Next Article in Journal
The Estimation of the Long-Term Agricultural Output with a Robust Machine Learning Prediction Model
Next Article in Special Issue
Isolation and Some Basic Characteristics of Lactic Acid Bacteria from Honeybee (Apis mellifera L.) Environment—A Preliminary Study
Previous Article in Journal
Spatial-Temporal Variability of Soil Organic Carbon Density and Its Related Factors in Fengqiu County of Yellow River Basin, China: A Model and GIS Technique Approach
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Honey Bee Foraging Decisions Influenced by Pear Volatiles

1
College of Animal Science, Shanxi Agricultural University, Jinzhong 030801, China
2
College of Horticulture, Shanxi Agricultural University, Taiyuan 030031, China
3
Key Laboratory for Insect-Pollinator Biology of the Ministry of Agriculture and Rural Affairs, Institute of Apicultural Research, Chinese Academy of Agricultural Sciences, Beijing 100093, China
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Agriculture 2022, 12(8), 1074; https://doi.org/10.3390/agriculture12081074
Submission received: 11 June 2022 / Revised: 11 July 2022 / Accepted: 20 July 2022 / Published: 22 July 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Pollinators in Agricultural Ecosystems)

Abstract

:
The interactions between plants and pollinators are complex. Flower volatiles as special olfactory cues could influence the foraging choices of pollinators. Here, we conducted bioassays to evaluate the role of flower volatiles on the attraction of honey bees (native Apis cerana and exotic Apis mellifera) to pears (native Pyrus bretschneideri and exotic Pyrus communis). Chemical and electrophysiological approaches were used to determine flower volatiles and evaluate the antennal responses of honey bees to volatiles from pear flowers. Bioassays demonstrated that flower volatiles were crucial for the attraction of honey bees to pear flowers; honey bees preferred to forage on P. communis flowers (p > 0.05), with approximately 64.37 ± 0.02% (A. mellifera) and 62.10 ± 0.02% (A. cerana) foraging on P. communis. Flowers of P. communis and P. bretschneideri yielded 27 and 31 compounds, respectively, with 17 of them being common. Honey bee antennae responded to 16 chemicals, including 5 contained in both pear species: 1-nonanol, linalool, methyl 2-hydroxy-3-methylpentanoate, methyl L-isoleucinate, and α-farnesene. In addition, there were 8 electrophysiologically active compounds in P. bretschneideri: methyl L-valine ester, benzaldehyde, 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one, isophorone, 2-methyl octane, longicyclene, longifolene, and caryophyllene; and 3 electrophysiologically active compounds in P. communis: β-ocimene, 4-oxoisophorone and lilac alcohol D. In conclusion, our study demonstrated the significant impact of pear flower volatiles on honey bee foraging choices. This knowledge provides a basis for the selection of honey bees for pear pollination and lays a foundation for further study of the chemical communication of pear attractiveness to honey bees.

1. Introduction

Approximately 90% of flowering plants worldwide rely on interactions with various pollinators in their environments for reproduction, and insects account for the majority of pollinators and perform pollination services for approximately 75% of global crops [1,2]. The long period of coevolution between pollinators and flowering plants has led to a complicated and mutually beneficial pollinator–plant relationship. Plants evolved direct and indirect floral traits, such as color, floral scent, and flower rewards (pollen and nectar), to attract pollinators by visual or olfactory signals [3]. Bees are capable of learning floral signals during their foraging bouts [4,5,6,7]. Nevertheless, insufficient, or inefficient pollination is still common in several crops and is caused by the foraging bias of pollinators [8,9]. Knowledge about the foraging decisions of pollinators on crops is still scarce [10,11].
Pear is an insufficiently and inefficiently pollinated crop. Due to their self-incompatibility, pear plants are highly dependent on insects or artificial pollination [12,13]. Among insect pollinators, bees, including honey bees, bumble bees, and solitary bees, are the pollinators with the greatest potential for pear pollination [14,15,16]. Honey bees are commonly regarded as the primary pollinators. However, it was demonstrated that pear flowers are not as attractive as the flowers of other crops, such as apple, peach, and rape, due to the low volume and sugar concentration of their nectar [13,14,17]. Microorganism inoculation, bee attractants, pollen trapping, and syrup feeding have been explored to promote honey bee foraging and pear pollination in orchards [18,19,20].
Studies have chemically characterized the floral scent of pear and identified those compounds thereof that are physiologically active in honey bees [21,22]. Linalool + methyl benzoate or methyl 2-hydroxy-3-methylpentanoate were the most abundant compounds in several European pear cultivars (P. communis), and 17 were electrophysiologically active in honey bee antennae [22]. For native pear species, 3-methyl-1-butanol and (+)-limonene volatiles were thought to have the highest relative content, and 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one, 2-phenethyl alcohol, and 1-nonanal showed low attraction to honey bees [21]. To date, no study has compared the scent of these two pear species, and there might be differences in their floral scents, with a potential impact on attractiveness to pollinators.
China contains the most plentiful Pyrus germplasm resources in the world and is the world’s leading country in pear production [23]. Pyrus bretschneideri (Rosales: Rosaceae) is the most important commercial and endemic pear species and comprises approximately 23% of the total pear production in China [24]. Pyrus communis (Rosales: Rosaceae) is a common European pear species that has been cultivated in parts of China in recent years [25]. Both pear species rely on honey bee pollination for successful fruit sets. Two managed honey bee species, A. cerana and A. mellifera, are the most promising honey bees for pear pollination in China. Apis cerana is an endemic bee species in Asia and is an important pollinator of native plants [26]. Apis mellifera is the most popular bee species reared around the world and is the most important pollinator for crops [27,28]. Both honey bees were mass reared and have been demonstrated to be the main pollinators of pear in China [16]. In particular, about 50% of pears in production at present are artificially pollinated, which has many disadvantages such as large labor and high cost [29]. It is essential to explore the foraging decision of both honey bees on pears to replace hand pollination.
Honey bees play an important role in the pollination of pear, yet the foraging choices and olfactory cues honeybees use when foraging on pear flowers are unknown. The foraging behaviors of native A. cerana and exotic A. mellifera on native pear P. bretschneideri and exotic pear P. communis were studied, and the biological activity of the pear flower volatiles in antennae of honeybee pollinators was studied by using gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC–MS) and gas chromatography coupled to electroantennographic detection (GC–EAD). The following questions were addressed: (1) Do native honey bees prefer to forage on native pear species? (2) Does pear species affect honey bee foraging decisions? (3) Do the antennae of honey bees respond to the compounds released by pear flowers? The results from this study will help to guide efforts to support pear pollination.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Pear Orchard and Honey Bees

This study was conducted in a pear orchard in the Yuncheng region, Shanxi Province, China. The area of the pear orchard was approximately 6.6 hectares. Pyrus bretschneideri is the dominant species planted in the orchard. The distance between rows was 3.5 m, and the distance between adjacent trees within a row was 3 m. The pear trees were approximately 20 years old. A few P. communis trees were planted to test their adaptability on the side of the orchard. Both pear species were cared for according to professional management suggestions.
At least six colonies of each honeybee A. cerana and A. mellifera was prepared every year. All colonies (never having foraged on pears before) were in single Langstroth hives and adjusted to equal honeybee populations (approximately 3000–3500 workers), each with two frames. Each colony had a laying queen, a few larvae, and little pollen and nectar. Bees were introduced to a flight cage one day prior to observation to acclimatize to the cage environment.

2.2. Behavioral Observation Experiment

2.2.1. The Flight Cage Arrangement

The experiments were performed in flight cages (6 m × 4 m × 3 m), which consisted of a steel frame covered with a fine nylon mesh (20 mesh) (Figure 1A). Two flight cages were built in an open, flat area of the orchard, about the center of the orchard surrounding by pear trees, and adjacent to each other to ensure that the climate factors inside the cages were similar. Clean water was supplied in plastic basins. Four to five newly blossoming P. bretschneideri and P. communis twigs with approximately 30 flowers containing anthers carrying pollen for each pear species were cut from trees and placed on a well-watered clay flower board (Figure 1B). The two flight cages were with both pear species, and each cage was for one honeybee species (one for A. mellifera and the other for A. cerana). The flowers were replaced by new pear flowers when the anthers were empty. The clay flower boards were watered every hour to maintain moisture.

2.2.2. Behavioral Observation

The relative attractiveness of P. bretschneideri and P. communis pear flowers to honey bees was determined during the peak pear flowering period from 2019 to 2021. Pear flowers were placed 4.5 m from the hive (Figure 1C). Observations were conducted from 9:00 a.m. to 17:00 p.m., and each observation lasted for 10 min per hour. The number of honey bees landing on flowers to gather pollen or nectar was recorded. Daily records were used for statistical analysis. The positions of the flowers from the two pear species were changed daily to avoid the potential influence of location memories during honey bee foraging. Metrological data such as daily temperature and relative humidity were recorded. The experienced observers were rotated daily to prevent observer bias. Since the pear flower blooming period lasted only a few days, we replicated the experiment over 3 years to have enough data and repetition for statistical power.

2.3. Flower Volatile Collection

To determine the extent to which pear flower volatiles influence honey bee foraging preferences, we collected volatile samples from P. bretschneideri and P. communis flowers in situ from 10:00 a.m. to 4:00 p.m. during the blooming period. One P. bretschneideri and one P. communis flower twig in full bloom were randomly selected and bagged with Tedlar PVF bags (1 L). Quartz glass tubes (length: 120 mm; inner diameter: 6 mm) filled with 150 mg Tenax-TA (mesh 60–80, Supelco, Bellefonte, PA, USA) were connected with an atmospheric sampler (QC-2B, BMILP Science & Technology Development, Beijing, China) by Teflon tubes, and the volatiles emitted from the flowers were trapped in the absorbent tubes. The flow rate was adjusted to 500 mL/min by a flowmeter, and samples were collected for 2.5 h. Samples were also collected from empty bags as controls. The trapped volatiles were eluted with 2 mL n-hexane (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA), concentrated to 400 µL under a gentle stream of N2, and stored at −80 °C until use.

2.4. Y-Tube Olfactometer Bioassays

The Y-tube olfactometer (stem 20 cm, arms 15 cm, at an angle of 45°, internal diameter of 2.2 cm) was used for the bioassays. The olfactometer arms were connected to glass gas desiccators. Ten microliters of volatile samples (P. bretschneideri and P. communis) were applied to two filter paper strips (3 × 1.5 cm2) and then put into two glass gas desiccators. The paper strips were allowed to evaporate for 30 s before gas was passed from both arms to the stem through cleaned and humidified airflow created by an air pump system with an activated charcoal filter and distilled water. The airflow through each of the olfactometer arms was 500 mL/min. We hung a white fluorescent light directly above the olfactometer to avoid the effect of honey bee phototaxis on selection.
The experimental bee colonies were never exposed to pear flowers. The honey bee foragers (collected from the entrances of the beehives) were tested independently through 5 min observation periods in the olfactometer, and their behaviors were assigned to one of three choices: (1) the individual moved toward the P. bretschneideri flower volatile samples (the workers went 5 cm past the Y junction and stayed there for more than 30 s); (2) the individual moved toward the P. communis flower volatile samples; and (3) the individual made no choice (the worker did not reach the decision line within 5 min). For each honey bee species, we repeated this test with 50 foragers and compared the number of different choices (i.e., moving toward one of the two pear flower volatile samples).

2.5. GC–MS and GC–EAD

The flower volatile samples of pear were analyzed by GC–MS (QP2010, Shimadzu, Kyoto, Japan) equipped with an HP-5Ms column (30 m × 0.25 mm× 0.25 μm, Supelco). Samples were run in a splitless system, and helium was used as the carrier gas (flow: 1.5 mL min−1). One microliter was injected into a 250 °C injector. The GC oven temperature started at 50 °C and was then increased by 10 °C/min to 280 °C, where it was held for 5 min. The MS interface worked at 250 °C. Mass spectra were taken at 70 eV (in EI mode) from 34 m/z to 550 m/z. Compounds were identified by comparing mass spectra against synthetic standards and NIST 14 library matches. The relative quantity of each compound was calculated by the area of each peak divided by the total area of all compounds.
Gas chromatography coupled to electroantennographic detection (GC–EAD) was used to identify volatile compounds of pear flowers perceived by antennae of A. mellifera and A. cerana workers. The GC–EAD system consisted of a gas chromatograph (Agilent 7890A, Santa Clara, CA, USA) equipped with a flame ionization detector (FID) and an EAD setup provided by Syntech (Kirchzarten, Germany). A 3 μL aliquot of each scent sample was injected (temperature of injector: 250 °C) at 50 °C oven temperature, and the GC program was the same as GC–MS. In the electrophysiological experiments, flower volatiles of P. communis were tested on the antennae of 12 A. mellifera workers and 9 A. cerana workers, and flower volatiles of P. bretschneideri were tested on antennae of 10 A. mellifera workers and 10 A. cerana workers.
The bees were caught at the entrance of hives, and antennae were cut off with iris scissors under the microscope, mounted between two glass capillary electrodes that were filled with Ringer’s solution (8.0 g/L NaCl, 0.4 g/L KCl, 0.4 g/L CaCl2), and connected to silver wire electrodes. The reference electrode was in contact with the cut surface of an antenna, while the recording electrode was in contact with the cut tip of an antenna. A flower volatile compound was described as EAD-active when it elicited an antenna response in at least half of the replicates. All EAD-active compounds were identified by GC–MS.

2.6. Data Statistics

The chi-square goodness of fit test was applied to daily field observation data and Y-tube test results to determine the foraging preference of bees between pear flowers. The number of bees observed every 10 min of the day was added up as daily observation data. The null hypothesis in these tests was that honey bees were equally attracted to the P. communis and P. bretschneideri pear flowers. Individual honey bees who did not respond to the Y-tube olfactometer were omitted from the statistical analysis. A generalized linear model (GLM) was used to examine the foraging proportion for differences in foraging between bee species on each pear species. The foraging proportion is the ratio of the number of bees that visited each pear species on a given day to the overall number of bees on that day. The observational data were separated into three groups by year, as observations were conducted in various years. The GLM included ‘honey bee species’ and ‘pear species’ as fixed factors, and ‘year’ as the random factor. The relative contents of flower volatile components of two pear species were calculated from the ratio of the peak area of each compound to the total peak area.

3. Results

3.1. Foraging Choices of Honey Bees on Pears

The three-year flight cage experiments showed that both honey bee species significantly preferred P. communis flowers over P. bretschneideri flowers (p < 0.01). On the first day of the experiment each year, there was little preference for honey bees foraging choices, and a low number of bees were recorded foraging on pear flowers (Figure 2).
Eleven days of observation showed that the number of A. cerana individuals foraging on P. communis was significantly higher than that foraging on P. bretschneideri (p < 0.01, Figure 2A). The numbers of A. cerana foraging on pear flowers daily ranged from 70 to 1181. The maximum number of A. cerana observed foraging on P. communis was 781, while 400 A. cerana individuals were foraging on P. bretschneideri2 = 122.914, p < 0.001). The minimum number of A. cerana observed foraging on P. communis was 54 bees, and on P. bretschneideri, it was 16 bees (χ2 = 20.629, p < 0.001) (Figure 2A).
The foraging preferences of A. mellifera differed significantly between P. communis and P. bretschneideri flowers (p < 0.05, Figure 2B). The numbers of A. mellifera foraging on pear flowers daily ranged from 41 to 248 bees. The maximum number of A. mellifera foraging on P. communis was 149, while that on P. bretschneideri was 99 bees (χ2 = 10.081, df = 1, p < 0.001). The minimum numbers of A. mellifera foraging on P. communis and P. bretschneideri were 27 and 14 bees (χ2 = 4.122, df = 1, p = 0.042), respectively.

3.2. Comparison of the Proportion of Honey Bees Foraging

Foraging proportions were significantly affected by pear species, whereas bee species and year had no effects (generalized linear model, bee species: p = 0.26; pear cultivar: p < 0.001 and year: p = 1.00). The exact differences between bee species are shown in Table 1. The foraging proportion in P. communis was higher for both A. cerana and A. mellifera than in P. bretschneideri (Figure 3).
The proportions of A. cerana and A. mellifera that foraged on P. bretschneideri were 35.63 ± 0.02% and 37.90 ± 0.02% (p > 0.05), respectively. For P. communis, A. mellifera showed a weaker preference than A. cerana at 64.37 ± 0.02% and 62.10 ± 0.02% (p > 0.05), respectively, according to records over three years. Both A. cerana and A. mellifera visited more P. communis flowers per year than P. bretschneideri flowers, with a similar foraging proportion.

3.3. Y-Tube Olfactometer Experiment

The results of the Y-tube olfactometer experiment showed that the honey bee species were significantly affected by pear volatiles (p < 0.05). Both honey bee species preferred the volatiles of P. communis over those of P. bretschneideri. In the experiment with A. mellifera, 30 bees (65.22%) were attracted to P. communis, 16 bees (34.78%) were attracted to P. bretschneideri2 = 4.261, df = 1, p = 0.032), and 4 bees did not make a choice. In the experiment with A. cerana, 32 bees (71.11%) were attracted to P. communis, 13 bees (28.89%) were attracted to P. bretschneideri2 = 8.022, df = 1, p = 0.005), and 5 bees did not make a choice (Figure 4).

3.4. GC–MS and GC–EAD

P. bretschneideri and P. communis flowers had considerably different volatile chemicals and relative amounts of those components (Table 2). In total, seven kinds were assigned to 41 compounds that were detected: 9 alkane compounds, 8 alkene compounds, 6 nitrogen-containing compounds, 5 terpenoids, four ketones compounds, three aldehyde compounds, two ester compounds, one alcohol compound, one phenolic compound, and two unknown compounds. Of these 41 compounds, only 17 compounds were detected in both pear species, and the amounts of these compounds also varied between species. Linalool, one of the principal compounds that dominated the fragrances of P. communis (22.92 ± 9.03%) but only accounted for 0.97 ± 0.78% in P. bretschneideri, had a quite striking difference in relative quantity. Methyl L-isoleucinate was the most abundant component in P. bretschneideri (52.60 ± 2.10%) but only 17.95 ± 3.58% in P. communis. There were also variations in other chemicals between species. For example, the content of methyl 2-hydroxy-3-methylpentanoate was 30.68 ± 3.58% in P. communis, whereas in P. bretschneideri, it was only 15.25 ± 3.36%.
The antennae of A. cerana and A. mellifera responded to 16 compounds of all pear flower volatiles (Figure 5). L-valine methyl ester, methyl L-isoleucinate, benzaldehyde, 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one, β-ocimene, and linalool were the only components that were EAD-active in both bee species. 1-nonanol, methyl 2-hydroxy-3-methylpentanoate, isophorone, 4-oxoisophorone, 2-methyl octane, longifolene, caryophyllene, α-farnesene, and longicyclene were the only EAD-active compounds for A. cerana. Lilac alcohol D was the only EAD-active compound for A. mellifera. Together, the number of EAD active components in P. bretschneideri accounted for 41.94%, while the content accounted for 81.23 ± 11.08%. The number of active compounds in P. communis accounted for 29.63%, and their content was 78.18 ± 17.36% of the total volatile amount.

4. Discussion

Flowering plants have evolved various floral characteristics to attract pollinators [3]. Flowers have varying degrees of colors, scents, and rewards that contribute to attracting pollinators [1,3]. The findings of this study revealed that pear flower volatiles significantly influence the foraging preferences of honey bees. Both native and exotic honey bees prefer to forage on the European pear P. communis. This means that the native honey bees were not prone to foraging on the native pear P. bretschneideri when they had a choice. Therefore, the long-term coevolution between the native pear and native honey bee species was less influential than the pear species’ identity on foraging behavior. This study offered a foundational understanding of honey bee foraging preference in pear species, which helps influence pear pollination support.
Native plants and pollinators are thought to form a mutually adaptable relationship in the process of coevolution [30]. As an endemic honey bee, A. cerana evolved with the native pear species and had a stronger preference than A. mellifera for foraging on P. bretschneideri, and it has been demonstrated that A. cerana has adapted to foraging on less concentrated nectars [16,31]. However, according to the results of the flight cage observations and Y-tube experiments, P. bretschneideri was not more attractive to A. cerana than P. communis was to A. mellifera. This pattern may have been caused by the advantage of the exotic pear, P. communis, which has evolved stronger attractive pollination traits, such as the production of volatile compounds, which insects prefer over other traits [22]. No differences were observed in the foraging patterns of either honey bee species on the first day of the field experiment (Figure 2). This pattern may have been caused by the bees that initially touched the pear flowers, causing the foragers to receive negative feedback from the larvae or younger workers feeding on the pollen or nectar [32,33].
Flower rewards have a great influence on the foraging preferences of pollinators. It was observed that the numbers of A. cerana and A. mellifera foraging on P. communis flowers were significantly higher than those foraging on P. bretschneideri. The nectar and pollen of pears were demonstrated to be less attractive to bees than those of apples and apricots [13,34]. It was also reported that the foraging activity of honey bees on several Asian pear species differed due to the sucrose contents in nectar, with higher contents increasing the attractiveness of pear flowers [35]. Therefore, the volume and sugar content of nectar are important factors that influence honey bee foraging behaviors [36,37]. The average amount of nectar in P. bretschneideri flowers was 1.77 μL/flower, which was lower than that of P. communis flowers, which offered a mean volume of 2.8 μL [16,38]. Moreover, the total sugar content of P. bretschneideri and P. communis were different (64.2 mg/mL and 90 mg/mL, respectively) [16,38]. In addition to nectar, pollen is also a vital food reward that affects the foraging decisions of bees, with the amino acid composition of pollen playing an especially important role [39]. Bees show a preference for pollen that is richer in essential amino acids [13,40]. Low levels of amino acids lead to inferior weight gain, less protein or nitrogen, reduced longevity, and an incomplete development of hypopharyngeal glands (HPG), which could impair the growth of larvae and queens [32,41,42,43,44]. The contents of essential amino acids (leucine, valine, and isoleucine) were higher in the pollen of P. communis than in that of native pear, P. bretschneideri [13,32]. Hence, further study of the influence of the amino acid composition of the pollen from these pear species on honey bee foraging behavior is necessary.
Flower volatiles are considered to be one of the primary determinants of pollinator foraging decisions, especially when flower colors and morphologies are identical [45,46,47]. The electrophysiological investigations demonstrated that bees are able to perceive volatile molecules from flowers and respond differently to volatile compounds. Honey bees may use flower-specific compounds to locate pear flowers. Of the 108 flower volatiles identified in several European pear cultivars, 17 compounds are electrophysiologically active in honey bee antennae [22]. Among the 76 compounds found in the volatiles of three pear cultivars (Su, Ya, and Xuehua), only 5 compounds generated robust electroantennography (EAG) responses in honey bees [21]. We confirmed that 16 compounds are electrophysiologically active. A few of these compounds, including linalool and 4-oxoisophorone, are known to elicit physiological or even behavioral responses in honeybees [22,48]. These compounds may play a role in the communication between pear flowers and bee pollinators, leading to preference or avoidance responses to various odor stimuli [49]. European pear flower volatiles contain some common floral compounds that have already been identified as being electrophysiologically or behaviorally active in honey bees (linalool, lilac aldehyde, 4-oxoisophorone, etc.) [50,51,52,53]. Among all these active compounds, several compounds, such as 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one, 2-phenethyl alcohol, and 1-nonanal, have been verified to be repellent to bees [21]. The lack of attractive volatile compounds may explain why honey bees are less attracted to honey bees than other contemporaneous flowering plants.
The foraging behavior of honey bees was greatly influenced by pear species, and flower volatiles played a certain role in foraging decisions. The three-year field experiment showed that honey bee foraging was influenced by pear species and their volatiles. The results of the field observation and Y-tube experiments showed that the proportions of the preferences of the two honey bee species were consistent. There were significant differences in the volatiles of P. bretschneideri and P. communis flowers; several compounds were physiologically active in honey bees and were potentially behaviorally active. The determination of single volatile compounds in attracting honey bees and the regulatory mechanisms of pear flower volatiles, especially specific odor-related receptor proteins and their functions, are still unclear. It would be interesting to test to what extent the potential functions of the volatiles identified in this study explain the behavior of pollinator switches among species and varieties in orchards.

5. Conclusions

We found that honey bees could make foraging decisions based on pear flower volatiles and had the ability to distinguish specific compounds within them. Our data indicated that bees were able to distinguish European pear P. communis from Asian pear P. bretschneideri by flower volatiles and had a substantial foraging preference for the European pear P. communis, which may be caused by flower volatiles or rewards. The relative contents of principal volatile aroma emissions from two kinds of pear flowers were discovered to be distinct, which led to a difference in the fragrance of the flowers. Furthermore, we discovered that honey bee antennae responded to a number of chemical constituents of flower smell that, depending on their nature or composition, may either attract or repel bees [54]. Despite our investigations of the interactions between honey bees and pears, future research should focus on the precise impacts of certain chemicals on the behavior of honey bees. This study provides a foundation for the combination of pear species in orchards and the selection of honey bees for pear pollination, as well as a foundation for further research into the chemical communication between pears and honey bees.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, W.S., Y.J. and J.H.; methodology, W.S., Y.J. and J.H.; software, W.S. and J.H.; validation, W.S., Y.J. and J.H.; investigation, W.S., W.M., Q.Z. and X.H.; resources, W.S.; data curation, W.S. and J.H.; writing—original draft preparation, W.S.; writing—review and editing, W.S., W.M., G.D., Y.J. and J.H.; visualization, W.S. and J.H.; supervision, J.H.; project administration, J.H.; funding acquisition, J.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Natural Science Foundation of China, grant number 31870211; the China Agriculture Research System of MOF and MARA, grant number CARS-44; and the Agricultural Science and Technology Innovation Program of CAAS, grant number CAAS-ASTIP-2021-IAR.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Jinshan Shen, Jun Lan, Ran Liu, and Hongru Guo for their assistance during the field work.

Conflicts of Interest

The named authors have no conflict of interest, financial or otherwise.

References

  1. Schiestl, F.P. The evolution of floral scent and insect chemical communication. Ecol. Lett. 2010, 13, 643–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Garibaldi, L.A.; Aizen, M.A.; Cunningham, S.; Klein, A.M. Pollinator shortage and global crop yield: Looking at the whole spectrum of pollinator dependency. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2009, 2, 37–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Wilbert, S.M.; Bradshaw, H.D.; Otto, K.G.; Schemske, D.W. Genetic mapping of floral traits associated with reproductive isolation in monkeyflowers (Mimulus). Nature 1995, 376, 762–765. [Google Scholar]
  4. Kaiser, L.; De Jong, R. Multi-odour memory influenced by learning order. Behav. Process. 1993, 30, 175–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Hill, P.S.M.; Wells, P.H.; Wells, H. Spontaneous flower constancy and learning in honey bees as a function of colour. Anim. Behav. 1997, 54, 615–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Beekman, M. How long will honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) be stimulated by scent to revisit past-profitable forage sites? J. Comp. Physiol. A 2005, 191, 1115–1120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Zhang, S.W.; Schwarz, S.; Pahl, M.; Zhu, H.; Tautz, J. Honeybee memory: A honeybee knows what to do and when. J. Exp. Biol. 2006, 209, 4420–4428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Quinet, M.; Jacquemart, A. Cultivar placement affects pollination efficiency and fruit production in European pear (Pyrus communis) orchards. Eur. J. Agron. 2017, 91, 84–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Grass, I.; Meyer, S.; Taylor, P.J.; Foord, S.H.; Hajek, P.; Tscharntke, T. Pollination limitation despite managed honeybees in South African macadamia orchards. Agric. Ecosyst. Environ. 2018, 260, 11–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Rodriguez-Saona, C.; Parra, L.; Quiroz, A.; Isaacs, R. Variation in highbush blueberry floral volatile profiles as a function of pollination status, cultivar, time of day and flower part: Implications for flower visitation by bees. Ann. Bot. 2011, 107, 1377–1390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Adler, L.S.; Irwin, R.E. What you smell is more important than what you see? Natural selection on floral scent. New Phytol. 2012, 195, 510–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Maccagnani, B.; Ladurner, E.; Santi, F.; Burgio, G. Osmia cornuta (Hymenoptera, Megachilidae) as a pollinator of pear (Pyrus communis): Fruit- and seed-set. Apidologie 2003, 34, 207–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Quinet, M.; Warzée, M.; Vanderplanck, M.; Michez, D.; Lognay, G.; Jacquemart, A. Do floral resources influence pollination rates and subsequent fruit set in pear (Pyrus communis L.) and apple (Malus x domestica Borkh) cultivars? Eur. J. Agron. 2016, 77, 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Monzón, V.; Bosch, J.; Retana, J. Foraging behavior and pollinating effectiveness of Osmia cornuta (Hymenoptera: Megachilidae) and Apis mellifera (Hymenoptera: Apidae) on “Comice” pear. Apidologie 2004, 35, 575–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Zisovich, A.H.; Goldway, M.; Schneider, D.; Steinberg, S.; Stern, E.; Stern, R.A. Adding bumblebees (Bombus terrestris L., Hymenoptera: Apidae) to pear orchards increases seed number per fruit, fruit set, fruit size and yield. J. Hortic. Sci. Biotech. 2012, 87, 353–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gemeda, T.K.; Shao, Y.Q.; Wu, W.Q.; Yang, H.P.; Huang, J.X.; Wu, J. Native honey bees outperform adventive honey bees in increasing Pyrus bretschneideri (Rosales: Rosaceae) pollination. J. Econ. Entomol. 2017, 110, 2290–2294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Faoro, I.D.; Orth, A.I. Flower visiting insects during the bloom period of Japanese pear orchards in Brazil. Acta Hortic. 2015, 1094, 275–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ma, W.H.; Shao, Y.Q.; Zhao, H.T.; Tian, S.H.; Meng, J.; Yang, S.S.; Du, Y.L.; Jiang, Y.S. Using bee attractants to improve honeybee foraging on Dangshan Pear (Pyrus communis L.). J. Agric. Sci. Technol. 2015, 17, 1551–1558. [Google Scholar]
  19. Gemeda, T.K.; Li, J.L.; Luo, S.D.; Yang, H.P.; Jin, T.T.; Huang, J.X.; Wu, J. Pollen trapping and sugar syrup feeding of honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) enhance pollen collection of less preferred flowers. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e203648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Colda, A.; Bossaert, S.; Verreth, C.; Vanhoutte, B.; Honnay, O.; Keulemans, W.; Lievens, B. Inoculation of pear flowers with Metschnikowia reukaufii and Acinetobacter nectaris enhances attraction of honeybees and hoverflies, but does not increase fruit and seed set. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e250203. [Google Scholar]
  21. Ma, W.H.; Long, D.L.; Wang, Y.; Li, X.Y.; Huang, J.X.; Shen, J.S.; Su, W.T.; Jiang, Y.S.; Li, J. Electrophysiological and behavioral responses of Asian and European honeybees to pear flower volatiles. J. Asia-Pac. Entomol. 2021, 24, 221–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lukas, K.; Harig, T.; Schulz, S.; Hadersdorfer, J.; Dötterl, S. Flowers of European pear release common and uncommon volatiles that can be detected by honey bee pollinators. Chemoecology 2019, 29, 211–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Teng, Y. The pear industry and research in China. Acta Hortic. 2011, 909, 161–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Qin, G.H.; Tao, S.T.; Zhang, H.P.; Huang, W.J.; Wu, J.Y.; Xu, Y.L.; Zhang, S.L. Evolution of the aroma volatiles of pear fruits supplemented with fatty acid metabolic precursors. Molecules 2014, 19, 20183–20196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Yu, D.J. Rosaceae. In Flora of China; Editorial Committee of Flora of China, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Ed.; Science Press: Beijing, China, 1974; Volume 36, p. 361. [Google Scholar]
  26. Koetz, A. Ecology, behaviour and control of Apis cerana with a focus on relevance to the Australian incursion. Insects 2013, 4, 558–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Díaz, P.C.; Arenas, A.; Fernández, V.M.; Susic Martin, C.; Basilio, A.M.; Farina, W.M. Honeybee cognitive ecology in a fluctuating agricultural setting of apple and pear trees. Behav. Ecol. 2013, 24, 1058–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Smessaert, J.; Honnay, O.; Keulemans, W. Monitoring pollinator activity in an apple and pear orchard, linked with the analysis of the nectar composition. Acta Hortic. 2019, 1231, 59–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Wenqing, W.; Yuan, G.; Jinshan, S.; Weihua, M.; Guibo, G.; Youquan, S. Present situation investigation of bee pollination for pear. Apic. China 2011, 62, 40–44. [Google Scholar]
  30. Krishna, S.; Keasar, T. Morphological complexity as a floral signal: From perception by insect pollinators to co-evolutionary implications. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2018, 19, 1681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Yang, W.C.; Kuang, H.; Wang, S.S.; Wang, J.; Liu, W.; Wu, Z.H.; Tian, Y.Y.; Huang, Z.Y.; Miao, X.Q. Comparative sucrose responsiveness in Apis mellifera and A. cerana foragers. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e79026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Lan, J.; Ding, G.L.; Ma, W.H.; Jiang, Y.S.; Huang, J.X. Pollen source affects development and behavioral preferences in honey bees. Insects 2021, 12, 130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Pang, C.; Dong, K.; Guo, Y.; Ding, G.; Lu, Y.; Guo, Z.; Wu, J.; Huang, J. Effects of three types of pollen on the growth and development of honey bee Larvae (Hymenoptera, Apidae). Front. Ecol. Environ. 2022, 10, 870081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Vaziritabar, S.; Oshidari, S.; Esmaeilzade, S.M. Comparative foraging behavior of eastern honeybee, (Apis cerana F.) and western honeybee, (Apis mellifera carnica) in pollinating pear and apricot flowers in Taleghan, Iran. J. Biol. Envion. Sci. 2015, 7, 136–149. [Google Scholar]
  35. Seo, H.; Song, J.; Yoon, H.J.; Lee, K.Y. Effects of nectar contents on the foraging activity of honeybee (Apis mellifera) on Asian pear (Pyrus pyrifolia Nakai). Sci. Hortic. 2019, 245, 185–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Canto, A.; Herrera, C.M.; Medrano, M.; Perez, R.; Garcia, I.M. Pollinator foraging modifies nectar sugar composition in Helleborus foetidus (Ranunculaceae): An experimental test. Am. J. Bot. 2008, 95, 315–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Arenas, A.; Kohlmaier, M.G. Nectar source profitability influences individual foraging preferences for pollen and pollen-foraging activity of honeybee colonies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2019, 73, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Jacquemart, A.L.; Michotte-Van Der Aa, A.; Raspé, O. Compatibility and pollinator efficiency tests on Pyrus communis L. cv. ‘Conference’. J. Hortic. Sci. Biotech. 2006, 81, 827–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Nepi, M.; Grasso, D.A.; Mancuso, S. Nectar in plant-insect mutualistic relationships: From food reward to partner manipulation. Front. Plant Sci. 2018, 9, 1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Cook, S.M.; Awmack, C.S.; Murray, D.A.; Williams, I.H. Are honey bees’ foraging preferences affected by pollen amino acid composition? Ecol. Entomol. 2003, 28, 622–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kunert, K.; Crailsheim, K. Seasonal changes in carbohydrate, lipid and protein content in emerging worker honeybees and their mortality. J. Apicult. Res. 1988, 27, 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Zaytoon, A.A.T.U.; Matsuka, M.; Sasaki, M. Feeding efficiency of pollen substitutes in a honeybee colony: Effect of feeding site on royal jelly and queen production. Appl. Entomol. Zool. 1988, 23, 481–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Schmidt, L.S.; Schmidt, J.O.; Rao, H.; Wang, W.Y.; Xu, L.G. Feeding preference and survival of young worker honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae) fed rape, sesame, and sunflower pollen. J. Econ. Entomol. 1995, 88, 1591–1595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Brodschneider, R.; Crailsheim, K. Nutrition and health in honey bees. Apidologie 2010, 41, 278–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Rachersberger, M.; Cordeiro, G.D.; Schäffler, I.; Dötterl, S. Honeybee pollinators use visual and floral scent cues to find apple (Malus domestica) flowers. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2019, 67, 13221–13227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Milet-Pinheiro, P.; Ayasse, M.; Schlindwein, C.; Dobson, H.E.M.; Dötterl, S. Host location by visual and olfactory floral cues in an oligolectic bee: Innate and learned behavior. Behav. Ecol. 2012, 23, 531–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Giurfa, M.; Núñez, J.; Backhaus, W. Odour and colour information in the foraging choice behaviour of the honeybee. J. Comp. Physiol. A 1994, 175, 773–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Twidle, A.M.; Mas, F.; Harper, A.R.; Horner, R.M.; Welsh, T.J.; Suckling, D.M. Kiwifruit flower odor perception and recognition by honey bees, Apis mellifera. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2015, 63, 5597–5602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Mas, F.; Horner, R.M.; Brierley, S.; Butler, R.C.; Suckling, D.M. Selection of key floral scent compounds from fruit and vegetable crops by honey bees depends on sensory capacity and experience. J. Insect Physiol. 2020, 121, 104002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Henning, J.A.; Peng, Y.; Montague, M.A.; Teuber, L.R. Honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) behavioral response to primary alfalfa (Rosales: Fabaceae) floral volatiles. J. Econ. Entomol. 1992, 85, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Blight, M.M.; Le Métayer, M.; Delègue, M.P.; Pickett, J.A.; Marion-Poll, F.; Wadhams, L.J. Identification of floral volatiles involved in recognition of oilseed rape flowers, Brassica napus by honeybees, Apis mellifera. J. Chem. Ecol. 1997, 23, 1715–1727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Knudsen, J.T.; Eriksson, R.; Gershenzon, J.; Ståhl, B. Diversity and distribution of floral scent. Bot. Rev. 2006, 72, 1–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Jürgens, A.; Glück, U.; Aas, G.; Dötterl, S. Diel fragrance pattern correlates with olfactory preferences of diurnal and nocturnal flower visitors in Salix caprea (Salicaceae). Bot. J. Linn. Soc. 2014, 175, 624–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Schiestl, F.P. Ecology and evolution of floral volatile-mediated information transfer in plants. New Phytol. 2015, 206, 571–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Behavioral observation experiment. (A) Flight cages; (B) flowers of P. bretschneideri and P. communis; (C) schematic setup of pear flowers and honey bees in the flight cages.
Figure 1. Behavioral observation experiment. (A) Flight cages; (B) flowers of P. bretschneideri and P. communis; (C) schematic setup of pear flowers and honey bees in the flight cages.
Agriculture 12 01074 g001
Figure 2. The daily foraging choices of honey bees. (A) Choices of A. cerana; (B) choices of A. mellifera (ns: p > 0.05; *: 0.01 < p < 0.05, **: 0.001 < p < 0.01, and ***: p < 0.001).
Figure 2. The daily foraging choices of honey bees. (A) Choices of A. cerana; (B) choices of A. mellifera (ns: p > 0.05; *: 0.01 < p < 0.05, **: 0.001 < p < 0.01, and ***: p < 0.001).
Agriculture 12 01074 g002
Figure 3. The mean proportions of A. cerana and A. mellifera foraging on P. communis and P. bretschneideri (mean ± SE; n = 13 for both bee species). Different letters indicate a significant difference (p < 0.05).
Figure 3. The mean proportions of A. cerana and A. mellifera foraging on P. communis and P. bretschneideri (mean ± SE; n = 13 for both bee species). Different letters indicate a significant difference (p < 0.05).
Agriculture 12 01074 g003
Figure 4. Choices of honey bees regarding different pear flower volatiles according to Y-tube olfactometer tests (*: 0.01 < p < 0.05, **: 0.001 < p < 0.01).
Figure 4. Choices of honey bees regarding different pear flower volatiles according to Y-tube olfactometer tests (*: 0.01 < p < 0.05, **: 0.001 < p < 0.01).
Agriculture 12 01074 g004
Figure 5. Examples of the antennal responses of honeybee workers (EAD) to volatiles collected from pear flowers (FID). Responses are numbered and correspond to numbers (No.) in Table 2. (A) antennal responses of A. cerana and A. mellifera workers to volatiles of P. communis flowers; (B) antennal responses of A. cerana and A. mellifera workers to volatiles of P. bretschneideri flowers.
Figure 5. Examples of the antennal responses of honeybee workers (EAD) to volatiles collected from pear flowers (FID). Responses are numbered and correspond to numbers (No.) in Table 2. (A) antennal responses of A. cerana and A. mellifera workers to volatiles of P. communis flowers; (B) antennal responses of A. cerana and A. mellifera workers to volatiles of P. bretschneideri flowers.
Agriculture 12 01074 g005
Table 1. Generalized linear model (calculated Wald χ2 and p value) of the effect of bee species (A. cerana and A. mellifera), pear species (P. bretschneideri and P. communis) and year on single pear flower foraging proportion.
Table 1. Generalized linear model (calculated Wald χ2 and p value) of the effect of bee species (A. cerana and A. mellifera), pear species (P. bretschneideri and P. communis) and year on single pear flower foraging proportion.
Bee SpeciesFactorsForaging Proportion
Wald χ2p-Value
Apis cerana
and
Apis mellifera
Bee species3.2411 × 10−290.260
Pear species175.67<0.001
Year2.287 × 10−281.000
Table 2. Number and relative peak area (% of total for compound classes and single components) of volatiles detected in flower volatile samples of P. communis and P. bretschneideri and their electrophysiological activity in antennae of honey bees (antennae responding to a compound).
Table 2. Number and relative peak area (% of total for compound classes and single components) of volatiles detected in flower volatile samples of P. communis and P. bretschneideri and their electrophysiological activity in antennae of honey bees (antennae responding to a compound).
No.Volatile CompoundsKRIRelative Peak Area (%)Electrophysiological Activity
P. communisP. bretschneideriA. ceranaA. mellifera
Nitrogen-containing compounds
1L-valine, methyl ester900 7.7 ± 2.575
2Methyl L-isoleucinate99917.949 ± 3.58452.574 ± 2.073
3Methyl nicotinate10541.965 ± 0.762
4Benzothiazole1208 0.549 ± 0.045
51-butanamine, N-(2-pyridinylmethylene)-14010.959 ± 0.557
6Benzoic acid, 4-ethoxy-, ethyl ester1448 0.509 ± 0.037
Aldehyde compounds
7Benzaldehyde982 0.678 ± 0.283
8Decanal12040.145 ± 0.1030.422 ± 0.188
9Dodecanal1402 0.266 ± 0.032
Ketone compounds
106-methyl-5-hepten-2-one938 1.033 ± 0.316
115,9-undecadien-2-one, 6,10-dimethyl-1420 0.769 ± 0.192
122,6-di-tert-butyl-p-benzoquinone1633 0.291 ± 0.03
132,5-cyclohexadiene-1,4-dione, 2,5-diphenyl-23530.163 ± 0.1290.589 ± 0.018
Alcohol compounds
141-nonanol11040.665 ± 0.470.165 ± 0.059
Ester compounds
15Methyl 2-hydroxy-3-methylpentanoate98330.675 ± 3.58215.254 ± 3.358
16Tris(1-chloro-2-propyl) phosphate0.272 ± 0.582
Phenol compounds
17Phenol9015.188 ± 2.0184.078 ± 3.545
Terpenoid compounds
18β-ocimene9762.005 ± 0.499
19Linalool108222.917 ± 9.0320.969 ± 0.786
20Isophorone1097 0.414 ± 0.063
214-oxoisophorone12683.502 ± 0.449
22Lilac alcohol D12510.224 ± 0.074
Alkane compounds
23Octane, 2-methyl1156 0.261 ± 0.02
24Dodecane12140.705 ± 0.5041.957 ± 0.434
25Dodecane, 2,6,11-trimethyl-13200.248 ± 0.0240.662 ± 0.163
26Tridecane1313 0.361 ± 0.029
27Tetradecane14131.504 ± 0.2093.233 ± 0.009
28Hexadecane16121.594 ± 0.3440.426 ± 0.109
29Heptadecane17113.309 ± 0.639
30Nonadecane16530.488 ± 0.0761.15 ± 0.022
31Heneicosane21090.892 ± 0.1610.949 ± 0.127
Alkene compounds
32Longicyclene1184 0.31 ± 0.079
33Longifolene1398 0.057 ± 0.019
34Caryophyllene1494 0.054 ± 0.007
35α-farnesene14580.245 ± 0.1210.498 ± 0.303
367-hexadecene, (Z)-16200.138 ± 0.0080.149 ± 0.033
37(E,E,E)-3,7,11,15-tetramethylhexadeca-1,3,6,10,14-pentaene19401.244 ± 0.399
389-eicosene, (E)-20171.390 ± 0.2861.556 ± 0.943
39(E)-4,8-dimethylnona-1,3,7-triene1.093 ± 0.767
Unknowns
40Unknown1 0.375 ± 0.08
41Unknown2 0.149 ± 0.0242.122 ± 0.378
KRI: Kovat’s retention index. Bolded numbers and “√” indicate the compound-elicited honey bee antenna response in at least half of the replicates. “Unknown” means that the compound had not been accurately identified by the NIST library but was present in multiple replicated samples.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Su, W.; Ma, W.; Zhang, Q.; Hu, X.; Ding, G.; Jiang, Y.; Huang, J. Honey Bee Foraging Decisions Influenced by Pear Volatiles. Agriculture 2022, 12, 1074. https://doi.org/10.3390/agriculture12081074

AMA Style

Su W, Ma W, Zhang Q, Hu X, Ding G, Jiang Y, Huang J. Honey Bee Foraging Decisions Influenced by Pear Volatiles. Agriculture. 2022; 12(8):1074. https://doi.org/10.3390/agriculture12081074

Chicago/Turabian Style

Su, Wenting, Weihua Ma, Qi Zhang, Xiao Hu, Guiling Ding, Yusuo Jiang, and Jiaxing Huang. 2022. "Honey Bee Foraging Decisions Influenced by Pear Volatiles" Agriculture 12, no. 8: 1074. https://doi.org/10.3390/agriculture12081074

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop