Next Article in Journal
Do Not Worry That Generative AI May Compromise Human Creativity or Intelligence in the Future: It Already Has
Previous Article in Journal
Delving into the Complexity of Analogical Reasoning: A Detailed Exploration with the Generalized Multicomponent Latent Trait Model for Diagnosis
Previous Article in Special Issue
Anxiety about Mathematics and Reading in Preadolescents Is Domain-Specific
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Editorial

The Interplay between Motivational, Affective Factors and Cognitive Factors in Learning: Editorial

by
Brenda R. J. Jansen
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, 1018 WS Amsterdam, The Netherlands
J. Intell. 2024, 12(7), 68; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12070068
Submission received: 17 July 2024 / Accepted: 18 July 2024 / Published: 19 July 2024
Academic success is assumed to be both the start and outcome of a cycle in which affect, motivation, and effort strengthen each other (Vu et al. 2022). But how exactly do affective, motivational, and cognitive factors interact in learning? Ashcraft and Kirk’s (2001) experiments are exemplary demonstrations of the interplay between affective and cognitive factors in academic learning, in this case, mathematics anxiety and working memory. Through cleverly designed experiments, Ashcraft and Kirk showed that mathematics anxiety reduces working memory, which then cannot be used for solving a mathematics problem, thereby lowering mathematics performance. Through follow-up studies, including by other researchers and in younger age groups, it became increasingly clear that learners with different cognitive abilities (e.g., strong working memory) apply different problem-solving strategies, taxing working memory to varying extents, which made them more or less susceptible to mathematics anxiety. This affected mathematics tasks differently, dependent on the extent to which the tasks taxed working memory (for an overview, see, e.g., Beilock and Ramirez 2011). Eysenck et al. (2007) provide an extensive theoretical account and an overview of empirical evidence of how anxiety can impair performance, including in other academic domains.
This Special Issue presents studies that show an interplay between affective and cognitive factors in learning. Motivational factors were also considered, as motivation and academic emotions are tightly connected and are both related to academic performance (Pekrun 2006; Vu et al. 2022). This editorial presents a concise introduction to the studies and highlights some specific findings.
The studies in this Special Issue show that the interplay between affective, motivational, and cognitive factors in learning can have different forms. Van der Ven et al. (2023) show an interaction between working memory and mathematics anxiety. That is, the negative relation between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance strengthened with better working memory in one task. In other studies, the interplay is a mediating effect. That is, an affective or motivational factor is expected to result in effort (e.g., practice behaviour, investing time in school work), which should then be positively related to academic performance (Hilz et al. 2023; Liu et al. 2023). Other studies investigate factors that influence affect and motivation, such as the academic domain (Sasanguie et al. 2024), task demands (Kramer and Huizenga 2023; O’Connor et al. 2023; Van der Ven et al. 2023), school demands (Zhang and Jiang 2023), or academic buoyancy (Putwain et al. 2023).
The empirical studies in this Special Issue were conducted in school settings, in children and adolescents. Some samples were impressively large, and some studies used data collected by school institutions as part of standard practice, for example, to monitor pupils’ performance (Hilz et al. 2023; Liu et al. 2023; O’Connor et al. 2023; Sasanguie et al. 2024; Van der Ven et al. 2023; Zhang and Jiang 2023). Studying the interplay between affective, motivational, and cognitive factors in the school setting itself, using actual monitoring data, promotes the generalisability of the results. In this way, experimental studies such as those by Ashcraft and Kirk (2001), which often took place in controlled settings with tasks specifically designed for the research question, are translated to daily school life. Such translations contribute to the relevance of the findings and enrich the field.
Strikingly, half of the studies in this Special Issue concern the domain of learning mathematics and focus on mathematics anxiety. Of course, when a guest editor asks researchers in their network for contributions to a Special Issue, many contributors will be active in the same research area, in my case learning mathematics. Yet, contributions from other academic domains were explicitly asked for. The attention on mathematics anxiety highlights that mathematics is especially associated with (mostly negative) academic emotions and that motivation for mathematics is uncommon, at least in Western students. How can we explain mathematics’ bad name? Below is a list of reasons, which is by no means intended to be complete. First, mathematics requires the active use of working memory and other executive functions (Bull and Scerif 2001), making it a strenuous task. Second, mathematics problems require a multitude of skills, not only executive functions and basic number knowledge but also creativity in problem-solving. Relatedly, there is no fix-all solution for mathematics problems. Although various strategies can be correct, their efficiency depends on the mathematics problem at hand, and it takes experience to determine which strategy is both correct and efficient. Becoming better at mathematics thus involves time and effort. Third, mathematical elements build on each other, and understanding basic concepts is required to understand more complex concepts, making it difficult to catch up when missing or not understanding a concept introduced earlier. Perhaps because of these technical aspects, mathematics has become associated with certain beliefs, or so-called “math myths” (Barlow and Reddish 2006). These myths state that mathematics is not for all, that men are better in mathematics than women, or that “there is a magic key to doing math”, for example (Barlow and Reddish 2006, p. 149). Yet, whether such beliefs are universally endorsed is a topic for future research (Cvencek et al. 2011; see also this Special Issue: Liu et al. 2023; Zhang and Jiang 2023).
This Special Issue shows that certain aspects of schools or academic tasks can worsen academic emotions and motivation. I discuss them from macro to micro levels. First, Zhang and Jiang (2023) mention that school demands may affect both satisfaction and frustration of the basic motivational needs of autonomy, relatedness, and competence. The authors note that the Chinese high school environment is quite demanding and competitive, which may explain why some students may focus on extrinsic goals and perceive low autonomy. Second, Sasanguie et al. (2024) highlight that anxiety is domain-specific. They show that mathematics performance is mainly related to mathematics anxiety, in comparison to reading anxiety and test anxiety. However, they do stress that academic anxieties are correlated and that one should be alert to anxiety in other academic domains when anxiety is seen in a particular academic domain. Third, task demands are related to academic emotions and motivation. Lengthy tasks lower motivation (Kramer and Huizenga 2023), and a high-stakes, complex mathematics task showed a stronger negative relation with performance than a low-stakes, simple mathematics task (Van der Ven et al. 2023). Also, mathematics anxiety had a stronger negative relation with mathematics performance in tasks that require application of the formal, symbolic number system, compared to non-symbolic tasks (O’Connor et al. 2023).
The studies in this Special Issue show that the operationalisation of concepts is non-trivial. Although mathematics anxiety was operationalised similarly (based on the assumption that mathematics anxiety is a trait, using self-reports—even in young children), motivation, effort, and academic performance were operationalised differently. Motivation was assessed using self-reports but focused on the willingness to continue working on school work (Kramer and Huizenga 2023), value attached to education (Liu et al. 2023), self-concept (Hilz et al. 2023), or satisfaction and frustration with basic needs (Zhang and Jiang 2023). Effort was operationalised using a measure of practice behaviour frequency in an online practice programme (Hilz et al.) or self-reported time investment in learning (Liu et al.). Finally, performance was also operationalised differently. Kramer and Huizenga used a score on a test that might affect motivation, which was assessed right after the test. Other studies assessed performance in separate situations using a specific standardised test (Hilz et al.; O’Connor et al. 2023; Sasanguie et al. 2024; Van der Ven et al. 2023), self-rated performance (Liu et al.), or scores from schools’ monitoring or assessment systems (Liu et al., Sasanguie et al., Van der Ven et al., Zhang and Jiang). With such diverse operationalisations, different outcomes on the interplay between affective, motivational, and cognitive factors in learning are conceivable.
The choice of statistical model is also non-trivial and may also result in different conclusions on the interplay. Liu et al. (2023) show that the dynamics of motivational and affective factors and performance can be described by classic cross-lagged panel models, as well as models with a random intercept or random curves, which control for stable, individual differences between learners. The statistical model determined whether a reciprocal relationship between motivation and performance was seen.
Individual differences in affect, motivation, achievement, and effort remain quite stable over time (Liu et al. 2023). This stability is surprising given that learners’ lives are full of events and daily “setbacks”, which may differ between individuals. Setbacks include disappointing results and dips in motivation (Putwain et al. 2023). A possible explanation for stability despite the individual differences in the dynamics of learners’ day-to-day lives is academic buoyancy: the extent to which an individual can bounce back after experiencing a setback. However, Putwain et al. show that results on the relation between academic buoyancy, and academic achievement are mixed. An important explanation for varying results may be that the timing of measuring setbacks, academic buoyancy, and academic performance is crucial. Setbacks, bouncing back, and academic performance recovery are unlikely to occur at the same time. Putwain et al. recommend time-intensive studies to capture these dynamics.
Detecting significant relations in the interplay between affective, motivational, and cognitive factors in learning may be a starting point for interventions. Caution is however needed. Bailey et al. (2018) warn against intervening on early predictors of later skills, one reason being that the relation can be explained by underlying, unmeasured factors. For example, child–parent relations may play a role in both a child’s level of reported mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance (Casad et al. 2015), which might restrict the effects of a mathematics anxiety intervention on mathematics performance. Also, one must consider the selected operationalisation of the factors. For example, one performance intervention is an adaptive computerised practice programme that tailors problems to an individual’s ability, thereby levelling positive feedback for all pupils (Hilz et al. 2023; Jansen et al. 2013). However, this may not lower mathematics anxiety or boost mathematics self-concept because pupils also use information like class rank and exam scores to evaluate their performance, which may then feed mathematics anxiety and mathematics self-concept.
An editorial is too short to do justice to all the individual studies. I invite the readers to dive into the studies that together offer an impressive array of research designs, age groups, and statistical techniques. All empirical studies took place in schools, complicating the implementation of a strict experimental design. The results convincingly show how the interplay between affective, motivational, and cognitive factors matters in everyday learning in schools.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ashcraft, Mark H., and Elizabeth P. Kirk. 2001. The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130: 224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Bailey, Drew H., Greg J. Duncan, Tyler Watts, Doug H. Clements, and Julie Sarama. 2018. Risky business: Correlation and causation in longitudinal studies of skill development. American Psychologist 73: 81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Barlow, Angela T., and Jill Mizell Reddish. 2006. Mathematical myths: Teacher candidates’ beliefs and the implications for teacher educators. The Teacher Educator 41: 145–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Beilock, Sian L., and Gerardo Ramirez. 2011. On the interplay of emotion and cognitive control: Implications for enhancing academic achievement. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Cambridge: Academic Press, vol. 55, pp. 137–69. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bull, Rebecca, and Gaia Scerif. 2001. Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology 19: 273–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Casad, Bettina J., Patricia Hale, and Faye L. Wachs. 2015. Parent-child math anxiety and math-gender stereotypes predict adolescents’ math education outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Cvencek, Dario, Andrew N. Meltzoff, and Anthony G. Greenwald. 2011. Math–gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Development 82: 766–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Eysenck, Michael W., Nazanin Derakshan, Rita Santos, and Manuel G. Calvo. 2007. Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion 7: 336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Hilz, Anna, Karin Guill, Janina Roloff, Daniel Sommerhoff, and Karen Aldrup. 2023. How to Continue? New Approaches to Investigating the Effects of Adaptive Math Learning Programs on Students’ Performance, Self-Concept, and Anxiety. Journal of Intelligence 11: 108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Jansen, Brenda R. J., Jolien Louwerse, Marthe Straatemeier, Sanne H. G. Van der Ven, Sharon Klinkenberg, and Han L. J. Van der Maas. 2013. The influence of experiencing success in math on math anxiety, perceived math competence, and math performance. Learning and Individual Differences 24: 190–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kramer, Anne-Wil, and Hilde M. Huizenga. 2023. Raven’s standard progressive matrices for adolescents: A case for a shortened version. Journal of Intelligence 11: 72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Liu, Meimei, TuongVan Vu, Nienke van Atteveldt, and Martijn Meeter. 2023. Testing the reciprocal effect between value of education, time investment, and academic achievement in a large non-western sample. Journal of Intelligence 11: 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. O’Connor, Patrick A., Kinga Morsanyi, and Teresa McCormack. 2023. Basic symbolic number skills, but not formal mathematics performance, longitudinally predict mathematics anxiety in the first years of primary school. Journal of Intelligence 11: 211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Pekrun, Reinhard. 2006. The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational Psychology Review 18: 315–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Putwain, David William, Joost Jansen in de Wal, and Thijmen van Alphen. 2023. Academic buoyancy: Overcoming test anxiety and setbacks. Journal of Intelligence 11: 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Sasanguie, Delphine, Charlotte Larmuseau, Fien Depaepe, and Brenda R. J. Jansen. 2024. Anxiety about mathematics and reading in preadolescents is domain-specific. Journal of Intelligence 12: 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Van der Ven, Sanne H. G., Emilie J. Prast, and Eva Van de Weijer-Bergsma. 2023. Towards an integrative model of math cognition: Interactions between working memory and emotions in explaining children’s math performance. Journal of Intelligence 11: 136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Vu, TuongVan, Lucía Magis-Weinberg, Brenda R. J. Jansen, Nienke van Atteveldt, Tieme W. P. Janssen, Nikki C. Lee, Han L. J. van der Maas, Maartje E. J. Raijmakers, Maien S. M. Sachisthal, and Martijn Meeter. 2022. Motivation-achievement cycles in learning: A literature review and research agenda. Educational Psychology Review 34: 39–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Zhang, Linjia, and Yi Jiang. 2023. Patterns of the satisfaction and frustration of psychological needs and their associations with adolescent students’ school affect, burnout, and achievement. Journal of Intelligence 11: 111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jansen, B.R.J. The Interplay between Motivational, Affective Factors and Cognitive Factors in Learning: Editorial. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12070068

AMA Style

Jansen BRJ. The Interplay between Motivational, Affective Factors and Cognitive Factors in Learning: Editorial. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(7):68. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12070068

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jansen, Brenda R. J. 2024. "The Interplay between Motivational, Affective Factors and Cognitive Factors in Learning: Editorial" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 7: 68. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12070068

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop