Actively Open-Minded Thinking and Its Measurement
Abstract
:1. Actively Open-Minded Thinking and Its Measurement
2. Twenty-Five Years Trying to Measure AOT
3. Examining a Wider Range of Correlates Leads to New Questions about AOT Scale Composition
- Beliefs should always be revised in response to new information or evidence.
- One should disregard evidence that conflicts with your established beliefs. (reverse-scored)
4. Scale Structure Changing over Time
5. Toward a New 13-Item Recommended AOT Scale
6. The AOT Measures Psychological Tendencies to Decouple and Decontextualize—A Critical Aspect of Modernity
7. The Paradox of AOT and Myside Thinking
8. Final Thoughts
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Optimal functioning does not result from maximizing cognitive styles. Instead, rationality, plotted against most thinking dispositions, is an inverted U-shaped function. One does not maximize rationality by maximizing the reflectivity/impulsivity dimension, for example, because a person doing so might get lost in interminable pondering and never make a decision. One does not maximize the dimension of belief flexibility either, because such a person might end up with a pathologically unstable personality. Reflectivity and belief flexibility are “good” cognitive styles only in the sense that most people are too low in both dimensions (Baron 2008). Most people would be more rational if they increased their degrees of reflectivity and belief flexibility, but this does not mean that either of these thinking dispositions should always be maximized. |
2 | Most of the other facets of openness, as measured in scales tapping the Big Five personality dimensions, are not related to AOT, as conceptualized in the rational thinking literature—for example, facets such as openness to fantasy, aesthetics, feelings, and actions. |
3 | For example, the comprehensive intellectual humility scale developed by Krumrei-Mancuso and Rouse (2016) includes five items measuring belief identification (e.g., “I feel small when others disagree with me on topics that are close to my heart”, reverse-scored), five items measuring belief revision (e.g., “I am open to revising my important beliefs in the face of new information”), six items measuring respect for the opinion of others (e.g., “I can have great respect for someone, even when we don’t see eye-to-eye on important topics”), and six items measuring the avoidance of overconfidence (e.g., “My ideas are usually better than other people’s ideas”). Likewise, the short humility scale studied by Leary et al. (2017) mixes belief revision items (“I reconsider my opinions when presented with new evidence”) with items tapping respect for alternative opinions (“I recognize the value in opinions that are different from my own”). |
4 | Our characterization of heuristics and biases tasks as implicating decoupling holds only for those instances where the task truly does create a hostile environment (see Stanovich 2018) for a particular subject because the normative response has not been practiced to automaticity. In cases where the normative response has been practiced to an automaticity so great that it dominates the intuitive response, there is no response conflict and cognitive decoupling is not required (see De Neys 2023; Thompson et al. 2018). |
5 | Most of the criticisms of this study by Baron (2017) would not reverse the direction of the association between polarization and AOT. They would, instead, drive the association toward zero, thus making the Kahan and Corbin (2016) results more consistent with the conclusion drawn in this section. |
6 |
References
- Abelson, Robert P. 1986. Beliefs are like possessions. Journal of the Theory of Social Behaviour 16: 223–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abelson, Robert P. 1988. Conviction. American Psychologist 43: 267–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abelson, Robert P., and Deborah A. Prentice. 1989. Beliefs as possessions: A functional perspective. In Attitudes, Structure, and Function. Edited by Anthony R. Pratkanis, Steven J. Breckler and Anthony G. Greenwald. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 361–81. [Google Scholar]
- Ackerman, Rakefet, and Valerie Thompson. 2017. Meta-reasoning: Monitoring and control of thinking and reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21: 607–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alexander, Scott. 2021. Book Review: The Scout Mindset. Astral Codex Ten, September 29. Available online: https://astralcodexten.substack.com/p/book-review-the-scout-mindset(accessed on 13 January 2023).
- Baron, Jonathan. 1985. Rationality and Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, Jonathan. 1988. Thinking and Deciding, 1st ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, Jonathan. 1993. Why teach thinking?—An essay. Applied Psychology: An International Review 42: 191–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron, Jonathan. 2008. Thinking and Deciding, 4th ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, Jonathan. 2017. Comment on Kahan and Corbin: Can polarization increase with actively open-minded thinking? Research & Politics 4: 2053168016688122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baron, Jonathan. 2019. Actively open-minded thinking in politics. Cognition 188: 8–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baron, Jonathan, and Mark Spranca. 1997. Protected values. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 70: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron, Jonathan, and Sarah Leshner. 2000. How serious are expressions of protected values? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 6: 183–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron, Jonathan, Burcu Gürçay, and S. Emlen Metz. 2017. Reflective thought and actively open-minded thinking. In Individual Differences in Judgment and Decision Making. Edited by Maggie E. Toplak and Joshua Weller. London: Psychology Press, pp. 107–26. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, Jonathan, Ozan Isler, and Onurcan Yilmaz. 2023. Actively open-minded thinking and the political effects of its absence. In Divided: Open-Mindedness and Dogmatism in a Polarized World. Edited by Victor Ottati and Chadly Stern. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, Jonathan, Sydney Scott, Katrina Fincher, and S. Emlen Metz. 2015. Why does the Cognitive Reflection Test (sometimes) predict utilitarian moral judgment (and other things)? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 4: 265–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bartels, Daniel M., and Douglas L. Medin. 2007. Are morally-motivated decision makers insensitive to the consequences of their choices? Psychological Science 18: 24–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bazerman, Max H. 2001. Consumer research for consumers. Journal of Consumer Research 27: 499–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beatty, Erin L., and Valerie A. Thompson. 2012. Effects of perspective and belief on analytic reasoning in a scientific reasoning task. Thinking & Reasoning 18: 441–60. [Google Scholar]
- Binnendyk, Jabin, and Gordon Pennycook. 2022. Intuition, reason, and conspiracy beliefs. Current Opinion in Psychology 47: 101387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bronstein, Michael V., Gordon Pennycook, Adam Bear, David G. Rand, and Tyrone D. Cannon. 2019. Belief in fake news is associated with delusionality, dogmatism, religious fundamentalism, and reduced analytic thinking. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 8: 108–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruine de Bruin, Wändi, Andrew M. Parker, and Baruch Fischhoff. 2007. Individual differences in adult decision-making competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92: 938–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campitelli, Guillermo, and Paul Gerrans. 2014. Does the cognitive reflection test measure cognitive reflection? A mathematical modeling approach. Memory & Cognition 42: 434–47. [Google Scholar]
- Ceci, Steven, and Wendy Williams. 2018. Who decides what is acceptable speech on campus? Why restricting free speech is not the answer. Perspectives on Psychological Science 13: 299–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cederblom, Jerry. 1989. Willingness to reason and the identification of the self. In Thinking, Reasoning, and Writing. Edited by E. Maimon, B. Nodine and F. O’Conner. New York: Longman, pp. 147–59. [Google Scholar]
- Charney, Evan. 2015. Liberal bias and the five-factor model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38: e139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Clements, Zakary A., and Geoffrey D. Munro. 2021. Biases and their impact on opinions of transgender bathroom usage. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 51: 370–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa, Paul T., and Robert R. McCrae. 1992. Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
- Crawford, Jarret, and Lee Jussim, eds. 2018. The Politics of Social Psychology. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- De Neys, Wim. 2014. Conflict detection, dual processes, and logical intuitions: Some clarifications. Thinking & Reasoning 20: 169–87. [Google Scholar]
- De Neys, Wim. 2023. Advancing theorizing about fast-and-slow thinking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Deniz, Hasan, Lisa Donnelly, and Irfan Yilmaz. 2008. Exploring the factors related to acceptance of evolutionary theory among Turkish preservice biology teachers. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 45: 420–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ditto, Peter H., Brittany S. Liu, and Sean P. Wojcik. 2012. Is anything sacred anymore? Psychological Inquiry 23: 155–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ditto, Peter H., Brittany S. Liu, Cory J. Clark, Sean P. Wojcik, Eric E. Chen, Rebecca H. Grady, Jared B. Celniker, and Joanne F. Zinger. 2019. At least bias Is bipartisan: A meta-analytic comparison of partisan bias in liberals and conservatives. Perspectives on Psychological Science 14: 273–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Duarte, José L., Jarret T. Crawford, Charlotta Stern, Jonathan Haidt, Lee Jussim, and Philip E. Tetlock. 2015. Political diversity will improve social psychological science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38: e130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eichmeier, April, and Neil Stenhouse. 2019. Differences that don’t make much difference: Party asymmetry in open-minded cognitive styles has little relationship to information processing behavior. Research & Politics 6: 2053168019872045. [Google Scholar]
- Einhorn, Hillel J., and Robin M. Hogarth. 1981. Behavioral decision theory: Processes of judgment and choice. Annual Review of Psychology 32: 53–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellis, J. M. 2020. The Breakdown of Higher Education. New York: Encounter. [Google Scholar]
- Epstein, Seymour, and Petra Meier. 1989. Constructive thinking: A broad coping variable with specific components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57: 332–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Erceg, Nikola, Zvonimir Galić, and Andreja Bubić. 2022. Normative responding on cognitive bias tasks: Some evidence for a weak rationality factor that is mostly explained by numeracy and actively open-minded thinking. Intelligence 90: 101619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, Jonathan St B. T. 2007. Hypothetical Thinking: Dual Processes in Reasoning and Judgment. New York: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, Jonathan St B. T. 2010. Thinking Twice: Two Minds in One Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, Jonathan St B. T. 2014. Reasoning, Rationality and Dual Processes. London: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, Jonathan St B. T. 2017. Belief bias in deductive reasoning. In Cognitive Illusions, 2nd ed. Edited by Rüdiger Pohl. London: Routledge, pp. 165–81. [Google Scholar]
- Evans, Jonathan St B. T., and Keith E. Stanovich. 2013. Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science 8: 223–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fazio, Russell H. 2007. Attitudes as object–evaluation associations of varying strength. Social Cognition 25: 603–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Finucane, Melissa L., and Christina M. Gullion. 2010. Developing a tool for measuring the decision-making competence of older adults. Psychology and Aging 25: 271–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fisher, Matthew, and Frank C. Keil. 2014. The illusion of argument justification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143: 425–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Goldman, Alvin I. 1986. Epistemology and Cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gould, Stephen Jay. 1991. Bully for the Brontosaurus. New York: Norton. [Google Scholar]
- Guay, Brian, and Christopher D. Johnston. 2022. Ideological asymmetries and the determinants of politically motivated reasoning. American Journal of Political Science 66: 285–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hahn, Ulrike, and Adam J. L. Harris. 2014. What does it mean to be biased: Motivated reasoning and rationality. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Edited by Gordon H. Ross. New York: Academic Press, vol. 61, pp. 41–102. [Google Scholar]
- Haidt, Jonathan. 2022. The Two Fiduciary Duties of Professors. Heterodox Blog, September 20. Available online: https://heterodoxacademy.org/blog/the-two-fiduciary-duties-of-professors/(accessed on 13 January 2023).
- Haran, Uriel, Ilana Ritov, and Barbara A. Mellers. 2013. The role of actively open-minded thinking in information acquisition, accuracy, and calibration. Judgment and Decision Making 8: 188–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harman, Gilbert. 1995. Rationality. In Thinking. Edited by Edward Smith and Daniel Osherson. Cambridge: The MIT Press, vol. 3, pp. 175–211. [Google Scholar]
- Howe, Lauren C., and Jon A. Krosnick. 2017. Attitude strength. Annual Review of Psychology 68: 327–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jastrzębski, Jan, and Adam Chuderski. 2022. Analytic thinking outruns fluid reasoning in explaining rejection of pseudoscience, paranormal, and conspiracist beliefs. Intelligence 95: 101705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jussim, Lee. 2019. The Threat to Academic Freedom … from Academics. Medium, December 27. Available online: https://medium.com/@leej12255/the-threat-to-academic-freedom-from-academics-4685b1705794(accessed on 13 January 2023).
- Jussim, Lee. 2022. Diversity Is Diverse: Social Justice Reparations and Science. Substack, December 30. Available online: https://unsafescience.substack.com/p/diversity-is-diverse(accessed on 13 January 2023).
- Kahan, Dan M. 2016. The politically motivated reasoning paradigm, part 1: What politically motivated reasoning is and how to measure it. In Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource. Edited by Robert A. Scott and Stephen Michael Kosslyn. New York: Wiley Online library. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahan, Dan M., and Jonathan C. Corbin. 2016. A note on the perverse effects of actively open-minded thinking on climate-change polarization. Research & Politics 3: 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kahan, Dan M., Ellen Peters, Erica Cantrell Dawson, and Paul Slovic. 2017. Motivated numeracy and enlightened self-government. Behavioural Public Policy 1: 54–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, Daniel. 2011. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux. [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. 1973. On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review 80: 237–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kelman, Mark. 2011. The Heuristics Debate. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Koehler, Derek J., and Nigel Harvey, eds. 2004. Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Koehler, Jonathan J. 1993. The influence of prior beliefs on scientific judgments of evidence quality. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 56: 28–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kokis, Judite V., Robyn Macpherson, Maggie E. Toplak, Richard F. West, and Keith E. Stanovich. 2002. Heuristic and analytic processing: Age trends and associations with cognitive ability and cognitive styles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 83: 26–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kovacs, Kristof, and Andrew R. A. Conway. 2016. Process Overlap Theory: A Unified Account of the General Factor of Intelligence. Psychological Inquiry 27: 151–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krumrei-Mancuso, Elizabeth J., and Steven V. Rouse. 2016. The Development and Validation of the Comprehensive Intellectual Humility Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment 98: 209–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leary, Mark, Kate Diebels, Erin Davisson, Katrina Jongman-Sereno, Jennifer Isherwood, Kaitlin Raimi, Samantha Deffler, and Rick Hoyle. 2017. Cognitive and interpersonal features of intellectual humility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43: 793–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levin, Irwin P., Edward A. Wasserman, and Shu-fang Kao. 1993. Multiple methods of examining biased information use in contingency judgments. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 55: 228–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loewenstein, George, and Andras Molnar. 2018. The renaissance of belief-based utility in economics. Nature Human Behaviour 2: 166–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luce, R. Duncan, and Howard Raiffa. 1957. Games and Decisions. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Macpherson, Robyn, and Keith E. Stanovich. 2007. Cognitive ability, thinking dispositions, and instructional set as predictors of critical thinking. Learning and Individual Differences 17: 115–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manktelow, Ken I. 2012. Thinking and Reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mele, Alfred, and Piers Rawling, eds. 2004. The Oxford Handbook of Rationality. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mellers, Barbara, Eric Stone, Pavel Atanasov, Nick Rohrbaugh, S. Emlen Metz, Lyle Ungar, Michael M. Bishop, Michael Horowitz, Ed Merkle, and Philip Tetlock. 2015. The psychology of intelligence analysis: Drivers of prediction accuracy in world politics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 21: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Mercier, Hugo. 2017. Confirmation bias—Myside bias. In Cognitive Illusions, 2nd ed. Edited by Rüdiger Pohl. New York: Routledge, pp. 99–114. [Google Scholar]
- Miyake, Akira, and Naomi P. Friedman. 2012. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21: 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newton, Christie, Justin Feeney, and Gordon Pennycook. 2023. On the Disposition to Think Analytically: Four Distinct Intuitive-Analytic Thinking Styles. Manuscript. Available online: https://osf.io/7kf5b/ (accessed on 13 January 2023).
- Nigg, Joel. 2017. On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 58: 361–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nozick, Robert. 1993. The Nature of Rationality. Princeton: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Oaksford, Mike, and Nick Chater. 2012. Dual processes, probabilities, and cognitive architecture. Mind & Society 11: 15–26. [Google Scholar]
- Oliver, J. Eric, and Thomas J. Wood. 2014. Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion. American Journal of Political Science 58: 952–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parker, Andrew M., and Baruch Fischhoff. 2005. Decision-making competence: External validation through an individual differences approach. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 18: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennycook, Gordon, James Allan Cheyne, Derek J. Koehler, and Jonathan A. Fugelsang. 2020. On the belief that beliefs should change according to evidence: Implications for conspiratorial, moral, paranormal, political, religious, and science beliefs. Judgement and Decision Making 15: 476–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennycook, Gordon, James Allan Cheyne, Nathaniel Barr, Derek J. Koehler, and Jonathan A. Fugelsang. 2014. Cognitive style and religiosity: The role of conflict detection. Memory & Cognition 42: 1–10. [Google Scholar]
- Perkins, David N. 1985. Postprimary education has little impact on informal reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 562–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piazza, Jared, and Justin F. Landy. 2013. “Lean not on your own understanding”: Belief that morality is founded on divine authority and non-utilitarian moral thinking. Judgment and Decision Making 8: 639–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinker, Steven. 2021. Rationality. New York: Penguin. [Google Scholar]
- Pronin, Emily. 2007. Perception and misperception of bias in human judgment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 37–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Raoelison, Matthieu, Valerie A. Thompson, and Wim De Neys. 2020. The smart intuitor: Cognitive capacity predicts intuitive rather than deliberate thinking. Cognition 204: 104381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rauch, Jonathan. 2021. The Constitution of Knowledge: A Defense of Truth. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rizeq, Jala, David B. Flora, and Maggie E. Toplak. 2021. An examination of the underlying dimensional structure of three domains of contaminated mindware: Paranormal beliefs, conspiracy beliefs, and anti-science attitudes. Thinking & Reasoning 27: 187–211. [Google Scholar]
- Sa, Walter C., and Keith E. Stanovich. 2001. The domain specificity and generality of mental contamination: Accuracy and projection in judgments of mental content. British Journal of Psychology 92: 281–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sá, Walter C., Carol N. Kelley, Caroline Ho, and Keith E. Stanovich. 2005. Thinking about personal theories: Individual differences in the coordination of theory and evidence. Personality and Individual Differences 38: 1149–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sá, Walter C., Richard F. West, and Keith E. Stanovich. 1999. The domain specificity and generality of belief bias: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 497–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samuelson, Peter L., and Ian M. Church. 2015. When cognition turns vicious: Heuristics and biases in light of virtue epistemology. Philosophical Psychology 28: 1095–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Savage, Leonard J. 1954. The Foundations of Statistics. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Sharot, Tali, Max Rollwage, Cass R. Sunstein, and Stephen M. Fleming. 2023. Why and When Beliefs Change. Perspectives on Psychological Science 18: 142–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sinatra, Gale M., Sherry A. Southerland, Frances McConaughy, and James W. Demastes. 2003. Intentions and beliefs in students’ understanding and acceptance of biological evolution. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 40: 510–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skitka, Linda J., Christopher W. Bauman, and Edward G. Sargis. 2005. Moral conviction: Another contributor to attitude strength or something more? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88: 895–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stanovich, Keith E. 1999. Who Is Rational? Studies of Individual Differences in Reasoning. Mahwah: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E. 2004. The Robot’s Rebellion: Finding Meaning in the Age of Darwin. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E. 2008. Higher-order preferences and the Master Rationality Motive. Thinking & Reasoning 14: 111–27. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E. 2009. What Intelligence Tests Miss: The Psychology of Rational Thought. New Haven: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E. 2011. Rationality and the Reflective Mind. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E. 2012. On the distinction between rationality and intelligence: Implications for understanding individual differences in reasoning. In The Oxford Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. Edited by Keith J. Holyoak and Robert G. Morrison. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 343–65. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E. 2013. Why humans are (sometimes) less rational than other animals: Cognitive complexity and the axioms of rational choice. Thinking & Reasoning 19: 1–26. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E. 2017. Were the Trump Voters Irrational? Quillette, September 28. Available online: https://quillette.com/2017/09/28/trump-voters-irrational/(accessed on 13 January 2023).
- Stanovich, Keith E. 2018. Miserliness in human cognition: The interaction of detection, override and mindware. Thinking & Reasoning 24: 423–44. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E. 2021. The Bias That Divides Us: The Science and Politics of Myside Thinking. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E., and Maggie E. Toplak. 2012. Defining features versus incidental correlates of Type 1 and Type 2 processing. Mind & Society 11: 3–13. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E., and Maggie E. Toplak. 2019. The need for intellectual diversity in psychological science: Our own studies of actively open-minded thinking as a case study. Cognition 187: 156–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stanovich, Keith E., and Richard F. West. 1997. Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology 89: 342–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, Keith E., and Richard F. West. 1998a. Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 127: 161–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, Keith E., and Richard F. West. 1998b. Who uses base rates and P(D/~H)? An analysis of individual differences. Memory & Cognition 26: 161–79. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E., and Richard F. West. 2000. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23: 645–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stanovich, Keith E., and Richard F. West. 2007. Natural myside bias is independent of cognitive ability. Thinking & Reasoning 13: 225–47. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E., and Richard F. West. 2008. On the failure of intelligence to predict myside bias and one-sided bias. Thinking & Reasoning 14: 129–67. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith E., Richard F. West, and Maggie E. Toplak. 2013. Myside bias, rational thinking, and intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science 22: 259–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stanovich, Keith E., Richard F. West, and Maggie E. Toplak. 2016. The Rationality Quotient: Toward a Test of Rational Thinking. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Stenhouse, Neil, Teresa A. Myers, Emily K. Vraga, John E. Kotcher, Lindsey Beall, and Edward W. Maibach. 2018. The potential role of actively open-minded thinking in preventing motivated reasoning about controversial science. Journal of Environmental Psychology 57: 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Svedholm, Annika M., and Marjaana Lindeman. 2013. The separate roles of the reflective mind and involuntary inhibitory control in gatekeeping paranormal beliefs and the underlying intuitive confusions. British Journal of Psychology 104: 303–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Svedholm-Hakkinen, Annika M., and Marjaana Lindeman. 2018. Actively open-minded thinking: Development of a shortened scale and disentangling attitudes towards knowledge and people. Thinking & Reasoning 24: 21–40. [Google Scholar]
- Swami, Viren, Martin Voracek, Stefan Stieger, Ulrich S. Tran, and Adrian Furnham. 2014. Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition 133: 572–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swami, Viren, Rebecca Coles, Stefan Stieger, Jakob Pietschnig, Adrian Furnham, Sherry Rehim, and Martin Voracek. 2011. Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology 102: 443–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swami, Viren, Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic, and Adrian Furnham. 2010. Unanswered questions: A preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11 conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology 24: 749–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tetlock, Philip E. 2003. Thinking the unthinkable: Sacred values and taboo cognitions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 320–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Thompson, Valerie A., and Jonathan St B. T. Evans. 2012. Belief bias in informal reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning 18: 278–310. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, Valerie A., Gordon Pennycook, Dries Trippas, and Jonathan St B. T. Evans. 2018. Do smart people have better intuitions? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 147: 945–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, Valerie A., Jamie A. Prowse Turner, Gordon Pennycook, Linden J. Ball, Hannah Brack, Yael Ophir, and Rakefet Ackerman. 2013. The role of answer fluency and perceptual fluency as metacognitive cues for initiating analytic thinking. Cognition 128: 237–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toplak, Maggie E., and Keith E. Stanovich. 2002. The domain specificity and generality of disjunctive reasoning: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology 94: 197–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toplak, Maggie E., and Keith E. Stanovich. 2003. Associations between myside bias on an informal reasoning task and amount of post-secondary education. Applied Cognitive Psychology 17: 851–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toplak, Maggie E., Eleanor Liu, Robyn MacPherson, Tony Toneatto, and Keith E. Stanovich. 2007. The reasoning skills and thinking dispositions of problem gamblers: A dual-process taxonomy. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 20: 103–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toplak, Maggie E., Richard F. West, and Keith E. Stanovich. 2011. The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics and biases tasks. Memory & Cognition 39: 1275–89. [Google Scholar]
- Toplak, Maggie E., Richard F. West, and Keith E. Stanovich. 2014a. Assessing miserly processing: An expansion of the Cognitive Reflection Test. Thinking & Reasoning 20: 147–68. [Google Scholar]
- Toplak, Maggie E., Richard F. West, and Keith E. Stanovich. 2014b. Rational thinking and cognitive sophistication: Development, cognitive abilities, and thinking dispositions. Developmental Psychology 50: 1037–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1974. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science 185: 1124–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1983. Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review 90: 293–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Viator, Ralph E., Nancy L. Harp, Shannon B. Rinaldo, and Blair B. Marquardt. 2020. The mediating effect of reflective-analytic cognitive style on rational thought. Thinking & Reasoning 26: 381–413. [Google Scholar]
- Weller, Joshua, Andrea Ceschi, Lauren Hirsch, Riccardo Sartori, and Arianna Costantini. 2018. Accounting for individual differences in decision-making competence: Personality and gender differences. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 2258–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- West, Richard F., Maggie E. Toplak, and Keith E. Stanovich. 2008. Heuristics and biases as measures of critical thinking: Associations with cognitive ability and thinking dispositions. Journal of Educational Psychology 100: 930–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yelbuz, Büşra Elif, Ecesu Madan, and Sinan Alper. 2022. Reflective thinking predicts lower conspiracy beliefs: A meta-analysis. Judgment and Decision Making 1: 720–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yilmaz, Onurcan, and S. Adil Saribay. 2017. The relationship between cognitive style and political orientation depends on the measures used. Judgment & Decision Making 12: 140–47. [Google Scholar]
People should always take into consideration evidence that goes against their opinions. | BR |
Changing your mind is a sign of weakness. (R) | REF/ALT/BR/OC |
I like to think that my actions are motivated by sound reasons. | REASONS |
It is important to stick to your opinions even when evidence is brought to bear against them. (R) | BR/OC/BEL_ID |
Intuition is the best guide in making decisions. (R) | REF |
Considering too many different opinions often leads to muddled thinking. (R) | AMB/ALT/OC |
One should disregard evidence that conflicts with your current opinions. (R) | BR/ALT |
Coming to decisions quickly is a sign of wisdom. (R) | REF |
Allowing oneself to be convinced by a solid opposing argument is a sign of good character. | BR/BEL_ID/OC |
If something I think feels right then I am comfortable, whether or not it is true. (R) | REF/TRUTH |
A person should always consider new information. | ALT |
People should revise their conclusions in response to relevant new information. | BR |
Certain opinions are just too important to abandon no matter how good a case can be made against them. (R) | BR |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Stanovich, K.E.; Toplak, M.E. Actively Open-Minded Thinking and Its Measurement. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11020027
Stanovich KE, Toplak ME. Actively Open-Minded Thinking and Its Measurement. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(2):27. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11020027
Chicago/Turabian StyleStanovich, Keith E., and Maggie E. Toplak. 2023. "Actively Open-Minded Thinking and Its Measurement" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 2: 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11020027