Next Article in Journal
Lessons from the Conservatory Model as a Basis for Undergraduate Education and the Development of Intelligence
Previous Article in Journal
Intelligent Academic Specialties Selection in Higher Education for Ukrainian Entrants: A Recommendation System
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Dietary Intake of Polyphenols Enhances Executive/Attentional Functioning and Memory with an Improvement of the Milk Lipid Profile of Postpartum Women from Argentina

by
Agustín Ramiro Miranda
1,2,
Mariela Valentina Cortez
1,2,
Ana Veronica Scotta
1,2 and
Elio Andrés Soria
1,3,*
1
Health Sciences Research Institute, National Scientific and Technical Research Council, Córdoba 5014, Argentina
2
School of Phonoaudiology, Faculty of Medical Sciences, National University of Córdoba, Córdoba 5014, Argentina
3
Chair of Cell Biology, Histology and Embryology, Institute of Cell Biology, National University of Córdoba, Córdoba 5014, Argentina
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Intell. 2022, 10(2), 33; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020033
Submission received: 19 March 2022 / Revised: 28 May 2022 / Accepted: 28 May 2022 / Published: 31 May 2022

Abstract

:
Puerperium may lead to memory and executive/attentional complaints that interfere with women’s daily life. This might be prevented by dietary compounds, such as neuroprotective polyphenols. Their bioactivity depends on their effects on lipid metabolism in different tissues, such as the brain, fat, and breast. Thus, a polyphenol-related cognitive improvement may be associated with changes of lipids in human milk, which are key for infant neurodevelopment. A cross-sectional study was conducted on 75 postpartum women from Córdoba (Argentina), involving several neuropsychological tests. Diet was registered to identify polyphenol intake and food pattern adherence, with sociodemographic and other psychological variables (insomnia, stress, subjective cognitive complaints) being also studied. Triacylglycerols, cholesterol, and their oxidative forms were analyzed as milk biomarkers. Multivariate statistical methods were applied. Results confirmed that women who consumed polyphenols presented better executive/attentional performance (i.e., higher correct responses, conceptual level responses, complete categories, verbal fluency; lower attentional interferences, and perseverative errors) and word retention with lower interference. Polyphenols were positively associated with milk lipids, which were higher in women with better cognition. Furthermore, they had lower oxidized triacylglycerols. In conclusion, polyphenolic intake during postpartum may improve executive/attentional functioning, memory, and milk lipid profile.

1. Introduction

Cognitive functioning during puerperium depends on neuropsychological adaptations with important psychosocial consequences for women, which require special attention in postpartum health care (Carrizo et al. 2020). Furthermore, women’s vulnerability was triggered by the impact on daily life derived from the COVID-19 pandemic, leading to stress, postpartum depression, insomnia, and cognitive impairment (Miranda et al. 2021b; Miranda et al. 2022a).
Executive/attentional functioning involves several processes, such as inhibition, planning, mental shifting, decision taking, updating, and dual-task coordination (Gilsoul et al. 2019; Tull et al. 2012). Puerperium also leads to memory adaptations, involving changes in neural networks and functional connectivity (Bak et al. 2020; Shin et al. 2018). Specific instruments are necessary to assess these changes. Complaints on these spheres compromise different aspects of women’s lives, such as nurture, breastfeeding, work, and study (Miranda et al. 2020b; Scotta et al. 2022). These concerns involve a persistent decline in memory, linguistic, attentional, and executive domains in women with previously normal frontal-executive functioning (Hohman et al. 2011; Mendonça et al. 2016). Thus, it is necessary to study preventative factors that support effective mental health measures.
Among these factors, nutrition is a crucial multitarget determinant of human health and well-being. In this sense, the diet provides several bioactive compounds that can prevent neuropsychological compromise. Polyphenols are promising candidates because they exert a wide range of neurotrophic effects (Miranda et al. 2018). These are heterogeneous compounds grouped into different chemical families, such as flavonoids (e.g., flavonols), and non-flavonoids (e.g., phenolic acids). They are present in plant foods, such as glycosides or aglycones (Singla et al. 2019). Although their bioactivity was thoroughly documented, the relevance of polyphenols as part of a habitual diet is still unclear due to the complexity of human nutrition and metabolism (Luca et al. 2020). Nonetheless, previous research suggested that the dietary intake of certain polyphenol families enhances memory during postpartum (Miranda et al. 2021a). Thus, HJ-Biplot, an innovative methodological approach in nutritional epidemiology, will be applied herein. This statistical method was recently developed to evaluate polyphenols comprehensively and their impact on lactating women (Miranda et al. 2022b).
Dietary polyphenols regulate lipid metabolism, which underlies several molecular mechanisms in the brain, fat, liver, endocrine system, muscles, and gut (Matacchione et al. 2020). Polyphenols are responsible for the Mediterranean diet’s benefits, such as preventing metabolic and neurological diseases that involve low-grade inflammation, oxidative stress, and anomalies in lipid metabolism (Milošević et al. 2021). Nonetheless, their involvement in the mammary gland, which may present unique responsiveness to these compounds, is partially unknown. Certain antioxidant effects on human milk lipids were reported (Poniedziałek et al. 2018; Valls-Bellés et al. 2021). Although the physiological mechanisms remain unclear, they may protect against peroxidative damage-related lipid degradation, upregulate gene expression related to lipid metabolism in the mammary gland, modulate neuroendocrine pathways, and enhance milk lipid clearance (Codini et al. 2020; Downing et al. 2017; Lin et al. 2018). Thus, additional biochemical studies are still needed because lipids are indispensable nutrients for healthy child development (Brink and Lönnerdal 2020).
Polyphenols target molecular pathways of lipid metabolism that are common to the brain and breast (Ehrmann et al. 2002; Senthil et al. 2021). Therefore, changes in the milk content of triacylglycerols and cholesterol may reflect a more complex internal response, which can participate in a cognitive improvement. Moreover, maternal behavior and milk production share neuroendocrine mediators, which determine lactating physiology and cognition (Jurek and Neumann 2018; Kiełbasa et al. 2021). Thus, this work aimed to establish the effects of dietary polyphenols on lactating women, based on the hypothesis that polyphenol intake during postpartum enhances executive/attentional functioning and memory in association with a quantitative and qualitative improvement of the human milk lipids.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Participants

This cross-sectional, analytical study was performed on 75 lactating women from Córdoba province (Argentina), who were interviewed face-to-face by health professionals trained in neuropsychology and nutrition assessment in primary care units of the public health system, the Maternal-Neonatal Hospital “Prof. Dr. Ramón Carrillo” and the Medical Sciences’ facilities of the National University of Cordoba. The inclusion criteria were: adult (≥18 years old), Córdoba inhabitant, postpartum (first six months), and breastfeeding. Exclusion criteria were: pregnancy, alcohol and drug abuse, currently active disease, neuropathology, and psychological conditions (e.g., depression).
Women signed an informed consent to be included voluntarily in this epidemiological study, approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the National Hospital of Clinics (National University of Cordoba), in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and current national legislation. Approval codes were RENIS-IS000548, RENIS-IS001262, RENIS-IS002045 (national registration), REPIS-145, REPIS-2654, and REPIS-5554 (province registration).

2.2. Interview

The assessments were carried out on working days (between 8 a.m. and 6 p.m.) and took place in a quiet test room at a hospital, faculty, or private setting. Participants were individually tested by trained technicians in neuropsychology (for the cognitive assessment) and in nutrition (for the nutritional assessment) in a single session lasting, on average, 120 min. Regarding the cognitive assessment, the participants responded to the tasks in the same order: first the participants answered the Stroop task, then the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, then they were assessed with the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and finally the Verbal Fluency Tasks. The rooms were sound-proofed and light controlled, had adequate ventilation and temperature, and the women were provided with comfortable furniture (chair and desk), as well as a babysitter to take care of their children during the assessment. The neuropsychological assessment was performed before the nutritional assessment.

2.2.1. General Evaluation

The following sociodemographic data were collected using an ad hoc questionnaire: age (years), educational level (<12 or ≥12 years of formal instruction), employment (formally employed or informally employed/unemployed), marital status (single or in a couple), number of births (primiparous or multiparous), gestational age at delivery (<37 weeks -preterm or ≥37 weeks -term-), time of milk sampling (08.00 to 12.00, 12.00 to 16.00, or 16.00 to 20.00), practice of exclusive breastfeeding (yes or no), and postpartum duration (days). In addition, the type of physical activity, minutes and times per week, were registered to calculate the metabolic equivalents (MET) (Roman-Viñas et al. 2010).

2.2.2. Cognitive Assessment

All of the participants were neuropsychologically assessed with a set of validated tests to evaluate attention, language, memory, and executive functions.

Attention

The Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT- Stroop 1935) is an instrument widely used in clinics and research that provides measures of inhibition, selective attention, processing speed, and mental flexibility (Rivera et al. 2015). Each one of its three sheets has 100 randomly organized elements in five columns. The first two sheets represent the “congruent condition” in which the participants should read the names of the “red”, “green”, and “blue” colors printed in black ink (first sheet “word”-W-) and name the ink color (red, green, and blue) in which “xxxx” (second sheet, “color”-C-) are printed. On the contrary, the “incongruous condition” is presented in the third sheet (“word-color”-WC-), in which the words of color are printed with a different color ink on the name of the color (for example, the word “blue” is printed with red ink) (Scarpina and Tagini 2017). For each sheet, participants were instructed to read from top to bottom and from left to right as quickly as possible, and the score derived from the number of items correctly appointed in 45 s. An interference rate (I) was calculated as I = WC − [(W × C)/(W + C)] (Rivera et al. 2015; Scarpina and Tagini 2017). Successful execution on the third page requires that subjects intentionally concentrate their attention to an aspect of the stimulus (print color), while inhibiting attention from the other (meaning of the word). SCWT has good psychometric properties for Latin American Spanish speakers (Rodríguez Barreto et al. 2016).

Memory

The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) for Spanish speakers was used to assess episodic memory (Ferreira Correia and Campagna Osorio 2014). This widely known test, developed by Rey (1958), is considered easy and quick to administer. RAVLT consists of presenting orally a list of 15 unrelated words in five consecutive trials (Burin et al. 2007). Then, a new list is read with 15 distractors (List B), and the person must remember them immediately. Next, participants are required to remember the words of the first list (Trial A6-postinterference free recall-). After 30 min, the participant must again remember the words of the first list (Trial A7-long term delayed free recall-), and finally identify them on a printed sheet with 75 words (visual recognition). For this study, a set of traditional and novel scores were obtained from RAVLT (Alviarez-Schulze et al. 2022; Miranda et al. 2021a; Tyagi et al. 2021).

Executive Functions

The manual version of the Wisconsin Card Classification Test (WCST) was used to assess executive functioning (De la Cruz 2001; Heaton et al. 1993). This task-switching instrument originally developed by Berg (1948), consists of 4 reference cards and 128 response cards, which are divided into two decks of 64 cards each. The cards contain geometric figures, which vary in shape (crosses, circles, triangles, or stars), color (red, blue, yellow, or green), and number (one, two, three, or four). The participant must match the cards with one of the four reference cards. The sorting criterion is not indicated, and after the subject’s response, the evaluator indicates whether the card was correctly or incorrectly matched, but still does not reveal the principle of correct classification. Therefore, this forces the subject to repeat the responses until he/she discovers the classification rule. Once the subject has correctly classified ten consecutive cards, the criterion changes without prior notice, so that the subject must modify their classification criteria to identify the new one. Thus, this instrument is widely used to measure higher-level cognitive processes related to the prefrontal cortex (e.g., attention, perseverance, working memory, abstract reasoning, cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and set shifting) (Faustino et al. 2021). For this purpose, traditional WCST scores were used for the analysis of executive functions (Faustino et al. 2020). This instrument proved to be reliable and valid for the study of executive functions with local normative data for Spanish speakers (Miranda et al. 2020a).

Language

Linguistic functioning was evaluated using four verbal fluency tasks (VFT): two phonological tasks with direct instructions (initial letter P and F), one semantics (animals), and one phonological with indirect instructions (words without “A”- excluded letter A-). VFT was systematized by Borkowski et al. (1967). The application of the VFT followed the procedure proposed by Marino and Alderete (2010). The time allotted for each test was one minute. All of the words were recorded on a spreadsheet and each correct answer was scored with one point. Intrusions (words that do not belong to the required semantic field or do not comply with the phonological instruction) and perseverations (the repetition of a previous response) were considered incorrect (Lezak et al. 2012). VFTs are a useful tool to evaluate different capacities and cognitive functions related to language, such as lexical-semantic memory and executive/attentional processes (Cardoso Silva de Souza et al. 2020). VFTs have adequate psychometric properties and there are normative data available for several VFTs for Spanish speakers (Marino and Alderete 2010).

2.2.3. Nutritional Assessment

A validated food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) was applied to record the usual diet during the last 12 months. The FFQ comprises 127 foods available in the country, grouped according to their nutritional profile and origin. Questions were about frequency (times per month/week/day, as appropriate) and usual portion size of each food consumed (three categories: small, medium, and large). This instrument has shown adequate levels of validity and reproducibility for the studied population (Cortez et al. 2021). A validated photographic atlas was used, based on the standard portion sizes in Argentina to help participants describe the amount of food consumed. Next, data were analyzed using the Phenol-Explorer database (version 3.6) (Rothwell et al. 2013). This database provides values for more than 500 different polyphenols in more than 400 foods in different forms, including raw, cooked, and processed foods. The intake was calculated as mg/day. Only polyphenols with a medium daily intake ≥5 mg/d were considered for the analyses (Kesse-Guyot et al. 2012). This cut off point was previously demonstrated to present health effects (Miranda et al. 2022b).
Nutritional assessment included anthropometric measures: self-reported height (meters) and weight (kilograms) were recorded to calculate body mass index (BMI); and body fat percentage (BFP) was measured by hand-held bioelectrical impedance analysis (Cortez et al. 2021).

2.3. Milk Sampling and Lipid Measurement

The sampling protocol of Cortez and Soria (2016) was followed. Ten mL of human milk was delivered by each woman by manual expression into sterile collection bottles. Samples were immediately stored at −20 °C for transportation, and then at −80 °C to avoid lipid deterioration by enzymatic activity until the analyses (George et al. 2018). Triacylglycerols and cholesterol were measured by spectrophotometry at 540 nm, using a Promega’s GloMax Multi Microplate Multimode Reader (USA).

2.3.1. Triacylglycerols

Triacylglycerols were estimated by a widely used enzymatic/chromogenic method provided by a GT Lab commercial kit under the manufacturer’s instructions (GPO/PAP, Triglicéridos Liquid PlusTM, GT Lab, Rosario, Argentina), who established the corresponding analytical conditions. The assay involves the following enzymes: lipase (which hydrolyzes triglycerides to glycerol and fatty acids); glycerol kinase (which phosphorylates to glycerol, forming glycerol-1-phosphate); glycerol phosphate oxidase (which oxidizes to glycerol 1-phosphate, forming dihydroxyacetone-phosphate and hydrogen peroxide); and peroxidase (which in the presence of 4-aminophenazone forms a pink color quinonimine from hydrogen peroxide) (Kumari et al. 2020). Briefly, 3 parts of milk were mixed with 300 parts of a reactant solution for 5 min at 37 °C. Triacylglycerol content per liter (g/L) was then calculated by measuring the reaction at 540 nm and using the corresponding standard.

2.3.2. Cholesterol

Milk cholesterol concentration was estimated using an enzymatic/colorimetric method provided by a GT Lab kit, following the manufacturer’s instructions (CHOD/PAP, Colesterol Liquid PlusTM, GT Lab, Rosario, Argentina), who established the corresponding analytical conditions. Firstly, cholesterol esters are hydrolyzed by cholesterol esterase. Subsequently, the free cholesterol is oxidized by cholesterol oxidase to Δ4-cholestenone. Under peroxidase action, the hydrogen peroxide produced in the previous reaction oxidizes the chromophore 4-aminophenazone, in the presence of phenol, to the red dye compound 4-(p-benzoquinone-monoimino)-phenazone (Kumari et al. 2020). It was measured at 540 nm to calculate mg/L using the corresponding standard, after 3 parts of milk and 300 parts of a reactant solution were mixed for 5 min at 37 °C.

2.3.3. Oxidized Lipids

Lipoperoxides are products of the oxidative damage of fatty acids and cholesterol in cell membranes and lipoproteins (Elisia and Kitts 2011). To determine their concentration in human milk, a commercial kit was used, according to the manufacturer’s instructions (PeroxiDetectTM, Sigma-Aldrich, Saint Louis, MO, USA), who established the corresponding analytical conditions. One part of HM, 0.1 parts of 25 mM ferrous ammonium sulfate (prepared in 2.5 M sulfuric acid), and 10 parts of a reactant (125 μM xylenol orange with 4 mM butylated hydroxytoluene) were mixed for 30 min at 37 °C to reveal the optical density of lipoperoxides at 540 nm per liter of milk (OD/L). For the lipid hydroperoxides, the measuring procedure was similar to that of lipoperoxides, but the reactant was replaced by 125 μM xylenol orange in 100 mM sorbitol. Given that triacylglycerols are the form of storage and transportation of the principal targets of lipid peroxidation (polyunsaturated fatty acids), the quotient between lipoperoxides and triacylglycerols were calculated as OD/g (Pérez-Rodríguez et al. 2015). On the other hand, oxysterols are oxidized cholesterol derivatives that can be generated in the human organism through different oxidation processes. The quotient between the lipoperoxides and cholesterol indicated non-polar oxysterols (e.g., esterified cholesterol), while the hydroperoxides/cholesterol quotient indicated polar oxysterols (e.g., non-esterified cholesterol or lipoprotein) (Brown and Jessup 2009), with the level of oxysterols being reported as OD/mg.

2.4. Other Measures

2.4.1. Maternal Insomnia

The Spanish version of the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) was used to evaluate the nature, severity, and impact of insomnia during the last month (Morin et al. 2011). This seven item self-report instrument is a traditional questionnaire used in insomnia research (Manzar et al. 2021). Each item was responded using a five-point Likert scale (0 to 4, where 0 = no problem and 4 = very severe problem) and the scores were transformed on a scale ranging from 0 to 28, with the highest scores indicating more severe insomnia and impact on quality of life. In the current study, the ISI showed good reliability (McDonald’s omega coefficient = 0.733).

2.4.2. Maternal Adherence to Dietary Patterns

Adherence to dietary patterns was evaluated using dietary indices for Argentinian lactating women. Adherence was calculated using three indices, based on the patterns identified by Cortez et al. (2021), which includes the following:
  • Macro-nutritional Dietary Index (MDI): Snacks; processed meats; legumes; dairy products; cheese; whole grains; and candies;
  • Phytochemical Dietary Index (PDI): vegetables A (5% of carbohydrate content); vegetables B (10% of carbohydrate content); fruits; and fatty fruits;
  • Energetic Dietary Index (EDI): vegetables C (20% of carbohydrate content); animal fats; vegetable oils; sugary drinks; and refined grains.
The dietary intake of each group was divided into deciles, with each decile being scored from 1 (lowest) to 10 (highest), whereas no intake received 0 points. The scoring system was adjusted for food groups with low intake frequency (i.e., snacks). Finally, points were totaled: MDI ranged from 0–41 points; PDI from 0–35; and EDI from 0–39, with higher scores indicating greater dietary pattern adherence.
In order to evaluate the quality of fats, carbohydrates, and proteins in the maternal diet, the Fat Quality Index (FQI) and the Protein to Carbohydrate Ratio (PCR) were calculated (Miranda et al. 2022b).

2.4.3. Subjective Memory Complaints

Participants responded to a Spanish version of the Memory Failures of Everyday questionnaire (MFE) (Lozoya-Delgado et al. 2012), which explores subjective memory complaints and related cognitive processes, such as attention, perceptual recognition, and language (Rodríguez-Blázquez et al. 2022). This instrument consists of 30 items about memory failures during situations and activities that take place in everyday life, which must be rated using a five-point Likert scale (0 to 4, 0 = never and 4 = always). In this study, MFE showed excellent reliability (McDonald’s omega coefficient = 0.924).

2.4.4. Subjective Executive Complaints

The Executive Complaint Questionnaire (ECQ) was used (Mías 2010). This questionnaire explores the subjective complaints of executive functions. The questionnaire has 15 items and participants must judge to what extent a series of daily life behaviors concerning mental functioning occur (Miranda et al. 2020c). Each item was rated on a five-point Likert scale (0 to 4, where 0 = never and 4 = always). In this work, it was found that the ECQ has a very good reliability (McDonald’s omega coefficient = 0.821).

2.5. Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using Infostat (version 2020, InfoStat Group, Córdoba, Argentina) and Stata (version 15, StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA). Mean, standard deviation (SD), median, 25th, and 75th percentiles were calculated for numerical variables, and percentages for categorical ones. Skewness (S) and kurtosis (K) were used to assess the distribution of the neuropsychological scores. McDonald’s omega coefficient was estimated to assess the reliability of self-report questionnaires.
HJ-Biplot multivariate analyses were performed to discover the correlations between polyphenols, as well as cognitive scores. This technique provides a graphical representation of the rows and columns of a data matrix in a low-dimensional subspace where their relative positions are interpretable (Frutos Bernal et al. 2020). The markers are obtained from the singular value decomposition of the data matrix, where the rows represent individuals (women), and the columns variables (polyphenols or cognitive scores), so that both sets of markers can be overlaid in the same reference frame with the highest quality of representation. The HJ-Biplot graphic representation must be interpreted based on the following criteria (Carrasco et al. 2019):
  • The distances between points are interpreted as an inverse function of their similarities;
  • The vector length approximates the variable standard deviation;
  • The cosines of the angles between the vectors approximate the correlations between them, and the cosines of the angles between vectors and axes approximate the correlations between both;
  • The order of the orthogonal projections of the points onto a vector approximates the order of those points in that vector.
Clustering analyses, based on coordinates using the K-means method, were conducted in the HJ-Biplot models (Nieto-Librero et al. 2017). This multivariate classification technique enables the detection and description of clusters of subjects or homogeneous variables, based on the values observed within a heterogeneous set, trying to achieve the maximum possible homogeneity in each group and the significant differences between them. Similarity calculation between the centroids and data points (i.e., women) was based on cosine similarity (Carrasco et al. 2019). Contributions and qualities of representation (QLR) for each variable and cluster on the factorial plane were calculated as data fit measures. It was shown that the HJ-Biplot is a valid tool for the study of nutrition and cognitive functioning (Miranda et al. 2022b; Cadavid Ruiz et al. 2018). The HJ-Biplot analysis was performed using the MULTBIPLOT software package (multivariate analysis using BIPLOT) (Vicente-Villardón 2015).
Analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) with Fisher’s post hoc comparisons were used to test the differences in cognitive performance among the polyphenolic clusters. In addition, ANCOVA was used to analyze the effect of dietary polyphenols and cognitive status on lipids in human milk. All of the models were adjusted by potential confounders (β). The sets of potential confounders for each model were selected according to directed acyclic graphs, using the criterion of minimal sufficient adjustment for direct effects (Figure 1) (Miranda et al. 2021a). Values of 0 and 1 were adopted to establish the absence and presence, respectively, of the covariates introduced in the ANCOVA models, which combines an analysis of variance to the data and a multiple linear regression to the data to eliminate the effect of the covariates. Moreover, a dichotomous factor can be entered into a regression equation by formulating a dummy regressor with a 0/1 codification (Fox 2015). The use of several covariates reduces the variability of the data, thus increasing the statistical power, with models being fitted appropriately in accordance with sample size and prior criteria (Shieh 2020). Coefficients of determinations of the model (R2), means, standard errors, p-values (for comparisons), and Cohen’s d coefficients were reported. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d, which were interpreted as small (≥0.20), medium (≥0.50), large (≥0.80), and very large (≥1.30). Finally, post hoc analyses showed that the sample size in this study achieved more than 80% power with an alpha of 0.05, taking 8 predictors in the ANCOVA models and size effects of 0.45. The homogeneity of variance among the groups was tested by Levene’s test (Mishra et al. 2019).

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

Maternal age averaged 29.92 years (SD = 6.13). Most women were in a couple (93.33%), and had finished at least 12 years of formal education (75.66%). Unemployment or informal employment affected 54.66% of the participants. Regarding reproductive variables, the mean postpartum days were 95.04 (SD = 52.46), with a pregnancy duration of 38.37 weeks (SD = 2.05). Cesarean sections were presented in 55% of the sample, and 60% of the women were multiparous. The percentage of participants who practiced exclusive breastfeeding was 53.33%. In relation to the nutrition status, women had a BMI of 24.79 (5.62) Kg/m2 and a BFP of 28.88 (7.00) %, while they exhibited 384.96 (SD = 686.33) MET of physical activity. Means of dietary adherence were similar: 25.25 (SD = 6.85) for MDI, 21.89 (SD = 7.18) for PDI, and 22.81 (SD = 7.71) for EDI (Table 1).
Table 2 describes the WCST performances. All of the scores showed levels of skewness and kurtosis ± 3, suggesting a normal distribution, except for trials to complete the first category, and the percentage of perseverative errors. Similarly, most scores in the RAVLT showed a normal approximation, except for the amount of intrusion errors, percentage of forgetting, and retroactive interference (Table 3). All of the SCWT and VFT scores showed acceptable values for skewness and kurtosis (Table 4).

3.2. Diet Characteristics

Thirty-six polyphenolic compounds were consumed at levels above 5 mg/d (Figure 2), including the following families: hydroxycinnamic acids (50%); flavanols (19.44%); flavanones (8.33%); hydroxybenzoic acids (8.33%); flavonols (5.55%); lignans (5.55%); and anthocyanin (2.77%). Figure 3 shows the HJ-Biplot graphics to visually represent the polyphenolic intake models in the factorial plane 1–2. The first model included those polyphenols consumed above 20 mg/d, and explained the 79.3% of variance (Figure 3a). These compounds showed adequate levels of contributions to the plane 1–2, and were represented with adequate quality (QLR > 200) (Supplementary Materials: Table S1). Three women clusters were identified in this model: C1 (women with lower polyphenolic intake, n = 34); C2 (women with higher intake of different hydroxycinnamic acids and one flavonol, n = 24); and C3 (women with higher intake of two hydroxycinnamic acids, one lignan, and one flavanone, n = 17). The qualities of representation for each cluster on the plane 1–2 exceed the value of 95%.
On the other hand, Figure 3b shows the HJ-Biplot representation on the main first plane for the polyphenols consumed between 5 and 20 mg/d, which explained the 50.19% of variance. Most of these compounds showed adequate representation qualities, except for naringenin, eriodyctiol, syringic, ellagic acid, and trans-ferulic acid (QLR < 150) (Supplementary Materials: Table S2). Three clusters were formed with qualities of representation above 90%: C1 (women with lower polyphenolic intake, n = 33); C2 (women with higher intake of different hydroxycinnamic acids, one hydroxybenzoic acid, and one lignan, n = 29); and C3 (women with higher intake of different flavanols and one hydroxybenzoic acid, n = 13).

3.3. Effects of Polyphenols on Women’s Cognition

For polyphenols > 20 mg/d, women from C3 had a significantly higher number of correct responses, conceptual level responses, percentage of conceptual level responses, and categories achieved in the WCST, with respect to C1. Moreover, C3 had lower total errors, percentage of total errors, non-perseverative responses, percentage of non-perseverative responses, and trials to complete the first category. All of these significant differences on the WCST scores presented moderate effect sizes (Cohen’s d ≥ 0.50) (Figure 4).
Figure 5 shows the results for the clusters > 20 mg/d on RAVLT, SCWT, and VFT. In this sense, women from C1 scored lower on forgetting speed and retention when compared with C2, but higher on attentional interference (SCWT). In addition, C1 showed higher means of proactive and retroactive interference than C3. Women from C1 produced significantly fewer words on the VFT letter F than the other ones. The effect sizes were moderate. Regarding adjustment variables, educational level was associated with all of the cognitive measures (p-values < 0.05). In addition, parity (primiparous > multiparous) and EDI (β = 0.04) were related, respectively, to Stroop interference and retroactive interference (p = 0.008 for both), whilst the number of trials to complete the first category was associated with MDI (β = −1.45, p = 0.0059) and EDI (β = 0.84, p = 0.0331).
There were also significant differences among the clusters for the polyphenols consumed between 5 to 20 mg/d (Figure 6). In this regard, C1 had lower scores in WCST than C3: correct responses; conceptual level responses; and failure to maintain set. Furthermore, C1 showed higher retroactive interference (RAVLT) and attentional interference (SCWT) than C3. Cluster 1 also differed from C2, which presented lower scores on color and word-color tasks, with a small effect size. Most effect sizes were medium. Concerning adjustment variables, educational level was associated with all of the cognitive measures (p < 0.05), parity with Stroop interference (primiparous > multiparous, p = 0.007), and EDI with retroactive interference (β = 0.04, p = 0.007).

3.4. Cognitive Profiles of Postpartum Women

First, the measures of executive/attentional functioning were analyzed together, using the HJ-Biplot technique (Figure 7a). By retaining two axes, the explained variance was 41.48% for the first axis and 16.16% for the second one, which indicated that this model explained over 50% of the data variability. Representation contributions and qualities indicated that the best subspace to interpret results was the plane 1–2, with all of the elements showing good representation (QLR > 200) (Supplementary Materials: Table S3). Three clusters were identified with good quality:
  • Cluster 1 or “higher executive/attentional performance”: This conglomerate represented 49.33% (n = 37) of the sample and was comprised of women with better scores on positive measures (e.g., conceptual level responses, learning to learn, and categories achieved). This conglomerate had a good quality of representation at the 1–2 plane (QLR = 99%);
  • Cluster 2 or “lower attentional set control”: This conglomerate (32.00% of the sample, n = 24) was comprised of women with low scoring in the SCWT and with high scores in the negative WCST indices (e.g., global score and trials to complete the first category), and had an adequate quality of representation (QLR = 98%);
  • Cluster 3 or “lower inhibition and flexibility”: Fourteen women (18.67%) comprised this cluster, and presented lower scores of inhibitory control and behavioral flexibility in the WCST (i.e., higher perseverative responses, perseverative errors, and failures to maintain set). Its representation quality was good with a QLR of 87%.
Figure 7b shows the HJ-Biplot representation of memory performance. The variance explained by the first axis was 34.72%, and 14.38% by the second one. In addition, contributions and qualities of all of the RAVLT and FVT scores confirmed that this model should be interpreted at the 1–2 plane (Supplementary Materials: Table S4). Two clusters were identified: Cluster 1 or “higher memory”, and Cluster 2 or “lower memory”, which contained 54.67% (n = 41) and 45.33% (n = 34) of the women, respectively. Both clusters showed a good quality of representation in the plane 1–2 (QLR of 99% for both).

3.5. Human Milk Lipids: Effects of Dietary Polyphenols and Cognitive Status

Finally, the ANCOVA models for the human milk lipids according to the polyphenol intake clusters are displayed in Table 5. Levene’s test (p > 0.05) indicated that the variances between the groups were statistically equal for all of the chemical variables. Regarding polyphenols between 5 and 20 mg/d, C3 had a significantly higher concentration of triacylglycerols than C1 (30.52 (2.79) versus 23.76 (1.87) g/L, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.17). In addition, C2 showed higher levels of cholesterol (113.71 (11.46) versus 80.04 (10.85) mg/L, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.17). These results had medium size effects (Cohen’s d > 0.50). No other significant differences were found. Moreover, no other statistically significant associations were found between polyphenol intake and human milk lipids or adjustment variables.
On the other hand, outcomes revealed that women with higher executive/attentional functioning (Cluster 1) had higher milk concentration of triacylglycerols (28.53 (1.71) g/L) than those with lower inhibition and behavioral flexibility (21.90 (8.84) g/L) and lower attentional set control (22.94 (1.98) g/L) (p < 0.05, R2 = 0.20). In addition, Cluster 1 showed a lower level of oxidized triacylglycerols (189.17 (103.55) OD/g) when compared with Cluster 3 (640.97 (172.12) OD/g) (p < 0.05, R2 = 0.22). In respect of women with better memory functioning, they showed more triacylglycerols (27.69 (1.60) g/L vs 22.83 (1.77) g/L, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.17) and lower oxidized triacylglycerols (194.52 (27.69) OD/g vs. 281.28 (30.85) OD/g, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.15). As can be seen in Table 6, the effects were medium in size in the executive-attentional functioning model (Cohen’s d > 0.50) and small in the memory model (Cohen’s d > 0.20). Concerning the variables that were included for the adjustment of ANCOVA models, insomnia was negatively related to milk cholesterol content in the executive/attentional functioning model (β = −3.13, p = 0.04, R2 = 0.22) and the memory model (β = −2.70, p = 0.07, R2 = 0.17). No other statistically significant associations were found between executive and attentional functioning and human milk lipids or adjustment variables.
Additionally, the analyses of human milk lipids according to extraction time, gestational age at delivery, breastfeeding frequency, and dietary macronutrients are displayed in Table S5 (Supplementary Materials). No associations were found in these ANCOVA models.

4. Discussion

Results showed marked interindividual variability in the daily intake of polyphenols, which were associated with better executive/attentional functioning and memory performance in postpartum women. In addition, these dietary compounds significantly correlated with the lipid profile of breast milk, with higher triacylglycerols and cholesterol concentrations. Furthermore, women with better cognitive status showed a better quantity and quality of these nutrients. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that integrates the effect of maternal diet and cognition on human milk composition. Taken together, these results support the stated hypothesis.
In this work, we used valid instruments to measure executive/attentional functioning and memory, to include their scores in statistical analyses (Henríquez et al. 2022). Most of them showed a normal distribution, confirming their adequacy for multivariate analyses (Aminu and Shariff 2014).
The nutritional assessment reported that 36 polyphenols presented a mean intake above 5 mg/d. Hydroxycinnamic acids represented the most ingested family, in concordance with previous research about the South American diet. This diet is composed of foods and beverages rich in hydroxycinnamic acids, lignans, and some flavonoids (Carnauba et al. 2021; Rossi et al. 2018).
Nutrition regulates physiological pathways via synergistic nutrient effects (Margină et al. 2020). Polyphenols with cognitive effects were hesperetin, lariciresinol, caffeic acid, and ferulic acid (for those consumed above 20 mg/d) and gallated flavonoids (for those consumed between 5 and 20 mg/d). Women who consumed these compounds showed higher scores in executive function, attention, memory, and language. These results are supported by studies about the cognitive benefits of polyphenols in other populations. In this sense, middle-aged women’s verbal memory is enhanced by the intake of lignans (Greendale et al. 2012), which showed brain bioaccumulation and prefrontal improvement (Kreijkamp-Kaspers et al. 2007; Wang et al. 2018).
On the other hand, flavonoids are neuroprotective by increasing blood flow and strengthening brain pathways related to executive functioning, learning, and memory (Bakoyiannis et al. 2019; Williamson et al. 2018; Kennedy et al. 2017). In the current study, hesperetin, a flavanone, belonged to the cluster associated with better cognitive performance. It was shown that hesperetin, naringenin, and their metabolites, are lipophilic flavonoids that cross hematic barriers (Bakoyiannis et al. 2019). In addition, hesperetin reduces lipid peroxidation (Moghaddam and Zare 2018). In addition to this antioxidant effect, it exerts positive effects on synaptic plasticity in cortical and hippocampal neurons (Vauzour et al. 2007). Only very few clinical trials have established the cognitive effects of hesperetin, which enhances cerebral blood flow and cognition (Salehi et al. 2021).
Only a small amount of information encoded in the nervous system is consolidated and stored by the long-term memory, whereas the rest is forgotten through passive and active mechanisms (Davis and Zhong 2017). In this work, women with low intake of polyphenols presented more interference than women with a higher intake. Memory interference occurs when additional information competes before, after, or during the coding of target information (Crawford et al. 2020). Lariciresinol, hesperetin, and caffeic acid were inversely related to proactive and retroactive interference (moderate effect sizes), with the latter being also associated with gallated flavonoids. Proactive interference occurs when previous knowledge hinders the ability to acquire new information, while retroactive interference refers to the effect of new learning on previously established knowledge, impairing memory consolidation (Crawford et al. 2020). Consequently, retroactive interference is more frequent and harmful (Edwards 2010). There is little evidence about the dietary polyphenolic effect on interference phenomena. Some data show that the intake polyphenolic sources improve memory consolidation by decreasing interference (Bell and Williams 2019) and diet-induced neuroplasticity in postpartum women (Xavier et al. 2021). Other studies found non-significant results (Lamport et al. 2016a; Lamport et al. 2016b) or contradictory ones (Whyte and Williams 2015). Nevertheless, polyphenols attenuate interference and promote cognition and attention (Crawford et al. 2020), which were also improved by these compounds in the present study.
Certain hydroxycinnamic acids formed the cluster with the greatest cognitive outcome, together with hesperetin (flavanone) and lariciresinol (lignan). However, the highly consumed chlorogenic acids, other hydroxycinnamic compounds, were less involved. In terms of cerebral function, there is little evidence about their effects on humans (Coman and Vodnar 2020), with contradictory data (Cropley et al. 2012; Camfield et al. 2013). The cognitive effects of chlorogenic acids imply mood improvement and acetylcholinesterase downregulation (Bouayed et al. 2007; Kwon et al. 2010). This evidence could partially explain the weak outcomes in postpartum women with a higher intake of chlorogenic acids. Conversely, the ferulic and caffeic acids exert well-established biological effects in the brain, supporting the results of the current work. These pro-cognitive effects depend on their higher intestinal absorption, redox-regulating activity, lipoxygenase inhibition, and glial/neuronal protection (Coman and Vodnar 2020; Singh et al. 2021; Habtemariam 2017).
Catechins were related to better cognition. They are classified into gallated and non-gallated catechins, whether they have gallic moieties or not. The gallated ones achieve more bioavailability (Chu and Pang 2018), with epigallocatechin gallate being anxiolytic, anti-amyloidogenic, and a promoter of memory, social cognition, and attention (Wightman et al. 2012). These benefits depend on their effects on brain tissue, such as antioxidant activity, improvement of parietal-frontal connectivity, neuroplasticity, and neuroreceptor modulation (Mancini et al. 2017).
Multiple effects of polyphenols on human milk and child health were described (Ríos et al. 2021). We discovered that these dietary compounds (polyphenols consumed at 5 to 20 mg/d) were positively associated with milk triacylglycerols and cholesterol, with low polyphenol intake being related to milk values below expected averages (Koletzko 2016). Polyphenols modulate lipid metabolism and adipokine secretion (Boccellino and D’Angelo 2020; Carpene et al. 2015). Experimental maternal models found metabolic and genetic effects. In this sense, polyphenols trigger breast tissue-specific gene upregulation, fatty acid bioavailability, lipogenesis, and adiponectin pathway (Caimari et al. 2017). Nonetheless, these mechanisms remain unknown in women, and in human milk.
Triacylglycerols are the main energetic component in human milk (Demmelmair and Koletzko 2018), which is produced by the mammary gland from circulating fatty acids and de novo synthesis to form fat globules (Yang et al. 2018). Numerous factors modify these processes (Yao et al. 2020), such as maternal genetics, environment, and diet (Yang et al. 2018). Adipokines and PPAR regulation determinate lipid homeostasis and inflammatory response to maintain human milk quality (Yang et al. 2018). Moreover, polyphenols target these pathways (Farràs et al. 2013). Thus, the higher lipid level found in women who consumed flavonoids may respond to these complex mechanisms (Caimari et al. 2017; Rabadan-Chávez et al. 2016; McManaman 2014).
The pharmacological potential of polyphenols, such as procyanidin, is applied against dyslipidemia (Rufino et al. 2021; Downing et al. 2017). Feng et al. (2019) also found that epigallocatechin gallate, kaempferol, and quercetin are bioactive on cholesterol metabolism by multiple gene regulation. Moreover, ellagic acid induces lipid metabolism (Kubota et al. 2019), with vegetable-based foods triggering lipid synthesis in breast tissue in an animal model (Palin et al. 2014). Lignans (lariciresinol, pinoresinol) and phenolic acids also contribute (Herchi et al. 2014; Zhong et al. 2021). Quercetin induces sensitivity to prolactin and the expression of stearoyl-CoA desaturase and fatty acid synthase in the murine mammary gland (Lin et al. 2018).
There is growing interest in the effect of psychological factors on breastfeeding (Fallon et al. 2016). However, few studies evaluated the impact of mental health on milk composition (Ziomkiewicz et al. 2021). Our study established that women with better executive/attentional functioning and memory showed a better lipid profile, characterized by increased triacylglycerols with lower oxidation. Ziomkiewicz et al. (2021) found that maternal response to stress is associated with neuroendocrine alterations and breast milk changes. Moreover, chronic stress leads to lipid oxidation (Troubat et al. 2009), with breastfeeding and cortisol levels responding to relaxation interventions (Mohd Shukri et al. 2019).
An inverse association existed between milk cholesterol concentration and insomnia severity. Given that dietary intake and fat storage were considered in multivariate analyses (confounding control), endogenous mechanisms are suggested. Lipids reach human milk from plasma and glandular biosynthesis by insomnia-sensitive mechanisms (Ontsouka and Albrecht 2014; Mohammad and Haymond 2013; Andreas et al. 2015; Fu et al. 2015; Casey et al. 2014). Although cholesterol was within a normal range, our results proposed that severe insomnia could compromise milk composition.
A limitation of this work is that specific mood indicators, which may modify milk composition, were not included (Kawano and Emori 2015; Stuebe et al. 2012). Nevertheless, clinical aspects of mood disorders, such as insomnia, were considered. Another possible limitation can be the sample size and the cross-sectional design. Although potential confounders were adjusted with adequate statistical power, longitudinal studies should replicate this research in larger samples. Although the timing of milk extraction was not significantly associated, milk chronobiology is of interest. Neuropsychological assessment between 8 a.m. and 6 p.m. may cause time-of-day bias (Venkat et al. 2020). Thus, future studies should control this factor. Some of the results have to be interpreted with caution. Future in-depth research should consider collecting milk samples at different time points (i.e., colostrum, transitional milk, and mature milk) with standardized sample collection (e.g., foremilk, hindmilk, complete breast expression, expression time). Specific analyses for human milk lipidomic are also encouraged (e.g., gas chromatography, mass spectrometry, NMR spectroscopy) (George et al. 2018).
Finally, our results reinforce the current perspective to approach puerperal health. Women’s mental health determines self-efficacy, and impacts breastfeeding behaviors and the composition of human milk (Fallon et al. 2016). Nutrient transference to this fluid is essential for the healthy growth and development of children. A complex interaction of psycho-,neuro-,immuno-endocrine responses regulates the maternal brain and mammary gland (Jonas and Woodside 2016).

5. Conclusions

In this study, polyphenolic intake during puerperium promoted two relevant aspects of the health of lactating women from Córdoba, Argentina. The first one was that the higher executive/attentional functioning and memory were related to the intake of different families of dietary polyphenols. The second one was a positive association with the milk content of triacylglycerols and cholesterol, crucial lipids for healthy infant growth and development. These findings suggest that human milk lipids can be modulated by certain dietary polyphenols, whose intake of 5–20 mg/d appears to be more suitable for future nutritional interventions. Results, therefore, supported a better cognitive status and lipid profile, which were related. Women with better cognition showed higher triacylglycerols concentration in milk with less peroxidation. In conclusion, maternal polyphenolic intake may improve executive/attentional functioning, memory, and milk lipid profile. Future studies with larger samples, repeated measures, and different methods are needed to validate these findings. The current work constitutes an innovative approach by integrating the assessment of biological, nutritional, and psychological issues of women’s health during this vital stage, and may contribute to understanding the complex nature of human milk.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jintelligence10020033/s1, Table S1: Contributions and representation qualities for each polyphenol consumed above 20 mg/d on the first two axes; Table S2: Contributions and representation qualities for each polyphenol consumed from 5 to 20 mg/d on the first two axes; Table S3: Contributions and representation qualities for each executive/attentional score on the first two axes; Table S4: Contributions and representation qualities for each memory score on the first two axes; Table S5: Human milk lipids according to extraction time, gestational age at delivery, breastfeeding frequency, and dietary macronutrients.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.R.M. and E.A.S.; methodology, A.R.M., M.V.C. and A.V.S.; software, A.R.M.; validation, A.R.M. and A.V.S.; formal analysis, A.R.M.; investigation, A.R.M., M.V.C. and A.V.S.; resources, E.A.S.; data curation, A.R.M., M.V.C. and A.V.S.; writing—original draft preparation, A.R.M.; writing—review and editing, A.R.M., M.V.C., A.V.S., and E.A.S.; visualization, A.R.M.; supervision, E.A.S.; project administration, E.A.S.; funding acquisition, E.A.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Funding was provided by the Secretaría de Ciencia y Tecnología, Universidad Nacional de Córdoba [(grant numbers SECYT-UNC 411/2018)], and Agencia Nacional de Promoción Científica y Tecnológica [(grant numbers PICT-2016-2846 RESOL-2017-285-APN-DANPCYT#MCT)].

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the National Hospital of Clinics (National University of Cordoba), in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and current national legislation. Approval codes were RENIS-IS000548, RENIS-IS001262, RENIS-IS002045 (national registration), REPIS-145, REPIS-2654, and REPIS-5554 (province registration). Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions.

Acknowledgments

ARM and AVS were supported by doctoral fellowships of Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, and MVC was supported by a postdoctoral fellowship of Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. The authors gratefully acknowledge the participating women.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Alviarez-Schulze, Vanessa, Gabriele Cattaneo, Catherine Pachón-García, Javier Solana-Sánchez, Josep M. Tormos, Alvaro Pascual-Leone, and David Bartrés-Faz. 2022. Validation and Normative Data of the Spanish Version of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test and Associated Long-Term Forgetting Measures in Middle-Aged Adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Aminu, Ibrahim Murtala, and Mohd Noor Mohd Shariff. 2014. Strategic orientation, access to finance, business environment and SMEs performance in Nigeria: Data screening and preliminary analysis. European Journal of Business and Management 6: 124–32. [Google Scholar]
  3. Andreas, N. J., B. Kampmann, and K. Mehring Le-Doare. 2015. Human breast milk: A review on its composition and bioactivity. Early Human Development 91: 629–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bak, Yunjin, Yoonjin Nah, Sanghoon Han, Seung-Koo Lee, and Na-Young Shin. 2020. Altered neural substrates within cognitive networks of postpartum women during working memory process and resting-state. Scientific Reports 10: 9110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bakoyiannis, Ioannis, Afrodite Daskalopoulou, Vasilios Pergialiotis, and Despina Perrea. 2019. Phytochemicals and cognitive health: Are flavonoids doing the trick? Biomedicine and Pharmacotherapy 109: 1488–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bell, Lynne, and Claire Williams. 2019. A pilot dose–response study of the acute effects of haskap berry extract (Lonicera caerulea L.) on cognition, mood, and blood pressure in older adults. European Journal of Nutrition 58: 3325–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Berg, Esta. 1948. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. The Journal of General Psychology 39: 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Boccellino, Mariarosaria, and Stefania D’Angelo. 2020. Anti-obesity effects of polyphenol intake: Current status and future possibilities. International Journal of Molecular Sciences 21: 5642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Borkowski, John G., Arthur L. Benton, and Otfried Spreen. 1967. Word fluency and brain damage. Neuropsychologia 5: 135–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bouayed, Jaouad, Hassan Rammal, Amadou Dicko, Chafique Younos, and Rachid Soulimani. 2007. Chlorogenic acid, a polyphenol from Prunus domestica (Mirabelle), with coupled anxiolytic and antioxidant effects. Journal of the Neurological Sciences 262: 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Brink, Lauren, and Bo Lönnerdal. 2020. Milk fat globule membrane: The role of its various components in infant health and development. The Journal of Nutritional Biochemistry 85: 108465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Brown, Andrew, and Wendy Jessup. 2009. Oxysterols: Sources, cellular storage and metabolism, and new insights into their roles in cholesterol homeostasis. Molecular Aspects of Medicine 30: 111–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Burin, Débora, Marina Drake, and Paula Harris. 2007. Evaluación Neuropsicológica en Adultos. Buenos Aires: Paidós SAICF. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cadavid Ruiz, Natalia, Jaime Egido, María Purificación Galindo-Villardón, and Pablo del Río. 2018. Advantages of using HJ-biplot analysis in executive functions studies. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa 34: e3426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Caimari, Antoni, Roger Mariné-Casadó, Noemí Boqué, Anna Crescenti, Lluís Arola, and Josep Maria Del Bas. 2017. Maternal intake of grape seed procyanidins during lactation induces insulin resistance and an adiponectin resistance-like phenotype in rat offspring. Scientific Reports 7: 12573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Camfield, David, Beata Silber, Andrew Scholey, Karen Nolidin, Antoniette Goh, and Con Stough. 2013. A randomised placebo-controlled trial to differentiate the acute cognitive and mood effects of chlorogenic acid from decaffeinated coffee. PLoS ONE 8: e82897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Cardoso Silva de Souza, Brenda, Tailah de Oliveira Barreiros Teixeira, Liara Dias da Silva, Corina Satler, and Maysa Luchesi Cera. 2020. Verbal fluency of younger and older adults from the Federal District: Proposed normative values. Audiology-Communication Research 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Carnauba, Renata, Neuza Hassimotto, and Franco Lajolo. 2021. Estimated dietary polyphenol intake and major food sources of the Brazilian population. British Journal of Nutrition 126: 441–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Carpene, Christian, Saioa Gomez-Zorita, Simon Deleruyelle, and Marie-Anne Carpene. 2015. Novel strategies for preventing diabetes and obesity complications with natural polyphenols. Current Medicinal Chemistry 22: 150–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Carrasco, Gonzalo, Jose-Luis Molina, María-Carmen Patino-Alonso, Marisela del Carmen Castillo, María-Purificación Vicente-Galindo, and María-Purificación Galindo-Villardón. 2019. Water quality evaluation through a multivariate statistical HJ-Biplot approach. Journal of Hydrology 577: 123993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Carrizo, Eugenia, Julieta Domini, Romina Quezada, Silvana Valeria Serra, Elio Andrés Soria, and Agustín Ramiro Miranda. 2020. Variaciones del estado cognitivo en el puerperio y sus determinantes: Una revisión narrativa. Ciencia & Saude Coletiva 25: 3321–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Casey, Theresa, Jennifer Crodian, Shawn S. Donkin, and Karen Plaut. 2014. Continuously changing light-dark phase decreases milk yield, fat, protein and lactose in dairy cows. Advances in Dairy Research 2: 119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Chu, Kai On, and Calvin Pang. 2018. Pharmacokinetics and Disposition of Green Tea Catechins. In Pharmacokinetics and Adverse Effects of Drugs—Mechanisms and Risks Factors. London: IntechOpen. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Codini, Michela, Carmela Tringaniello, Lina Cossignani, Antonio Boccuto, Alessandra Mirarchi, Laura Cerquiglini, Stefania Troiani, Giuseppa Verducci, Federica Filomena Patria, Carmela Conte, and et al. 2020. Relationship between fatty acids composition/antioxidant potential of breast milk and maternal diet: Comparison with infant formulas. Molecules 25: 2910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Coman, Vasile, and Dan Vodnar. 2020. Hydroxycinnamic acids and human health: Recent advances. Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture 100: 483–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Cortez, Mariela Valentina, Agustín Ramiro Miranda, Ana Veronica Scotta, Laura Rosana Aballay, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2021. Patrones alimentarios de mujeres argentinas durante el puerperio en relación con factores socioeconómicos y sanitarios. Revista Médica del Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social 59: 7–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Cortez, Mariela Valentina, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2016. The effect of freeze-drying on the nutrient, polyphenol, and oxidant levels of breast milk. Breastfeeding Medicine 11: 551–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Crawford, Lindsay, Hong Li, Liye Zou, Gao-Xia Wei, and Paul Loprinzi. 2020. Hypothesized mechanisms through which exercise may attenuate memory interference. Medicina 56: 129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Cropley, Vanessa, Rodney Croft, Beata Silber, Chris Neale, Andrew Scholey, Con Stough, and Jeroen Schmitt. 2012. Does coffee enriched with chlorogenic acids improve mood and cognition after acute administration in healthy elderly? A pilot study. Psychopharmacology 219: 737–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Davis, Ronald L., and Yi Zhong. 2017. The biology of forgetting—A perspective. Neuron 95: 490–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. De la Cruz, María Victoria. 2001. Manual del Test de Clasificación de Tarjetas de Wisconsin (WCST). Madrid: TEA Ediciones. [Google Scholar]
  32. Demmelmair, Hans, and Berthold Koletzko. 2018. Lipids in human milk. Best Practice & Research Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 32: 57–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Downing, Laura E., Daniel Edgar, Patricia A. Ellison, and Marie-Louisse Ricketts. 2017. Mechanistic insight into nuclear receptor-mediated regulation of bile acid metabolism and lipid homeostasis by grape seed procyanidin extract (GSPE). Cell Biochemistry and Function 35: 12–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Edwards, William. 2010. Motor Learning and Control: From Theory to Practice. Wadsworth: Cengage Learning. [Google Scholar]
  35. Ehrmann, Jirí, Nicol Vavrusová, Yrjo Collan, and Zdenek Kolár. 2002. Peroxisome proliferator-activated receptors (PPARs) in health and disease. Biomedical Papers of the Medical Faculty of the University Palacky, Olomouc, Czechoslovakia 146: 11–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Elisia, Ingrid, and David D. Kitts. 2011. Quantification of hexanal as an index of lipid oxidation in human milk and association with antioxidant components. Journal of Clinical Biochemistry and Nutrition 49: 147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Fallon, Victoria, Rachael Groves, Jason Christian Grovenor Halford, Kate Mary Bennett, and Joan Allison Harrold. 2016. Postpartum anxiety and infant-feeding outcomes: A systematic review. Journal of Human Lactation 32: 740–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Farràs, Marta, Rosa M. Valls, Sara Fernández-Castillejo, Montserrat Giralt, Rosa Solà, Isaac Subirana, María José Motilva, Valentini Konstantinidou, María Isabel Covas, and Montserrat Fitó. 2013. Olive oil polyphenols enhance the expression of cholesterol efflux related genes in vivo in humans. A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of Nutritional Biochemistry 24: 1334–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Faustino, Bruno, Jorge Oliveira, and Paulo Lopes. 2020. Normative scores of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in a sample of the adult Portuguese population. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult 27: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Faustino, B., J. Oliveira, and P. Lopes. 2021. Diagnostic precision of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in assessing cognitive deficits in substance use disorders. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult 28: 165–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Feng, Juan, Jian Yang, Yujun Chang, Liansheng Qiao, Honglei Dang, Kun Luo, Hongyan Guo, Yannan An, Chengmei Ma, Hong Shao, and et al. 2019. Caffeine-free hawk tea lowers cholesterol by reducing free cholesterol uptake and the production of very-low-density lipoprotein. Communications Biology 2: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Ferreira Correia, A., and I. Campagna Osorio. 2014. The Rey auditory verbal learning test: Normative data developed for the venezuelan population. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 29: 206–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Fox, John. 2015. Applied Regression Analysis and Generalized Linear Models. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  44. Frutos Bernal, Elisa, Ángel Martin del Rey, and María Purificación Galindo Villardón. 2020. Analysis of madrid metro network: From structural to HJ-biplot perspective. Applied Sciences 10: 5689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Fu, Manjie, Lingsong Zhang, Azza Ahmed, Karen Plaut, David M. Haas, Kinga Szucs, and Theresa M. Casey. 2015. Does Circadian Disruption Play a Role in the Metabolic-Hormonal Link to Delayed Lactogenesis II? Frontiers in Nutrition 2: 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. George, Alexandra D., Melvin C. Gay, Robert D. Trengove, and Donna T. Geddes. 2018. Human milk lipidomics: Current techniques and methodologies. Nutrients 10: 1169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Gilsoul, Jessica, Jessica Simon, Michaël Hogge, and Fabienne Collette. 2019. Do attentional capacities and processing speed mediate the effect of age on executive functioning? Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition 26: 282–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Greendale, Gail A., Mei-Hua Huang, Katherine Leung, Sybil L. Crawford, Ellen B. Gold, Richard Wight, Elaine Waetjen, and Arun S. Karlamangla. 2012. Dietary phytoestrogen intakes and cognitive function during the menopause transition: Results from the SWAN phytoestrogen study. Menopause 19: 894–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Habtemariam, Solomon. 2017. Protective effects of caffeic acid and the Alzheimer’s brain: An update. Mini Reviews in Medicinal Chemistry 17: 667–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Heaton, Robert K., Gordon J. Chelune, Jack L. Talley, Gary G. Kay, and Glenn Curtiss. 1993. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual: Revised and Expanded. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
  51. Henríquez, Fernando, Victoria Cabello, Sandra Baez, Leonardo Cruz de Souza, Patricia Lillo, David Martínez-Pernía, Loreto Olavarría, Teresa Torralva, and Andrea Slachevsky. 2022. Multidimensional Clinical Assessment in Frontotemporal Dementia and Its Spectrum in Latin America and the Caribbean: A Narrative Review and a Glance at Future Challenges. Frontiers in Neurology 12: 768591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Herchi, Wahid, David Arráez-Román, Hajer Trabelsi, Intidhar Bouali, Sadok Boukhchina, Habib Kallel, Antonio Segura-Carretero, and Alberto Fernández-Gutierrez. 2014. Phenolic compounds in flaxseed: A review of their properties and analytical methods. An overview of the last decade. Journal of Oleo Science 63: 7–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Hohman, Timothy J., Lori L. Beason-Held, Melissa Lamar, and Susan M. Resnick. 2011. Subjective cognitive complaints and longitudinal changes in memory and brain function. Neuropsychology 25: 125–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Jonas, Wibke, and Barbara Woodside. 2016. Physiological mechanisms, behavioral and psychological factors influencing the transfer of milk from mothers to their young. Hormones and Behavior 77: 167–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Jurek, Benjamin, and Inga D. Neumann. 2018. The Oxytocin Receptor: From Intracellular Signaling to Behavior. Physiological Reviews 98: 1805–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Kawano, Atsuko, and Yoko Emori. 2015. The relationship between maternal postpartum psychological state and breast milk secretory immunoglobulin A level. Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association 21: 23–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Kennedy, David O., Philippa A. Jackson, Joanne Forster, Julie Khan, Torsten Grothe, Tania Perrinjaquet-Moccetti, and Crystal F. Haskell-Ramsay. 2017. Acute effects of a wild green-oat (Avena sativa) extract on cognitive function in middle-aged adults: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subjects trial. Nutritional Neuroscience 20: 135–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Kesse-Guyot, Emmanuelle, Léopold Fezeu, Valentina A. Andreeva, Mathilde Touvier, Augustin Scalbert, Serge Hercberg, and Pilar Galan. 2012. Total and specific polyphenol intakes in midlife are associated with cognitive function measured 13 years later. The Journal of Nutrition 142: 76–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Kiełbasa, Anna, Renata Gadzała-Kopciuch, and Boguslaw Buszewski. 2021. Cytokines-Biogenesis and Their Role in Human Breast Milk and Determination. International Journal of Molecular Sciences 22: 6238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Koletzko, Berthold. 2016. Human milk lipids. Annals of Nutrition & Metabolism 69: 28–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Kreijkamp-Kaspers, Sanne, Linda Kok, Diederick Grobbee, Edward De Haan, André Aleman, and Yvonne T. Van Der Schouw. 2007. Dietary phytoestrogen intake and cognitive function in older women. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 62: 556–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Kubota, Shizuka, Yuma Tanaka, and Satoshi Nagaoka. 2019. Ellagic acid affects mRNA expression levels of genes that regulate cholesterol metabolism in HepG2 cells. Bioscience, Biotechnology, and Biochemistry 83: 952–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Kumari, Suchitra, Jyotirmayee Bahinipati, Tapaswini Pradhan, and Durgesh P. Sahoo. 2020. Comparison of test performance of biochemical parameters in semiautomatic method and fully automatic analyzer method. Journal of Family Medicine and Primary Care 9: 3994–4000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Kwon, Seung-Hwan, Ha-Kyung Lee, Ji-Ah Kim, Sa-Ik Hong, Hyoung-Chun Kim, Tae-Hyung Jo, Young-In Park, Chong-Kil Lee, Yong-Bin Kim, Seok-Yong Lee, and et al. 2010. Neuroprotective effects of chlorogenic acid on scopolamine-induced amnesia via anti-acetylcholinesterase and anti-oxidative activities in mice. European Journal of Pharmacology 649: 210–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lamport, Daniel J., Clare L. Lawton, Natasha Merat, Hamish Jamson, Kyriaki Myrissa, Denise Hofman, Helen K. Chadwick, Frits Quadt, JoLynne D. Wightman, and Louise Dye. 2016a. Concord grape juice, cognitive function, and driving performance: A 12-wk, placebo-controlled, randomized crossover trial in mothers of preteen children. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 103: 775–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Lamport, Daniel J., Deepa Pal, Anna L. Macready, Sofia Barbosa-Boucas, John M. Fletcher, Claire M. Williams, Jeremy PE Spencer, and Laurie T. Butler. 2016b. The effects of flavanone-rich citrus juice on cognitive function and cerebral blood flow: An acute, randomised, placebo-controlled cross-over trial in healthy, young adults. British Journal of Nutrition 116: 2160–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Lezak, Muriel Deutsch, Diane B. Howieson, Erin D. Bigler, and Daniel Tranel. 2012. Neuropsychological Assessment, 5th ed. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  68. Lin, Man, Na Wang, Bei Yao, Yao Zhong, Yan Lin, and Tianhui You. 2018. Quercetin improves postpartum hypogalactia in milk-deficient mice via stimulating prolactin production in pituitary gland. Phytotherapy Research 32: 1511–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Lozoya-Delgado, Paz, José María de León Ruiz-Sánchez, and Eduardo J. Pedrero-Perez. 2012. Validation of a cognitive complaints questionnaire for young adults: The relation between subjective memory complaints, prefrontal symptoms and perceived stress. Revista de Neurología 54: 137–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Luca, Simon Vlad, Irina Macovei, Alexandra Bujor, Anca Miron, Krystyna Skalicka-Woźniak, Ana Clara Aprotosoaie, and Adriana Trifan. 2020. Bioactivity of dietary polyphenols: The role of metabolites. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition 60: 626–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Mancini, Edele, Cristoph Beglinger, Jürgen Drewe, Davide Zanchi, Undine E. Lang, and Stefan Borgwardt. 2017. Green tea effects on cognition, mood and human brain function: A systematic review. Phytomedicine 34: 26–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Manzar, Md Dilshad, Haitham A. Jahrami, and Ahmed S. Bahammam. 2021. Structural validity of the Insomnia Severity Index: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews 60: 101531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Margină, Denisa, Anca Ungurianu, Carmen Purdel, George Mihai Nițulescu, Dimitris Tsoukalas, Evangelia Sarandi, Maria Thanasoula, Tatyana I. Burykina, Fotis Tekos, Aleksandra Buha, and et al. 2020. Analysis of the intricate effects of polyunsaturated fatty acids and polyphenols on inflammatory pathways in health and disease. Food and Chemical Toxicology 143: 111558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Marino, Julián C., and Ana María Alderete. 2010. Valores Normativos de Pruebas de Fluidez Verbal Categoriales, Fonológicas, Gramaticales y Combinadas y Análisis Comparativo de la Capacidad de Iniciación. Revista Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencias 10: 82–93. [Google Scholar]
  75. Matacchione, Giulia, Felicia Gurău, Simone Baldoni, Francesco Prattichizzo, Andrea Silvestrini, Angelica Giuliani, Armanda Pugnaloni, Emma Espinosa, Francesco Amenta, Massimiliano Bonafè, and et al. 2020. Pleiotropic effects of polyphenols on glucose and lipid metabolism: Focus on clinical trials. Ageing Research Reviews 61: 101074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. McManaman, James L. 2014. Lipid transport in the lactating mammary gland. Journal of mammary Gland Biology and Neoplasia 19: 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Mendonça, Marcelo D., Luísa Alves, and Paulo Bugalho. 2016. From Subjective Cognitive Complaints to Dementia: Who is at Risk?: A Systematic Review. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and other Dementias 31: 105–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Mías, Carlos D. 2010. Quejas de Memoria y Deterioro Cognitivo Leve: Concepto, Evaluación y Prevención. Córdoba: Encuentro. [Google Scholar]
  79. Milošević, Maja, Aleksandra Arsić, Zorica Cvetković, and Vesna Vučić. 2021. Memorable Food: Fighting Age-Related Neurodegeneration by Precision Nutrition. Frontiers in Nutrition 8: 688086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Miranda, Agustín Ramiro, Claudia Albrecht, Mariela Valentina Cortez, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2018. Pharmacology and toxicology of polyphenols with potential as neurotropic agents in non-communicable diseases. Current Drug Targets 19: 97–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Miranda, Agustín Ramiro, Juliana Franchetto Sierra, Amparo Martínez Roulet, Luisina Rivadero, Silvana Valeria Serra, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2020a. Age, education and gender effects on Wisconsin card sorting test: Standardization, reliability and validity in healthy Argentinian adults. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition 27: 807–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Miranda, Agustín Ramiro, Luisina Rivadero, Jorge Ángel Bruera, Viriginia Villarreal, Laura Yhicel Bernio, María de Los Ángeles Baydas, Mónica Liliana Brizuela, and Silvana Valeria Serra. 2020b. Examining the relationship between engagement and perceived stress-related cognitive complaints in the Argentinian working population. Europe’s Journal of Psychology 16: 12–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. Miranda, Agustín Ramiro, Luisina Rivadero, Silvana Valeria Serra, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2020c. Multidomain self-report assessment of fronto-executive complaints in Spanish-speaking adults. Psychology and Neuroscience 13: 357–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Miranda, Agustín Ramiro, Mariela Valentina Cortez, Ana Veronica Scotta, Luisina Rivadero, Silvana Valeria Serra, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2021a. Memory enhancement in Argentinian women during postpartum by the dietary intake of lignans and anthocyanins. Nutrition Research 85: 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Miranda, Agustín Ramiro, Ana Veronica Scotta, Mariela Valentina Cortez, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2021b. Triggering of postpartum depression and insomnia with cognitive impairment in Argentinian women during the pandemic COVID-19 social isolation in relation to reproductive and health factors. Midwifery 102: 103072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Miranda, Agustín Ramiro, Mariela Valentina Cortez, Ana Veronica Scotta, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2022a. COVID-19-related stress in postpartum women from Argentina during the second wave in 2021: Identification of impairing and protective factors. Midwifery 108: 103290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Miranda, Agustín Ramiro, Ana Veronica Scotta, Mariela Valentina Cortez, Nerea González-García, María Purificación Galindo-Villardón, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2022b. Association of dietary intake of polyphenols with an adequate nutritional profile in postpartum. Preventive Nutrition & Food Science 27: 20–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Mishra, Prabhaker, Uttam Singh, Chandra M. Pandey, Prishadarshni Mishra, and Gaurav Pandey. 2019. Application of student’s t-test, analysis of variance, and covariance. Annals of Cardiac Anaesthesia 22: 407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Moghaddam, Akbar Hajizadeh, and Mahboobeh Zare. 2018. Neuroprotective effect of hesperetin and nano-hesperetin on recognition memory impairment and the elevated oxygen stress in rat model of Alzheimer’s disease. Biomedicine & Pharmacotherapy 97: 1096–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Mohammad, Mahmoud A., and Morey W. Haymond. 2013. Regulation of lipid synthesis genes and milk fat production in human mammary epithelial cells during secretory activation. American Journal of Physiology. Endocrinology and Metabolism 305: E700–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Mohd Shukri, Nurul Husna, Jonathan Wells, Simon Eaton, Firdaus Mukhtar, Ana Petelin, Zala Jenko-Pražnikar, and Mary Fewtrell. 2019. Randomized controlled trial investigating the effects of a breastfeeding relaxation intervention on maternal psychological state, breast milk outcomes, and infant behavior and growth. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 110: 121–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  92. Morin, Charles M., Geneviève Belleville, Lynda Bélanger, and Hans Ivers. 2011. The Insomnia Severity Index: Psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep 34: 601–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  93. Nieto-Librero, A. B., C. Sierra, M. P. Vicente-Galindo, O. Ruíz-Barzola, and M. P. Galindo-Villardón. 2017. Clustering Disjoint HJ-Biplot: A new tool for identifying pollution patterns in geochemical studies. Chemosphere 176: 389–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Ontsouka, Edgar C., and Christiane Albrecht. 2014. Cholesterol transport and regulation in the mammary gland. Journal of Mammary Gland Biology and Neoplasia 19: 43–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Palin, Marie France, Cristiano Côrtes, Chaouki Benchaar, Pierre Lacasse, and Hélène V. Petit. 2014. mRNA Expression of lipogenic enzymes in mammary tissue and fatty acid profile in milk of dairy cows fed flax hulls and infused with flax oil in the abomasum. British Journal of Nutrition 111: 1011–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  96. Pérez-Rodríguez, Lorenzo, Ana A. Romero-Haro, Audrey Sternalski, Jaime Muriel, Francois Mougeot, Diego Gil, and Carlos Alonso-Alvarez. 2015. Measuring oxidative stress: The confounding effect of lipid concentration in measures of lipid peroxidation. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 88: 345–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. Poniedziałek, Barbara, Piotr Rzymski, Malgorzata Pięt, Monika Gąsecka, Anna Stroińska, Przemyslaw Niedzielski, Miroslaw Mleczek, Pawel Rzymski, and Maciej Wilczak. 2018. Relation between polyphenols, malondialdehyde, antioxidant capacity, lactate dehydrogenase and toxic elements in human colostrum milk. Chemosphere 191: 548–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Rabadan-Chávez, Griselda, Lucia Quevedo-Corona, Angel Miliar Garcia, Elba Reyes-Maldonado, and Maria Jaramillo-Flores. 2016. Cocoa powder, cocoa extract and epicatechin attenuate hypercaloric diet-induced obesity through enhanced β-oxidation and energy expenditure in white adipose tissue. Journal of Functional Foods 20: 54–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Rey, André. 1958. L’examen Clinique en Psychologie. [The Clinical Examination in Psychology]. Paris: Presses Universitaries De France. [Google Scholar]
  100. Ríos, Jimena, Viviana Valero-Jara, and Samanta Thomas-Valdés. 2021. Phytochemicals in breast milk and their benefits for infants. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Rivera, Diego, Paul B. Perrin, Lillian Flores Stevens, María Teresa Garza, Carlos Weil, Carmen Patricia Saracho, Walter Rodriguez, Yaneth Rodríguez-Agudelo, Brenda Rábago, Gudrun Weiler, and et al. 2015. Stroop color-word interference test: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 37: 591–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Rodríguez Barreto, Lucía Carlota, Ninfa del Carmen Pulido, and Carlos Alejandro Pineda Roa. 2016. Propiedades psicométricas del Stroop, test de colores y palabras en población colombiana no patológica. Universitas Psychologica 15: 255–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Rodríguez-Blázquez, Carmen, Alba Ayala-García, Maria Joao Forjaz, Sara García-Herranz, César Venero, Raquel Rodríguez-Fernández, and María del Carmen Díaz-Mardomingo. 2022. Validation of the Spanish version of the Memory Failures of Everyday questionnaire in older adults using Rasch analysis. Geriatrics and Gerontology International 22: 332–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Roman-Viñas, Blanca, Lluis Serra-Majem, Maria Hagströmer, Lourdes Ribas-Barba, Michael Sjöström, and Ramon Segura-Cardona. 2010. International physical activity questionnaire: Reliability and validity in a Spanish population. European Journal of Sport Science 10: 297–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Rossi, María Constanza, María Natalia Bassett, and Norma Cristina Samman. 2018. Dietary nutritional profile and phenolic compounds consumption in school children of highlands of Argentine Northwest. Food Chemistry 238: 111–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Rothwell, Joseph A., Jara Perez-Jimenez, Vanessa Neveu, Alexander Medina-Remon, Nouha M’hiri, Paula García-Lobato, Claudine Manach, Craig Knox, Roman Eisner, David S. Wishart, and et al. 2013. Phenol-Explorer 3.0: A major update of the Phenol-Explorer database to incorporate data on the effects of food processing on polyphenol content. Database, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Rufino, Ana T., Vera M. Costa, Félix Carvalho, and Eduarda Fernandes. 2021. Flavonoids as antiobesity agents: A review. Medicinal Research Reviews 41: 556–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Salehi, Bahare, Natália Cruz-Martins, Monica Butnariu, Ioan Sarac, Iulia-Cristina Bagiu, Shahira M. Ezzat, Jinfan Wang, Aaron Koay, Helen Sheridan, Charles Oluwaseun Adetunji, and et al. 2021. Hesperetin’s health potential: Moving from preclinical to clinical evidence and bioavailability issues, to upcoming strategies to overcome current limitations. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Scarpina, Federica, and Sofía Tagini. 2017. The Stroop Color and Word Test. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Scotta, Ana Veronica, Mariela Valentina Cortez, and Agustín Ramiro Miranda. 2022. Insomnia is associated with worry, cognitive avoidance and low academic engagement in Argentinian university students during the COVID-19 social isolation. Psychology, Health & Medicine 27: 199–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Senthil, Renganathan, Manokaran Sakthivel, and Singaravelu Usha. 2021. Structure-based drug design of peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor gamma inhibitors: Ferulic acid and derivatives. Journal of Biomolecular Structure & Dynamics 39: 1295–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Shin, Na-Young, Yunjin Bak, Yoonjin Nah, Sanghoon Han, Dong Joon Kim, Se Joo Kim, Jong Eun Lee, Sang Guk Lee, and Seung Koo Lee. 2018. Disturbed retrieval network and prospective memory decline in postpartum women. Scientific Reports 8: 5476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  113. Singh, Yash Pal, Himanshu Rai, Gourav Singh, Gireesh Kumar Singh, Sunil Mishra, Saroj Kumar, S. Srikrishna, and Gyan Modi. 2021. A review on ferulic acid and analogs based scaffolds for the management of Alzheimer’s disease. European Journal of Medicinal Chemistry 215: 113278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Shieh, Gwowen. 2020. Power analysis and sample size planning in ANCOVA designs. Psychometrika 85: 101–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Singla, Rajeev K., Ashok K. Dubey, Arun Garg, Ramesh K. Sharma, Marco Fiorino, Sara M. Ameen, Moawiya A. Haddad, and Masnat Al-Hiary. 2019. Natural polyphenols: Chemical classification, definition of classes, subcategories, and structures. Journal of AOAC International 102: 1397–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Stuebe, Allison M., Karen Grewen, Cort A. Pedersen, Cathi Propper, and Samantha Meltzer-Brody. 2012. Failed lactation and perinatal depression: Common problems with shared neuroendocrine mechanisms? Journal of Women’s Health 21: 264–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Stroop, John Ridley. 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18: 643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Troubat, Nicolas, Marie-Agnes Fargeas-Gluck, Mikko Tulppo, and Benoit Dugué. 2009. The stress of chess players as a model to study the effects of psychological stimuli on physiological responses: An example of substrate oxidation and heart rate variability in man. European Journal of Applied Physiology 105: 343–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Tull, Matthe T., Danielle J. Maack, Andres G. Viana, and Kim L. Gratz. 2012. Behavioral inhibition and attentional network functioning. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy 41: 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Tyagi, Rahul, Harshita Arvind, Manoj Goyal, Akshay Anand, and Manju Mohanty. 2021. Working Memory Alterations Plays an Essential Role in Developing Global Neuropsychological Impairment in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 613242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Valls-Bellés, Victoria, Cristina Abad, María Teresa Hernández-Aguilar, Amalia Nacher, Carlos Guerrero, Pablo Baliño, Francisco J. Romero, and María Muriach. 2021. Human Milk Antioxidative Modifications in Mastitis: Further Beneficial Effects of Cranberry Supplementation. Antioxidants 11: 51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Vauzour, David, Katerina Vafeiadou, Catherine Rice-Evans, Robert J. Williams, and Jeremy P. Spencer. 2007. Activation of pro-survival Akt and ERK1/2 signalling pathways underlie the anti-apoptotic effects of flavanones in cortical neurons. Journal of Neurochemistry 103: 1355–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Venkat, Nanditha, Meenakshi Sinha, Ramanjan Sinha, Jayshri Ghate, and Babita Pande. 2020. Neuro-Cognitive Profile of Morning and Evening Chronotypes at Different Times of Day. Annals of Neurosciences 27: 257–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Vicente-Villardón José Luis. MultBiplot. 2015. A Package for Multivariate Analysis Using Biplots. Salamanca: Departamento de Estadística, Universidad de Salamanca. [Google Scholar]
  125. Wang, Zhuo, Linjun You, Yan Cheng, Kaiyong Hu, Zhanbo Wang, Yanan Cheng, Jin Yang, Yong Yang, and Guangji Wang. 2018. Investigation of pharmacokinetics, tissue distribution and excretion of schisandrin B in rats by HPLC–MS/MS. Biomedical Chromatography 32: e4069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Whyte, Adrian R., and Claire M. Williams. 2015. Effects of a single dose of a flavonoid-rich blueberry drink on memory in 8 to 10 y old children. Nutrition 31: 531–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Wightman, Emma L., Crystal F. Haskell, Joanne S. Forster, Rachel C. Veasey, and David O. Kennedy. 2012. Epigallocatechin gallate, cerebral blood flow parameters, cognitive performance and mood in healthy humans: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover investigation. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental 27: 177–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Williamson, Gary, Colin D. Kay, and Alan Crozier. 2018. The bioavailability, transport, and bioactivity of dietary flavonoids: A review from a historical perspective. Comprehensive Reviews in Food Science and Food Safety 17: 1054–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  129. Xavier, Soniya, Alita Soch, Simin Younesi, Sajida Malik, Sarah Spencer, and Luba Sominsky. 2021. Maternal diet before and during pregnancy modulates microglial activation and neurogenesis in the postpartum rat brain. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 98: 185–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Yang, Dengbao, HoangDinh Huynh, and Yihong Wan. 2018. Milk lipid regulation at the maternal-offspring interface. Seminars in Cell and Developmental Biology 81: 141–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Yao, Junpeng, Pengcheng Hu, Yanhong Zhu, Yingyan Xu, Qingsong Tan, and Xufang Liang. 2020. Lipid-Lowering Effects of Lotus Leaf Alcoholic Extract on Serum, Hepatopancreas, and Muscle of Juvenile Grass Carp via Gene Expression. Frontiers in Physiology 11: 1480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Zhong, Jing, Yuxuan Liang, Yongchun Chen, Jiawei Zhang, Xiaoying Zou, Jie Deng, Da Wang, Yuanming Sun, and Meiying Li. 2021. Study and Experimental Validation of the Functional Components and Mechanisms of Hemerocallis citrina Baroni in the Treatment of Lactation Deficiency. Foods 10: 1863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Ziomkiewicz, Anna, Magdalena Babiszewska, Anna Apanasewicz, Magdalena Piosek, Patrycja Wychowaniec, Agnieszka Cierniak, Olga Barbarska, Marek Szołtysik, Dariusz Danel, and Szymon Wichary. 2021. Psychosocial stress and cortisol stress reactivity predict breast milk composition. Scientific Reports 11: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Directed acyclic graph of the relationship among polyphenol intake, cognition, and human milk lipids in postpartum Argentinian women. Green squares indicate the independent (dietary polyphenol intake and cognition) and dependent variables (human milk lipids); red squares indicate minimal sufficient adjustment variables; the bold arrow indicates the relationship between the independent and dependent variables; thin arrows indicate other causal relationships.
Figure 1. Directed acyclic graph of the relationship among polyphenol intake, cognition, and human milk lipids in postpartum Argentinian women. Green squares indicate the independent (dietary polyphenol intake and cognition) and dependent variables (human milk lipids); red squares indicate minimal sufficient adjustment variables; the bold arrow indicates the relationship between the independent and dependent variables; thin arrows indicate other causal relationships.
Jintelligence 10 00033 g001
Figure 2. Daily dietary polyphenolic intake in postpartum women from Córdoba, Argentina (n = 75), with data being expressed as mean and standard errors.
Figure 2. Daily dietary polyphenolic intake in postpartum women from Córdoba, Argentina (n = 75), with data being expressed as mean and standard errors.
Jintelligence 10 00033 g002
Figure 3. HJ-Biplot representation (axis 1–2) for clusters of polyphenols consumed above 20 mg/d (a) and between 5 and 20 mg/d (b) by Argentinian postpartum women.
Figure 3. HJ-Biplot representation (axis 1–2) for clusters of polyphenols consumed above 20 mg/d (a) and between 5 and 20 mg/d (b) by Argentinian postpartum women.
Jintelligence 10 00033 g003
Figure 4. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) scores according to polyphenolic intake clusters (>20 mg/d). ANCOVA models were carried out with Fisher’s post hoc contrast and adjusted by educational level, postpartum days, parity, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical, and energetic), and insomnia. C1: n = 34; C2: n = 24; C3: n = 17. D = Cohen’s d for effect size; * p < 0.05.
Figure 4. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) scores according to polyphenolic intake clusters (>20 mg/d). ANCOVA models were carried out with Fisher’s post hoc contrast and adjusted by educational level, postpartum days, parity, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical, and energetic), and insomnia. C1: n = 34; C2: n = 24; C3: n = 17. D = Cohen’s d for effect size; * p < 0.05.
Jintelligence 10 00033 g004
Figure 5. Postpartum women’s performance in the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT), and Verbal Fluency Task (VFT) according to polyphenolic intake clusters (>20 mg/d). ANCOVA models were adjusted by educational level, postpartum days, parity, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical and energetic), and insomnia, with a Fisher’s post hoc contrast. C1: n = 34; C2: n = 24; C3: n = 17. d = Cohen’s d for effect size; * p < 0.05.
Figure 5. Postpartum women’s performance in the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT), and Verbal Fluency Task (VFT) according to polyphenolic intake clusters (>20 mg/d). ANCOVA models were adjusted by educational level, postpartum days, parity, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical and energetic), and insomnia, with a Fisher’s post hoc contrast. C1: n = 34; C2: n = 24; C3: n = 17. d = Cohen’s d for effect size; * p < 0.05.
Jintelligence 10 00033 g005
Figure 6. Postpartum women’s performance in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), and Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) according to polyphenolic intake clusters (5 to 20 mg/d). ANCOVA models were adjusted by educational level, postpartum days, parity, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical, and energetic), and insomnia, with a Fisher’s post hoc contrast. C1: n = 33; C2: n = 29; C3: n = 13. d = Cohen’s d for effect size; * p < 0.05.
Figure 6. Postpartum women’s performance in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), and Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) according to polyphenolic intake clusters (5 to 20 mg/d). ANCOVA models were adjusted by educational level, postpartum days, parity, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical, and energetic), and insomnia, with a Fisher’s post hoc contrast. C1: n = 33; C2: n = 29; C3: n = 13. d = Cohen’s d for effect size; * p < 0.05.
Jintelligence 10 00033 g006
Figure 7. HJ-Biplot representation for clusters in the main plane (axis 1–2) for the executive/attentional functioning (a) and memory (b) in Argentinian postpartum women. SCWT = Stroop color and word test; I = Interference; C = Color; W = Word; W/C = Word-Color; WCST = Wisconsin card sorting test; TC1 °C = Trials to complete first category; %NPE = Percentage of non-perseverative errors; %TE = Percentage of total errors; GS = Global score; TA = Trials administered; FMS = Failure to maintain set; %PR = Percentage of perseverative responses; %PE = Percentage of perseverative errors; CR = Correct responses; CLR = Conceptual level responses; CA = Categories achieved; %CLR = Percentage of conceptual level responses; LL = Learning to learn; VFT:ELA = Verbal fluency task, excluded letter A; RHR = recency hit rate; MHR = middle hit rate; TA1 = Trial A1; IT = interference trial (B); PIT = post-interference trial (A6); PHR = primacy hit rate; R = recognition; DR = delayed recall; MEI = memory efficiency index; F = forgetting; %F = percentage of forgetting; L = Learning; CTL = Corrected total learning; ΣA1–5 = sum trials A1 to A5; IR, immediate recall; VF-P, verbal fluency letter P; VF-F, verbal fluency letter F; VF-Animals, verbal fluency Animals; PrI = proactive interference; ReI = retroactive interference; TA5 = trial A5; E = evocation; Ret = retention; IE = Intrusion errors; ER = errors of repetitions; FS = forgetting speed; VFT = verbal fluency task; LF = letter F; LP = letter P; S = semantic.
Figure 7. HJ-Biplot representation for clusters in the main plane (axis 1–2) for the executive/attentional functioning (a) and memory (b) in Argentinian postpartum women. SCWT = Stroop color and word test; I = Interference; C = Color; W = Word; W/C = Word-Color; WCST = Wisconsin card sorting test; TC1 °C = Trials to complete first category; %NPE = Percentage of non-perseverative errors; %TE = Percentage of total errors; GS = Global score; TA = Trials administered; FMS = Failure to maintain set; %PR = Percentage of perseverative responses; %PE = Percentage of perseverative errors; CR = Correct responses; CLR = Conceptual level responses; CA = Categories achieved; %CLR = Percentage of conceptual level responses; LL = Learning to learn; VFT:ELA = Verbal fluency task, excluded letter A; RHR = recency hit rate; MHR = middle hit rate; TA1 = Trial A1; IT = interference trial (B); PIT = post-interference trial (A6); PHR = primacy hit rate; R = recognition; DR = delayed recall; MEI = memory efficiency index; F = forgetting; %F = percentage of forgetting; L = Learning; CTL = Corrected total learning; ΣA1–5 = sum trials A1 to A5; IR, immediate recall; VF-P, verbal fluency letter P; VF-F, verbal fluency letter F; VF-Animals, verbal fluency Animals; PrI = proactive interference; ReI = retroactive interference; TA5 = trial A5; E = evocation; Ret = retention; IE = Intrusion errors; ER = errors of repetitions; FS = forgetting speed; VFT = verbal fluency task; LF = letter F; LP = letter P; S = semantic.
Jintelligence 10 00033 g007
Table 1. Sociodemographic and reproductive characteristics of postpartum women (n = 75).
Table 1. Sociodemographic and reproductive characteristics of postpartum women (n = 75).
VariableMeanSD%N
Age (years)29.926.13
Marital status
   In a couple 93.3370
   Single 6.665
Educational level
   <12 years of formal instruction 25.3319
   ≥12 years of formal instruction 75.6656
Employment
   Formally employed 45.3334
   Informally employed/unemployed 54.6641
Postpartum duration (days)95.0452.46
Gestational age at delivery38.372.05
   <37 weeks (preterm) 14.6611
   ≥37 weeks (term) 85.3364
Mode of delivery
   Cesarean section 54.6641
   Vaginal 45.3334
Number of births
   Primiparous 4030
   Multiparous 6045
Practice of exclusive breastfeeding
   No 46.6635
   Yes 53.3340
Breastfeeding frequency (hours)3.012.72
Time of sampling
   08.00 to 12.00 62.6647
   12.00 to 16.00 18.6614
   16.00 to 20.00 18.6614
BMI24.795.62
BFP28.887
Physical activity (MET)384.96686.33
Macro-nutritional Dietary Index25.256.85
Phytochemical Dietary Index21.897.18
Energetic Dietary Index22.817.71
Fat Quality Index27.9423.84
Protein to Carbohydrate Ratio 0.340.08
Note. SD = standard deviation; BMI = body mass index; BFP = body fat percentage; MET = metabolic equivalents of task.
Table 2. Postpartum women’s performance in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (n = 75).
Table 2. Postpartum women’s performance in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (n = 75).
WCST ScoreMSDMdn25P–75PSK
Trials administered100.6725.3593.0076.00–128.000.03−1.83
Correct responses67.3612.2866.0062.00–75.00−0.281.10
Total errors33.3026.6820.0011.00–55.000.76−0.77
% Total errors29.1318.4420.4014.67–42.970.87−0.45
Perseverative responses12.649.599.006.00–16.001.300.67
% Perseverative responses11.626.889.337.14–14.131.090.63
Perseverative errors9.797.437.005.00–12.001.330.77
% Perseverative errors11.0612.597.625.71–11.694.5021.78
Non-perseverative errors23.3223.6111.006.00–37.001.320.91
% Non-perseverative errors20.1518.6611.687.59–28.131.803.32
Conceptual level responses55.8917.6860.0054.00–65.00−1.341.65
% Conceptual level responses61.1225.8073.7442.19–82.28−0.77−0.65
Categories achieved4.492.066.003.00–6.00−0.95−0.73
Global score55.7443.6233.0016.00–98.000.37−1.57
Trials to complete first category19.1324.5311.0011.00–13.003.5611.35
Failure to maintain set0.701.180.000.00–1.001.912.92
Learning to learn−1.844.13−1.26−2.86–0.33−1.502.59
Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Mdn = median; 25P = 25th percentile; 75P = 75th percentile; S = Skewness; K = Kurtosis.
Table 3. Postpartum women’s performance in the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (n = 75).
Table 3. Postpartum women’s performance in the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (n = 75).
RAVLT ScoreMSDMdn25P–75PSK
Trial A15.682.025.004.00–7.000.520.26
Trial A28.032.748.006.00–9.000.30−0.41
Trial A39.733.0910.008.00–12.00−0.54−0.43
Trial A410.822.7211.009.00–13.00−1.071.15
Trial A511.482.6212.0011.00–13.00−1.513.97
Σ A1–A545.9511.1148.0037.00–53.00−0.17−0.50
Interference (trial B)5.392.385.004.00–6.000.910.81
Post-interference (trial A6)10.072.6110.008.00–12.00−0.15−0.50
Delayed recall (trial A7)10.192.8610.009.00–12.00−0.25−0.63
Recognition (trial A8)12.992.2114.0011.00–15.00−1.291.11
Errors of repetitions5.453.935.003.00–7.001.181.46
Intrusion errors2.894.611.000.00–4.003.9520.01
Corrected total learning17.347.8718.0012.00–24.00−0.56−0.53
Learning5.732.376.004.00–7.00−1.032.57
Forgetting1.312.201.000.00–3.00−0.792.13
% of forgetting0.120.230.100.00–0.25−2.6914.73
Forgetting speed1.020.191.000.91–1.110.270.07
Retention0.051.670.00−1.00–1.00−0.23−0.20
Evocation2.632.383.001.00–4.00−0.702.29
Primacy0.650.160.650.55–0.80−0.24−0.83
Recency0.680.160.700.60–0.80−0.51−0.37
Total hit rate0.590.120.610.49–0.67−0.08−0.59
Primacy hit rate0.620.130.630.54–0.75−0.19−0.88
Middle hit rate0.540.160.540.44–0.68−0.16−0.52
Recency hit rate0.650.150.670.54–0.75−0.43−0.54
Memory Efficiency Index1.990.322.041.84–2.19−0.441.22
Proactive interference1.030.511.000.71–1.251.734.31
Retroactive interference0.900.320.890.75–1.004.2024.40
Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Mdn = median; 25P = 25th percentile; 75P = 75th percentile; S = Skewness; K = Kurtosis.
Table 4. Postpartum women’s performance in the Stroop Color and Word Test, Verbal Fluency Tasks, and self-report complaints (n = 75).
Table 4. Postpartum women’s performance in the Stroop Color and Word Test, Verbal Fluency Tasks, and self-report complaints (n = 75).
ScoresMSDMdnP25–P75SK
Stroop Color and Word Test
 Word92.5113.9396.0085.00–102.00−0.840.39
 Color67.0110.9469.0059.00–74.00−0.24−0.17
 Word-color40.508.8641.5035.00–47.00−0.470.26
 Interference1.877.711.54−2.66–6.480.100.87
Verbal fluency task
 Phonological Letter P14.034.3813.0011.00–17.000.48−0.50
 Phonological Letter F9.733.629.507.00–12.000.560.41
 Semantic Animals17.555.6817.0014.00–22.00−0.180.25
 Phonological Letter Excluded A7.693.908.005.00–11.000.12−0.03
Self-report complaints
 Insomnia Severity Index9.784.779.007.00–12.000.440.10
 Executive Complaints24.648.8525.0017.00–31.000.01−0.60
 Memory Failures of Everyday28.7617.2725.0016.00–36.001.020.69
Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Mdn = median; 25P = 25th percentile; 75P = 75th percentile; S = Skewness; K = Kurtosis.
Table 5. Human milk lipids’ concentration according to polyphenolic clusters in postpartum women from Córdoba, Argentina (n = 75).
Table 5. Human milk lipids’ concentration according to polyphenolic clusters in postpartum women from Córdoba, Argentina (n = 75).
CompoundPolyphenols from 5 to 20 mg/dPolyphenols > 20 mg/d
C1
(n = 33)
C2
(n = 29)
C3
(n = 13)
dC1
(n = 34)
C2
(n = 24)
C3
(n = 17)
d
Triacylglycerols (g/L)23.76
(1.87)
25.10
(1.99)
30.52
(2.79) *
0.65425.06
(2.03)
26.61
(2.19)
24.81
(2.98)
-
Cholesterol (mg/L)80.04
(10.85)
113.71
(11.46) *
97.73
(16.10)
0.55285.94
(11.51)
92.02
(16.91)
112.63
(12.38)
-
Oxidized triacylglycerols (OD/g)335.96
(116.59)
237.66
(123.71)
248.77
(173.78)
-275.09
(122.65)
241.49
(132.17)
349.39
(180.12)
-
Polar oxysterols (OD/mg)92.90
(10.77)
80.39
(11.37)
84.81
(15.97)
-91.79
(11.28)
78.72
(12.13)
88.18
(16.57)
-
Non-polar oxysterols (OD/mg)81.31
(8.50)
59.02
(8.97)
61.65
(12.61)
-71.11
(9.11)
62.43
(9.80)
74.85
(13.38)
-
Note. Data expressed as mean (standard error). ANCOVA models were adjusted by breastfeeding, parity, postpartum days, body fat percentage, and dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical and energetic), with post hoc comparisons (d = Cohen’s d for size effect; * p < 0.05). Thresholds of the effect size: small (0.20), medium (0.50), large (0.80), very large (1.30).
Table 6. Human milk lipids concentration according to executive/attentional functioning and memory of postpartum women from Córdoba, Argentina (n = 75).
Table 6. Human milk lipids concentration according to executive/attentional functioning and memory of postpartum women from Córdoba, Argentina (n = 75).
CompoundExecutive/Attentional FunctioningMemory
>EF
(n = 37)
<IF
(n = 14)
<AS
(n = 24)
d>M
(n = 41)
<M
(n = 34)
d
Triacylglycerols (g/L)28.53
(1.71)
21.90
(2.84) *
22.94
(1.98) *
−0.647
−0.561
27.69
(1.60)
22.83
(1.77) *
−0.479
Cholesterol (mg/L)90.97
(10.26)
112.34
(17.88)
96.84
(11.88)
-94.35
(9.53)
98.36
(10.70)
-
Oxidized triacylglycerols (OD/g)189.17
(103.55)
640.97
(172.12) *
241.97
(120.01)
0.727194.52
(27.69)
281.28
(30.85) *
0.493
Polar oxysterols (OD/mg)95.34
(9.77)
65.13
(17.03)
84.06
(11.32)
-92.89
(9.10)
78.89
(10.22)
-
Non-polar oxysterols (OD/mg)71.32
(7.97)
55.70
(13.90)
72.32
(9.24)
-64.96
(7.37)
74.48
(8.28)
-
Note. Data expressed as mean (standard error). ANCOVA models were adjusted by breastfeeding, parity, postpartum days, body fat percentage, dietary indices (macronutritional, phytochemical, and energetic), and insomnia severity index, with post hoc comparisons (d = Cohen’s d for size effect; * p < 0.05). Thresholds of the effect size: small (0.20), medium (0.50), large (0.80), very large (1.30). >EF = cluster with higher executive functioning; <IF = cluster with lower inhibitory control and behavioral flexibility; <AS = cluster with lower attentional set control; M = memory.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Miranda, A.R.; Cortez, M.V.; Scotta, A.V.; Soria, E.A. Dietary Intake of Polyphenols Enhances Executive/Attentional Functioning and Memory with an Improvement of the Milk Lipid Profile of Postpartum Women from Argentina. J. Intell. 2022, 10, 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020033

AMA Style

Miranda AR, Cortez MV, Scotta AV, Soria EA. Dietary Intake of Polyphenols Enhances Executive/Attentional Functioning and Memory with an Improvement of the Milk Lipid Profile of Postpartum Women from Argentina. Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(2):33. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020033

Chicago/Turabian Style

Miranda, Agustín Ramiro, Mariela Valentina Cortez, Ana Veronica Scotta, and Elio Andrés Soria. 2022. "Dietary Intake of Polyphenols Enhances Executive/Attentional Functioning and Memory with an Improvement of the Milk Lipid Profile of Postpartum Women from Argentina" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 2: 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020033

APA Style

Miranda, A. R., Cortez, M. V., Scotta, A. V., & Soria, E. A. (2022). Dietary Intake of Polyphenols Enhances Executive/Attentional Functioning and Memory with an Improvement of the Milk Lipid Profile of Postpartum Women from Argentina. Journal of Intelligence, 10(2), 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10020033

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop