On the Relation between the Development of Working Memory Updating and Working Memory Capacity in Preschoolers
Abstract
:1. Introduction
The Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Measures
2.3.1. Updating Task
2.3.2. Working Memory (M Capacity) Tasks
3. Results
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Research on working memory development keeps flourishing; recently, some relevant studies investigated issues such as verbal coding, rehearsal, and the interference and distraction from irrelevant sounds (e.g., Elliott et al. 2016, 2021; Joseph et al. 2018; Röer et al. 2018). However, these lines of working memory research are not examined in this article, which only focuses on working memory capacity and its relation with updating. |
2 | Two other meta-analytic studies (Kassai et al. 2019; Sala and Gobet 2017) are potentially relevant here. However, they regard working memory globally as an executive function, without specifying whether the working memory trainings involved updating or capacity tasks. Moreover, Kassai et al. (2019) define far transfer as an effect of working memory training on inhibition or shifting performance—not on intelligence or academic performance. Therefore, the implications of these two studies for our argument are unclear. |
3 | The 118 children from the Panesi and Morra (2020) study took part in four sessions with various language, drawing, working memory, and executive function tasks. They performed the Direction Following Task in the first session, after three drawing tasks; the Mr. Cucumber and the Backward Word Span tasks were performed in the third session, followed by a language task; and the Magic House task was performed in the fourth session, between a shifting task (the Dimensional Change Card Sort) and a language task (the Test of Grammar Comprehension for Children). The other 58 children took part in four sessions with a large battery of working memory and executive function tasks. The placement of tasks in different sessions and the task order were counterbalanced across participants. This battery included no other working memory capacity measure than the three reported here; it also included three updating tasks, i.e., a Self-Ordered Pointing task and a child version of the Keep Track task, in addition to the Magic House task reported here. |
4 | To check whether we could safely assume that the relation of each test to age was linear, we performed a series of regression analyses, in which each measure was, in turn, the dependent variable. We inserted age as a predictor (which was highly significant, p < .001 for every test) and then, as potential predictors, using a stepwise method, we considered age-squared, age-cube, square root of age, logarithm of age, and a dummy variable (0 for children younger than five years, 1 for at least five years of age). None of these potential predictors entered the regression equation with a significant contribution; the highest t-value that we found was t(173) = .59, p > .55, for the logarithm of age with respect to the Mr. Cucumber task. In sum, there was no evidence for any non-linear component in the relation between any test and age. |
References
- Agostino, Alba, Janice Johnson, and Juan Pascual-Leone. 2010. Executive functions underlying multiplicative reasoning: Problem type matters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 105: 286–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Au, Jacky, Ellen Sheehan, Nancy Tsai, Greg J. Duncan, Martin Buschkuehl, and Susanne M. Jaeggi. 2015. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22: 366–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Au, Jacky, Martin Buschkuehl, Greg J. Duncan, and Susanne M. Jaeggi. 2016. There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is NOT effective: A reply to Melby-Lervåg and Hulme 2015. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23: 331–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrouillet, Pierre, Sophie Bernardin, and Valérie Camos. 2004. Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133: 83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barrouillet, Pierre, Nathalie Gavens, Evie Vergauwe, Vinciane Gaillard, and Valérie Camos. 2009. Working memory span development: A time-based resource-sharing model account. Developmental Psychology 45: 477–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Belacchi, Carmen, Barbara Carretti, and Cesare Cornoldi. 2010. The role of working memory and updating in Coloured Raven Matrices performance in typically developing children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 22: 1010–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Case, Robbie. 1985. Intellectual Development: Birth to Adulthood. New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan, Nelson. 2005. Working Memory Capacity. Hove: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowan, Nelson. 2016. Working memory maturation: Can we get to the essence of cognitive growth? Perspectives on Psychological Science 11: 239–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowan, Nelson. 2017. The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 24: 1158–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Diamond, Adele. 2013. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64: 135–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Elliott, Emily M., Robert W. Hughes, Alicia Briganti, Tanya N. Joseph, John E. Marsh, and Bill Macken. 2016. Distraction in verbal short-term memory: Insights from developmental differences. Journal of Memory and Language 88: 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Elliott, Emily M., Candice C. Morey, Angela M. AuBuchon, Nelson Cowan, Chris Jarrold, Eryn J. Adams, Meg Attwood, Büşra Bayram, Stefen Beeler-Duden, Taran Y. Blakstvedt, and et al. 2021. Multilab Direct Replication of Flavell, Beach, and Chinsky 1966: Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science 4: 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engle, Randall W., Michael J. Kane, and Stephen W. Tuholski. 1999. Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In Models of Working Memory. Edited by Akira Miyake and Priti Shah. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 102–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friedman, Naomi P., Akira Miyake, Robin P. Corley, Susan E. Young, John C. Defries, and John K. Hewitt. 2006. Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science 17: 172–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garon, Nancy, Susan E. Bryson, and Isabel M. Smith. 2008. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin 134: 31–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hildebrand, David K., James D. Laing, and Howard Rosenthal. 1977. Prediction Analysis of Cross Classifications. New York: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Himi, Samsad A., Markus Bühner, and Sven Hilbert. 2021. Advancing the understanding of the factor structure of executive functioning. Journal of Intelligence 9: 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iglesias-Sarmiento, Valentín, Nuria Carriedo López, and Juan L. Rodríguez Rodríguez. 2015. Updating executive function and performance in reading comprehension and problem solving. Anales de Psicología 31: 298–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Im-Bolter, Nancie, Janice Johnson, and Juan Pascual-Leone. 2006. Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development 77: 1822–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jaeggi, Susanne M., Martin Buschkuehl, John Jonides, and Walter J. Perrig. 2008. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 6829–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Johnson, Janice, Nancie Im-Bolter, and Juan Pascual-Leone. 2003. Development of mental attention in gifted and mainstream children: The role of mental capacity, inhibition, and speed of processing. Child Development 74: 1594–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Joseph, Tanya N., Robert W. Hughes, Patrik Sörqvist, and John E. Marsh. 2018. Differences in Auditory Distraction between Adults and Children: A Duplex-mechanism Approach. Journal of Cognition 1: 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kane, Michael J., Leslie H. Brown, Jennifer C. McVay, Paul J. Silvia, Inez Myin-Germeys, and Thomas R. Kwapil. 2007. For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science 18: 614–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kassai, Reka, Judit Futo, Zsolt Demetrovics, and Zsofia K. Takacs. 2019. A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence on the near- and far-transfer effects among children’s executive function skills. Psychological Bulletin 145: 165–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervåg, Monica, Thomas S. Redick, and Charles Hulme. 2016. Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer”: Evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science 11: 512–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Meng, Xiao-Li, Robert Rosenthal, and Donald B. Rubin. 1992. Comparing correlated correlation coefficients. Psychological Bulletin 111: 172–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, Akira, Naomi P. Friedman, Michael J. Emerson, Alexander H. Witzki, Amy Howerter, and Tor D. Wager. 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Morra, Sergio. 1994. Issues in working memory measurement: Testing for M capacity. International Journal of Behavioral Development 17: 143–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morra, Sergio, Roberta Camba, Guglielmo Calvini, and Fabrizio Bracco. 2013. Italians do it better? M-capacity measurement and cross-linguistic differences in the direction following task (DFT). Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics 13: 9–24. [Google Scholar]
- Morra, Sergio, Sabrina Panesi, Laura Traverso, and M. Carmen Usai. 2018. Which tasks measure what? Reflections on executive function development and a commentary on Podjarny, Kamawar, and Andrews 2017. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 167: 246–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morris, Neil, and Dylan M. Jones. 1990. Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. British Journal of Psychology 81: 111–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muthén, Linda K., and Bengt O. Muthén. 2002. How to use a Monte Carlo study to decide on sample size and determine power. Structural Equation Modeling 9: 599–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oberauer, Klaus. 2009. Design for a working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 51: 45–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Palladino, Paola, Cesare Cornoldi, Rossana De Beni, and Francesca Pazzaglia. 2001. Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Memory & Cognition 29: 344–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Panesi, Sabrina, and Sergio Morra. 2017. La Casetta Magica. Un nuovo strumento per indagare l’aggiornamento (updating) della memoria di lavoro in età prescolare [The Magic House: A new measure of updating for preschoolers]. Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo 21: 443–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panesi, Sabrina, and Sergio Morra. 2020. Executive Functions and Mental Attentional Capacity in Preschoolers. Journal of Cognition and Development 21: 72–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pascual-Leone, Juan. 1970. A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget’s developmental stages. Acta Psychologica 63: 301–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pascual-Leone, Juan. 1987. Organismic Processes for neo-Piagetian theories: A dialectical causal account of cognitive development. International Journal of Psychology 22: 531–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pascual-Leone, Juan, and Janice Johnson. 2005. A dialectical constructivist view of developmental intelligence. In Handbook of Understanding and Measuring Intelligence. Edited by Oliver Wilhelm and Randall W. Engle. Thousand Oaks: Sage, pp. 177–201. [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone, Juan, and Janice Johnson. 2011. A developmental theory of mental attention. In Cognitive Development and Working Memory: A Dialogue between Neo-Piagetian Theories and Cognitive Approaches. Edited by Pierre Barrouillet and Vinciane Gaillard. Hove: Psychology Press, pp. 13–46. [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone, Juan, and Janice Johnson. 2021. The Working Mind: Meaning and Mental Attention in Human Development. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rac-Lubashevsky, Rachel, and Yoav Kessler. 2016. Dissociating working memory updating and automatic updating: The reference-back paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 42: 951–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Redick, Thomas S., and Dakota R. B. Lindsey. 2013. Complex span and n-back measures of working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20: 1102–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Redick, Thomas S., Zach Shipstead, Tyler L. Harrison, Kenny L. Hicks, David E. Fried, David Z. Hambrick, Michael J. Kane, and Randall W. Engle. 2013. No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142: 359–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Röer, Jan P., Raoul Bell, Ulrike Körner, and Axel Buchner. 2018. Equivalent auditory distraction in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 172: 41–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruchkin, Daniel S., Jordan Grafman, Katherine Cameron, and Rita S. Berndt. 2003. Working memory retention systems: A state of activated long-term memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26: 709–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sala, Giovanni, and Fernand Gobet. 2017. Does far transfer exist? Negative evidence from chess, music, and working memory training. Current Directions in Psychological Science 26: 515–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schmiedek, Florian, Andrea Hildebrandt, Martin Lövdén, Oliver Wilhelm, and Ulman Lindenberger. 2009. Complex span versus updating tasks of working memory: The gap is not that deep. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35: 1089–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Shipstead, Zach, Thomas S. RedIck, and Randall W. Engle. 2012. Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin 138: 628–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Soveri, Anna, Jan Antfolk, Linda Karlsson, Benny Salo, and Matti Laine. 2017. Working memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24: 1077–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Traverso, Laura, Paola Viterbori, and Maria Carmen Usai. 2015. Improving executive function in childhood: Evaluation of a training intervention for 5-year-old children. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vergauwe, Evie, Nele Dewaele, Naomi Langerock, and Pierre Barrouillet. 2012. Evidence for a central pool of general resources in working memory. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 24: 359–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Min | Max | Mean | S.D. | Skew | Kurtosis | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Magic House | 4.0 | 18.0 | 11.85 | 3.08 | −0.06 | −0.55 |
Mr. Cucumber | 0.00 | 6.00 | 1.69 | 0.86 | 0.57 | 2.91 |
Backward Word Span | 1.00 | 5.00 | 1.94 | 0.84 | 0.47 | −0.37 |
Direction Following Task | 0.00 | 3.50 | 1.76 | 1.05 | −0.09 | −0.87 |
Magic House | Backward Word Span | Mr. Cucumber | Direction Following Task | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 0.45 *** | 0.68 *** | 0.53 *** | 0.62 *** |
Magic House | 0.44 *** | 0.38 *** | 0.44 *** | |
Backward Word Span | 0.52 *** | 0.59 *** | ||
Mr. Cucumber | 0.38 *** |
M Capacity (Average of Three Working Memory Tasks) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | Total | ||
High | 9(23.9) | 30(24.5) | 21(11.6) | 60 | |
Magic House | Intermediate | 32(30.2) | 32(31.1) | 12(14.7) | 76 |
Low | 29(15.9) | 10(16.4) | 1(7.7) | 40 | |
Total | 70 | 72 | 34 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Panesi, S.; Bandettini, A.; Traverso, L.; Morra, S. On the Relation between the Development of Working Memory Updating and Working Memory Capacity in Preschoolers. J. Intell. 2022, 10, 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10010005
Panesi S, Bandettini A, Traverso L, Morra S. On the Relation between the Development of Working Memory Updating and Working Memory Capacity in Preschoolers. Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(1):5. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10010005
Chicago/Turabian StylePanesi, Sabrina, Alessia Bandettini, Laura Traverso, and Sergio Morra. 2022. "On the Relation between the Development of Working Memory Updating and Working Memory Capacity in Preschoolers" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 1: 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10010005
APA StylePanesi, S., Bandettini, A., Traverso, L., & Morra, S. (2022). On the Relation between the Development of Working Memory Updating and Working Memory Capacity in Preschoolers. Journal of Intelligence, 10(1), 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10010005