has received grants and served as consultant, advisor or CME speaker for the following identities: AstraZeneca, Ferrer, Janssen Cilag, and Lundbeck, Lundbeck-Otsuka, Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, and Instituto de Salud Carlos III. M.B. (Miquel Bioque) has received honoraria from talks and consultancy of Adamed, has received honoraria from consultancy of Ferrer, has received research support and honoraria from talks and consultancy of Janssen-Cilag, has received honoraria from talks and consultancy of Lundbeck, has received honoraria from talks and consultancy of Otsuka, and has received a research prize from Pfizer. C.G.R. has received honoraria/travel support from Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck, and Ferrer. E.F.-E. has received unrestricted research funding from Genus Pharmaceuticals and consultancy fees from Roche/Genentech. E.V. has received research support from or served as a consultant, adviser, or speaker for AB-Biotics, Actavis, Allergan, Angelini, AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma, Ferrer, Forest Research Institute, Gedeon Richter, Glaxo-Smith-Kline, Janssen, Lundbeck, Otsuka, Pfizer, Roche, Sanofi-Aventis, Servier, Shire, Sunovion, Takeda, Telefónica, the Brain and Behaviour Foundation, the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (CIBERSAM), the Seventh European Framework Programme (ENBREC), and the Stanley Medical Research Institute. M.B. (Miquel Bernardo) has been a consultant for, received grant/research support and honoraria from, and been on the speakers/advisory board of ABBiotics, Adamed, AMGEN, Eli Lilly, Ferrer, Forum Pharmaceuticals, Gedeon, Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck, Otsuka, Pfizer, and Roche. The rest of authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Appendix A

Jcm 08 00586 i001

References

  1. Stern, Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2002, 8, 448–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2006, 20, S69–S74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Barnett, J.; Salmond, C.; Jones, P.; Sahakian, B. Cognitive reserve in neuropsychiatry. Psychol. Med. 2006, 36, 1053–1064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. De la Serna, E.; Andrés-Perpiñá, S.; Puig, O.; Baeza, I.; Bombin, I.; Bartrés-Faz, D.; Arango, C.; Gonzalez-Pinto, A.; Parellada, M.; Mayoral, M.; et al. Cognitive reserve as a predictor of two year neuropsychological performance in early onset first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2013, 143, 125–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kontis, D.; Huddy, V.; Reeder, C.; Landau, S.; Wykes, T. Effects of age and cognitive reserve on cognitive remediation therapy outcome in patients with schizophrenia. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2013, 21, 218–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Leeson, V.; Sharma, P.; Harrison, M.; Ron, M.; Barnes, T.; Joyce, E. IQ trajectory, cognitive reserve, and clinical outcome following a first episode of psychosis: A 3-year longitudinal study. Schizophr. Bull. 2011, 37, 768–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Leeson, V.; Harrison, I.; Ron, M.; Barnes, T.; Joyce, E. The effect of cannabis use and cognitive reserve on age at onset and psychosis outcomes in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2012, 38, 873–880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Forcada, I.; Mur, M.; Mora, E.; Vieta, E.; Bartres-Faz, D.; Portella, M. The influence of cognitive reserve on psychosocial and neuropsychological functioning in bipolar disorder. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015, 25, 214–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Anaya, C.; Torrent, C.; Caballero, F.F.; Vieta, E.; Bonnin, C.M.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L.; CIBERSAM Functional Remediation Group. Cognitive reserve in bipolar disorder: Relation to cognition, psychosocial functioning and quality of life. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2016, 133, 386–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Amoretti, S.; Bernardo, M.; Bonnin, C.M.; Bioque, M.; Cabrera, B.; Mezquida, G.; Solé, B.; Vieta, E.; Torrent, C. The impact of cognitive reserve in the outcome of first-episode psychoses: 2-year follow-up study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016, 26, 1638–1648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Grande, I.; Sanchez-Moreno, J.; Sole, B.; Jimenez, E.; Torrent, C.; Bonnin, C.M.; Varo, C.; Tabares-Seisdedos, R.; Balanzá-Martínez, V.; Valls, E.; et al. High cognitive reserve in bipolar disorders as a moderator of neurocognitive impairment. J. Affect. Disord. 2017, 208, 621–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Amoretti, S.; Cabrera, B.; Torrent, C.; Mezquida, G.; Lobo, A.; González-Pinto, A.; Parellada, M.; Corripio, I.; Vieta, E.; de la Serna, E.; et al. Cognitive reserve as an outcome predictor: First-episode affective versus non-affective psychosis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2018, 138, 441–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Valenzuela, M.J.; Sachdev, P. Assessment of complex mental activity across the lifespan: Development of the Lifetime of Experiences Questionnaire (LEQ). Psychol. Med. 2007, 37, 1015–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Rami, L.; Valls-Pedret, C.; Bartrés-Faz, D.; Caprile, C.; Solé-Padullés, C.; Castellvi, M.; Olives, J.; Bosch, B.; Molinuevo, J.L. Cognitive reserve questionnaire. Scores obtained in a healthy elderly population and in one with Alzheimer’s disease. Rev. Neurol. 2011, 52, 195–201. [Google Scholar]
  15. Nucci, M.; Mapelli, D.; Mondini, S. Cognitive Reserve Index questionnaire (CRIq): A new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 2012, 24, 218–226. [Google Scholar]
  16. León, I.; García-García, J.; Roldán-Tapia, L. Estimating cognitive reserve in healthy adults using the Cognitive Reserve Scale. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e102632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Salagre, E.; Arango, C.; Artigas, F.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L.; Bernardo, M.; Castro-Fornieles, J.; Bobes, J.; Desco, M.; Fañanás, L.; González-Pinto, A.; et al. CIBERSAM: Ten years of collaborative translational research in mental disorders. Rev. Psiquiatr. Salud Ment. 2019, 12, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hollingshead, A.; Redlich, F. Social Class and Mental Illness: Community Study; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1958. [Google Scholar]
  19. First, M.; Gibbon, M.; Spitzer, R.; Williams, J.; Benjamin, L. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II); American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  20. First, M.; Spitzer, R.; Gibbon, M.; Williams, J. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders-Clinician (SCID-I); American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  21. Kay, S.; Fiszbein, A.; Opler, L. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1987, 13, 261–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Peralta, V.; Cuesta, M. Psychometric properties of the positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1994, 53, 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Colom, F.; Vieta, E.; Martínez-Arán, A.; Garcia-Garcia, M.; Reinares, M.; Torrent, C.; Goikolea, J.; Banús, S.; Salamero, M. Spanish version of a scale for the assessment of mania: Validity and reliability of the Young Mania Rating Scale. Med. Clin. (Barc.) 2002, 119, 366–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Young, R.; Biggs, J.; Ziegler, V.; Da, M. A rating scale for mania: Reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br. J. Psychiatry 1978, 133, 429–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Lobo, A.; Chamorro, L.; Luque, A.; Dal-Re, R.; Badia, X.; Baro, E. Validation of the Spanish versions of the Montgomery-Asberg depression and Hamilton anxiety rating scales. Med. Clin. (Barc.) 2002, 118, 493–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Montgomery, S.; Asberg, M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br. J. Psychiatry 1979, 134, 382–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Rosa, A.; Sánchez-Moreno, J.; Martínez-Aran, A.; Salamero, M.; Torrent, C.; Reinares, M.; Comes, M.; Colom, F.; Van Riel, W.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.; et al. Validity and reliability of the Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST) in bipolar disorder. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health 2007, 7, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  29. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III); Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  30. Benedet, M.J.; Alejandre, M.A. Test de Aprendizaje Verbal España-Complutense (TAVEC); TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  31. Conners, C.K. Continuous Performance Test II; Multi-Health Systems: North Tonawanda, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  32. Reitan, R.M. Validity of the Trail Making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 1958, 8, 271–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Benton, A.L.; Hamsher, K.S. Multilingual Aphasia Examination; AJA Associates; University of Iowa: Iowa City, IA, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
  34. Pereda, M.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L.; Gómez Del Barrio, A.; Echevarria, S.; Farinas, M.C.; García Palomo, D.; González Macias, J.; Vázquez-Barquero, J.L. Factors associated with neuropsychological performance in HIV-seropositive subjects without AIDS. Psychol. Med. 2000, 30, 205–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kelley, M.; Gilbertson, M.; Mouton, A.; Van Kammen, D. Deterioration in premorbid functioning in schizophrenia: A developmental model of negative symptoms in drug-free patients. Am. J. Psychiatry 1992, 149, 1543–1548. [Google Scholar]
  36. Rabinowitz, J.; De Smedt, G.; Harvey, P.; Davidson, M. Relationship between premorbid functioning and symptom severity as assessed at first episode of psychosis. Am. J. Psychiatry 2002, 159, 2021–2026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Mezquida, G.; Cabrera, B.; Bioque, M.; Amoretti, S.; Lobo, A.; González-Pinto, A.; Espliego, A.; Corripio, I.; Vieta, E.; Castro-Fornieles, J.; et al. The course of negative symptoms in first-episode schizophrenia and its predictors: A prospective two-year follow-up study. Schizophr. Res. 2017, 189, 84–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Penadés, R.; Catalán, R.; Pujol, N.; Masana, G.; García-Rizo, C.; Bernardo, M. The integration of cognitive remediation therapy into the whole psychosocial rehabilitation process: An evidence-based and person-centered approach. Rehabil. Res. Pract. 2012, 2012, 386895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Bourne, C.; Aydemir, Ö.; Balanzá-Martínez, V.; Bora, E.; Brissos, S.; Cavanagh, J.T.O.; Clark, L.; Cubukcuoglu, Z.; Dias, V.V.; Dittmann, S.; Ferrier, I.N.; et al. Neuropsychological testing of cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: An individual patient data meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2013, 128, 149–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Arts, B.; Jabben, N.; Krabbendam, L.; van, O.J. Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychol. Med. 2008, 38, 771–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Martinez-Aran, A.; Vieta, E.; Torrent, C.; Sanchez-Moreno, J.; Goikolea, J.M.; Salamero, M.; Malhi, G.S.; Gonzalez-Pinto, A.; Daban, C.; varez-Grandi, S.; et al. Functional outcome in bipolar disorder: The role of clinical and cognitive factors. Bipolar Disord. 2007, 9, 103–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Bonnín, C.M.; González-Pinto, A.; Solé, B.; Reinares, M.; González-Ortega, I.; Alberich, S.; Crespo, J.M.; Salamero, M.; Vieta, E.; Martínez-Arán, A.; et al. Verbal memory as a mediator in the relationship between subthreshold depressive symptoms and functional outcome in bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2014, 160, 50–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Vieta, E.; Torrent, C. Functional remediation: The pathway from remission to recovery in bipolar disorder. World Psychiatry 2016, 15, 288–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bonnin, C.M.; Reinares, M.; Martínez-Arán, A.; Balanzá-Martínez, V.; Sole, B.; Torrent, C.; Tabarés-Seisdedos, R.; García-Portilla, M.P.; Ibáñez, A.; Amann, B.L.; et al. Effects of functional remediation on neurocognitively impaired bipolar patients: Enhancement of verbal memory. Psychol. Med. 2016, 46, 291–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Bonnin, C.M.; Torrent, C.; Arango, C.; Amann, B.L.; Solé, B.; González-Pinto, A.; Crespo, J.M.; Tabarés-Seisdedos, R.; Reinares, M.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L.; et al. Functional remediation in bipolar disorder: 1-year follow-up of neurocognitive and functional outcome. Br. J. Psychiatry 2016, 208, 87–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Vieta, E. Staging and early intervention in bipolar disorder. Lancet Psychiatry 2015, 2, 483–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Solé, B.; Jiménez, E.; Torrent, C.; Reinares, M.; Bonnin, C.M.; Torres, I.; Varo, C.; Grande, I.; Valls, E.; Salagre, E.; et al. Cognitive Impairment in Bipolar Disorder: Treatment and Prevention Strategies. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017, 20, 70–680. [Google Scholar]
  48. Salagre, E.; Dodd, S.; Aedo, A.; Rosa, A.; Amoretti, S.; Pinzon, J.; Reinares, M.; Berk, M.; Kapczinski, F.P.; Vieta, E.; et al. Toward Precision Psychiatry in Bipolar Disorder: Staging 2.0. Front. Psychiatry 2018, 9, 641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Curve between patients and controls (CRASH and cognitive reserve questionnaire CRQ). 1 ROC = receiver operating characteristic, 2 CRASH = Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health, 3 CRQ = Cognitive Reserve Questionnaire.
Figure 1. Curve between patients and controls (CRASH and cognitive reserve questionnaire CRQ). 1 ROC = receiver operating characteristic, 2 CRASH = Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health, 3 CRQ = Cognitive Reserve Questionnaire.
Jcm 08 00586 g001
Table 1. Sociodemographic, functional, clinical, and neuropsychological variables for patients with severe mental illness and healthy controls.
Table 1. Sociodemographic, functional, clinical, and neuropsychological variables for patients with severe mental illness and healthy controls.
Patients (n = 100)Healthy Controls (n = 66)Statisticp-Value
Gender: Male N (%)65 (65)35 (55)χ2 = 2.3780.084
Age ( X ¯ ± SD)32.68 ± 8.5930.12 ± 8.15t = 1.7200.087
SES 1 (%) χ2 = 5.9670.309
High30 (30)
Medium-High23 (23)
Medium19 (19)
Medium-Low19 (19)
Low7 (7)
Missing value2 (2)
Tobacco use: Yes N (%)39 (39)19 (29)χ2 = 2.5420.076
Cannabis use: Yes N (%)11 (11)10 (15)χ2 =0.3720.352
Functional variables ( X ¯ ± SD)
GAF 2 score74.77 ± 11.9490.79 ± 6.01U = 228.50<0.001
FAST 318.64 ± 13.222.66 ± 6.24U = 392.00<0.001
Clinical variables ( X ¯ ± SD)
PANSS 4 positive10.54 ± 4.25NA 8
PANSS negative15.90± 6.20NA
PANSS general26.82 ± 8.29NA
PANSS total53.25 ± 16.89NA
YMRS 5 score0.84 ± 1.81NA
MADRS 6 score3.90 ± 4.82NA
Neuropsychological performance ( X ¯ ± SD)
Premorbid IQ 7101.87 ± 11.85108.56 ± 9.48t = −3.771<0.001
Verbal memory41.88 ± 11.4555.25 ± 8.48U = 1217.50<0.001
Sustained attention47.22 ± 13.2645.91 ± 10.87t = 0.6210.535
Processing speed48.51 ± 11.7861.73 ± 6.27U = 134.50<0.001
Working memory47.15 ± 11.9651.98 ± 9.03U = 2188.000.011
Verbal fluency47.60 ± 15.8344.29 ± 17.28t = −1.2680.209
1 SES = Socioeconomic status, 2 GAF = Global Assessment of Functioning, 3 FAST = Functioning Assessment Short Test, 4 PANSS = Positive and Negative Symptom Scale, 5 YMRS = Young Mania Rating Scale, 6 MADRS = Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale, 7 IQ = Intelligence Quotient, 8 NA = Not applicable. Significant differences (p < 0.05) marked in bold.
Table 2. Baseline sociodemographic, functional, clinical, and neuropsychological performance for patients (affective vs. non-affective).
Table 2. Baseline sociodemographic, functional, clinical, and neuropsychological performance for patients (affective vs. non-affective).
Patients (n = 100)Statisticp-Value
Non-Affective (n = 62)Affective (n = 38)
Gender: Male N (%)40 (64.52)25 (65.79)χ2 = 0.0090.551
Age ( X ¯ ± SD)30.82 ± 7.7435.71 ± 9.13t = −2.5780.011
SES 1 N (%) χ2 = 7.7060.173
High21 (34)9 (24)
Medium-High13 (21)10 (26)
Medium11 (18)8 (21)
Medium-Low11 (18)8 (21)
Low6 (9)1 (3)
Missing value0 (0)2 (5)
Age of onset25.67 ± 625.00 ± 7t = 0.4890.626
Tobacco use: Yes N (%)22 (35)16 (42)χ2 = 0.2010.407
Cannabis use: Yes N (%)7 (11)4 (11)χ2 = 0.0770.526
Functional variables ( X ¯ ± SD)
GAF 2 score74.04 ± 11.6678.57 ± 7.45t = −1.3660.177
FAST 319.19 ± 14.3416.61 ± 11.41t =0.8380.405
Clinical variables ( X ¯ ± SD)
PANSS 4 positive11.43 ± 4.487.86 ± 1.61U = 127.000.001
PANSS negative17.24 ± 6.0711.07 ± 3.87U = 132.000.001
PANSS general28.11 ± 8.5822.71 ± 6.21t = 2.1800.033
PANSS total56.76 ± 17.1341.64 ± 10.60U = 147.500.002
YMRS 5 score0.73 ± 2.201.26 ± 1.56t = −1.1500.253
MADRS 6 score5.17 ± 6.153.68 ± 3.00U = 779.000.899
Neuropsychological performance ( X ¯ ± SD)
Premorbid IQ 798.73 ±11.63107.16 ± 10.35t = -3.5150.001
Verbal memory38.42 ± 10.1647.06 ± 12.40U = 696.500.002
Sustained attention44.77 ± 12.2751.10 ± 14.07t = −2.1510.034
Processing speed39.85 ± 9.5454.94 ± 8.84t = −5.996<0.001
Working memory45.55 ± 12.3249.78 ± 11.01t = −1.7130.090
Verbal fluency45.55 ± 16.8846.71 ± 14.06t = −0.2270.821
1 SES=Socioeconomic status, 2 GAF= Global Assessment of Functioning, 3 FAST=Functioning Assessment Short Test, 4 PANSS= Positive and Negative Symptom Scale, 5 YMRS= Young Mania Rating Scale, 6 MADRS= Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale, 7 IQ= Intelligence Quotient. Significant differences (p < 0.05) marked in bold.
Table 3. Principal Components Factor Solution for Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health (CRASH).
Table 3. Principal Components Factor Solution for Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health (CRASH).
Factor IFactor IIFactor IIIFactor IV
SociabilityLDG 1Leisure ActivitiesLDGOccupationLDGEducationLDG
10. Social activity in childhood and adolescence0.8677. Leisure activities in childhood and adolescence0.8025. Training0.7621. Education0.726
11. Social activity in adulthood0.9088. Leisure activities in adulthood0.9176. Employment0.8902. Educational attainment of parents0.697
12. Social activity in the past year0.9239. Leisure activities in the past year0.874 3. Scholastic performance0.512
4. Languages0.558
1 LDG = Loading.
Table 4. Spearman’s correlation analyses of CRASH and CRQ with functional, clinical, and cognitive outcomes.
Table 4. Spearman’s correlation analyses of CRASH and CRQ with functional, clinical, and cognitive outcomes.
PatientsHealthy Controls
Spearman’s Correlation CRASH 1p-ValueSpearman’s Correlation CRQ 2p-ValueSpearman’s Correlation CRASHp-ValueSpearman’s Correlation CRQp-Value
Functional variables ( X ¯ ± SD)
GAF 30.2750.0330.3090.016−0.0770.628−0.1000.53
FAST 4−0.2980.007−0.3760.001−0.0260.838−0.0860.51
Clinical variables ( X ¯ ± SD)
PANSS 5 positive−0.3330.002−0.455<0.001NA
PANSS negative−0.2770.032−0.3330.009
PANSS general−0.2120.10−0.2740.034
PANSS total−0.2800.030−0.3630.004
YMRS 6 score− 0.0700.376−0.0580.61
MADRS 7 score−0.0470.369−0.1500.18
Neuropsychological performance ( X ¯ ± SD)
Premorbid IQ 80.405<0.0010.427<0.0010.2080.1050.2670.036
Verbal memory0.533<0.0010.443<0.0010.2220.0880.0770.56
Sustained attention−0.1170.27−0.0210.84−0.0890.515−0.3400.010
Processing speed0.3970.0030.3590.0100.4250.0620.4770.033
Working memory0.2050.0450.1990.050.2270.0810.2500.054
Verbal Fluency0.3250.0020.2500.016−0.3470.1140.0230.87
1 CRASH=Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health, 2 CRQ= Cognitive Reserve Questionnaire, 3 GAF= Global Assessment of Functioning, 4 FAST=Functioning Assessment Short Test, 5 PANSS= Positive and Negative Symptom Scale, 6 YMRS= Young Mania Rating Scale, 7 MADRS= Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale, 8 IQ= Intelligence Quotient. Significant differences (p < 0.05) marked in bold.
Table 5. Sociodemographic, clinical, functional, and cognitive performance among subjects with high and low CRASH scores.
Table 5. Sociodemographic, clinical, functional, and cognitive performance among subjects with high and low CRASH scores.
Non-Affective (n = 62)Affective (n = 38)
Low CRASH (<37)High CRASH (≥37)pLow CRASH (<45)High CRASH (≥45)p
Sociodemographic variables
Gender: Male N (%)20 (65)20 (65)0.6414 (67)11 (65)0.74
Age ( X ¯ ± SD)29.77 ± 8.0731.87 ± 7.380.2935.76 ± 9.6135.65 ± 8.790.97
SES 1 N (%) 0.43 0.19
High9 (28)12 (40) 4 (27)5 (24)
Medium-High5 (17)8 (26)4 (27)6 (29)
Medium6 (17)6 (19)3 (20)5 (24)
Medium-Low8 (24)3 (11)3 (20)6 (24)
Low5 (15)1 (4)1 (7)0 (0)
Missing value0 (0)0 (0)1 (7)1 (5)
Age of onset25.17 ± 5.8826.11 ± 5.310.5523.95 ± 6.0725.93 ± 8.850.43
Functional variables ( X ¯ ± SD)
GAF 270.55 ± 11.0476.73 ± 11.610.0679.50 ± 776.25 ± 80.48
FAST 323.41 ± 14.3415.62 ± 13.600.0718.55 ± 1213.50 ± 100.23
Clinical variables ( X ¯ ± SD)
PANSS 4 positive12.71 ± 5.4710.36 ± 3.160.078.11 ± 1.977.40 ± 0.550.45
PANSS negative19.38 ± 6.0715.44 ± 5.580.02711.56 ± 4.2510.26 ± 3.350.55
PANSS general30.33 ± 9.4426.24 ± 7.460.1023.22 ± 6.4421.80 ± 6.380.69
PANSS total62.43 ± 18.6352 ± 14.470.03842.89 ± 11.2539.40 ± 10.110.58
YMRS 51.04 ± 3.000.46 ± 1.170.371.52 ± 1.670.85 ± 1.350.23
MADRS 65.32 ± 5.785.04 ± 6.560.883.76 ± 2.953.54 ± 3.210.83
Neuropsychological performance ( X ¯ ± SD)
Premorbid IQ 793.61 ± 11103.84 ± 10<0.001105.71 ± 11.10108.94 ± 9.370.35
Verbal memory34.07 ± 8.4342.50 ± 10.06<0.00137.90 ± 11.1853.26 ± 7.82<0.001
Sustained attention48.68 ± 11.7541.12 ± 11.780.01851.80 ± 14.8650.26 ± 13.500.75
Processing speed34.50 ± 9.3543.29 ± 8.230.02754.03 ± 7.4855.67 ± 9.990.61
Working memory43.24 ± 11.8247.86 ± 12.570.1448.56 ± 11.4551.31 ± 10.590.46
Verbal fluency39.76 ± 16.9151.35 ± 14.990.00845.55 ± 16.7748.37 ± 9.250.57
1 SES=Socioeconomic status, 2 GAF= Global Assessment of Functioning, 3 FAST=Functioning Assessment Short Test, 4 PANSS= Positive and Negative Symptom Scale, 5 YMRS= Young Mania Rating Scale, 6 MADRS= Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale, 7 IQ= Intelligence Quotient. Significant differences (p < 0.05) marked in bold.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Amoretti, S.; Cabrera, B.; Torrent, C.; Bonnín, C.d.M.; Mezquida, G.; Garriga, M.; Jiménez, E.; Martínez-Arán, A.; Solé, B.; Reinares, M.; et al. Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health (CRASH): Its Validity and Reliability. J. Clin. Med. 2019, 8, 586. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8050586

AMA Style

Amoretti S, Cabrera B, Torrent C, Bonnín CdM, Mezquida G, Garriga M, Jiménez E, Martínez-Arán A, Solé B, Reinares M, et al. Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health (CRASH): Its Validity and Reliability. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2019; 8(5):586. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8050586

Chicago/Turabian Style

Amoretti, Silvia, Bibiana Cabrera, Carla Torrent, Caterina del Mar Bonnín, Gisela Mezquida, Marina Garriga, Esther Jiménez, Anabel Martínez-Arán, Brisa Solé, Maria Reinares, and et al. 2019. "Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health (CRASH): Its Validity and Reliability" Journal of Clinical Medicine 8, no. 5: 586. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8050586

APA Style

Amoretti, S., Cabrera, B., Torrent, C., Bonnín, C. d. M., Mezquida, G., Garriga, M., Jiménez, E., Martínez-Arán, A., Solé, B., Reinares, M., Varo, C., Penadés, R., Grande, I., Salagre, E., Parellada, E., Bioque, M., Garcia-Rizo, C., Meseguer, A., Anmella, G., ... Bernardo, M. (2019). Cognitive Reserve Assessment Scale in Health (CRASH): Its Validity and Reliability. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 8(5), 586. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8050586

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop