The Great Debate: General Ability and Specific Abilities in the Prediction of Important Outcomes
Abstract
:1. Introduction to the Special Issue
“To state one argument is not necessarily to be deaf to all others.”—Robert Louis Stevenson [1] (p. 11).
- Do the data present evidence for the usefulness of specific abilities?
- How important are specific abilities relative to general abilities for predicting grades?
- To what degree could (or should) researchers use different prediction models for each of the different outcome criteria?
2. Data Provided
3. Theoretical Motivation
4. Editorial Note on the Contributions
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Stevenson, R.L. An Apology for Idlers and Other Essays; Thomas B. Mosher: Portland, ME, USA, 1916. [Google Scholar]
- Danziger, K. Naming the Mind: How Psychology Found Its Language; Sage: London, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Sharp, S.E. Individual psychology: A study in psychological method. Am. J. Psychol. 1899, 10, 329–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wissler, C. The correlation of mental and physical tests. Psychol. Rev. 1901, 3, i-62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Binet, A.; Simon, T. New methods for the diagnosis of the intellectual level of subnormals. L’Annee Psychol. 1905, 12, 191–244. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, W.H. After Binet: French intelligence testing, 1900–1950. J. Hist. Behav. Sci. 1992, 28, 111–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benjamin, L.T. Hugo Münsterberg: Portrait of an applied psychologist. In Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology; Kimble, G.A., Wertheimer, M., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2000; Volume 4, pp. 113–129. [Google Scholar]
- Kell, H.J.; Lubinski, D. Spatial ability: A neglected talent in educational and occupational settings. Roeper Rev. 2013, 35, 219–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kevles, D.J. Testing the Army’s intelligence: Psychologists and the military in World War I. J. Am. Hist. 1968, 55, 565–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moskowitz, M.J. Hugo Münsterberg: A study in the history of applied psychology. Am. Psychol. 1977, 32, 824–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bingham, W.V. On the possibility of an applied psychology. Psychol. Rev. 1923, 30, 289–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Katzell, R.A.; Austin, J.T. From then to now: The development of industrial-organizational psychology in the United States. J. Appl. Psychol. 1992, 77, 803–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sackett, P.R.; Lievens, F.; Van Iddekinge, C.H.; Kuncel, N.R. Individual differences and their measurement: A review of 100 years of research. J. Appl. Psychol. 2017, 102, 254–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Terman, L.M. The status of applied psychology in the United States. J. Appl. Psychol. 1921, 5, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gardner, H. Who owns intelligence? Atl. Mon. 1999, 283, 67–76. [Google Scholar]
- Gardner, H.E. Intelligence Reframed: Multiple Intelligences for the 21st Century; Hachette UK: London, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, R.J. (Ed.) North American approaches to intelligence. In International Handbook of Intelligence; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2004; pp. 411–444. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, R.J. Testing: For better and worse. Phi Delta Kappan 2016, 98, 66–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kell, H.J.; Lang, J.W.B. Specific abilities in the workplace: More important than g? J. Intell. 2017, 5, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lang, J.W.B.; Kersting, M.; Hülsheger, U.R.; Lang, J. General mental ability, narrower cognitive abilities, and job performance: The perspective of the nested-factors model of cognitive abilities. Pers. Psychol. 2010, 63, 595–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thorndike, R.M.; Lohman, D.F. A Century of Ability Testing; Riverside: Chicago, IL, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, K. What can we learn from “Not Much More than g”? J. Intell. 2017, 5, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olea, M.M.; Ree, M.J. Predicting pilot and navigator criteria: Not much more than g. J. Appl. Psychol. 1994, 79, 845–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ree, M.J.; Earles, J.A. Predicting training success: Not much more than g. Pers. Psychol. 1991, 44, 321–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ree, M.J.; Earles, J.A. Predicting occupational criteria: Not much more than g. In Human Abilities: Their Nature and Measurement; Dennis, I., Tapsfield, P., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1996; pp. 151–165. [Google Scholar]
- Ree, M.J.; Earles, J.A.; Teachout, M.S. Predicting job performance: Not much more than g. J. Appl. Psychol. 1994, 79, 518–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowman, D.B.; Markham, P.M.; Roberts, R.D. Expanding the frontier of human cognitive abilities: So much more than (plain) g! Learn. Individ. Differ. 2002, 13, 127–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, K.R. Individual differences and behavior in organizations: Much more than g. In Individual Differences and Behavior in Organizations; Murphy, K., Ed.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1996; pp. 3–30. [Google Scholar]
- Stankov, L. g: A diminutive general. In The General Factor of Intelligence: How General Is It? Sternberg, R.J., Grigorenko, E.L., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2002; pp. 19–37. [Google Scholar]
- Gustafsson, J.-E.; Balke, G. General and specific abilities as predictors of school achievement. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1993, 28, 407–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- LePine, J.A.; Hollenbeck, J.R.; Ilgen, D.R.; Hedlund, J. Effects of individual differences on the performance of hierarchical decision-making teams: Much more than g. J. Appl. Psychol. 1997, 82, 803–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levine, E.L.; Spector, P.E.; Menon, S.; Narayanan, L. Validity generalization for cognitive, psychomotor, and perceptual tests for craft jobs in the utility industry. Hum. Perform. 1996, 9, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reeve, C.L. Differential ability antecedents of general and specific dimensions of declarative knowledge: More than g. Intelligence 2004, 32, 621–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, K.R.; Cronin, B.E.; Tam, A.P. Controversy and consensus regarding the use of cognitive ability testing in organizations. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 660–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reeve, C.L.; Charles, J.E. Survey of opinions on the primacy of g and social consequences of ability testing: A comparison of expert and non-expert views. Intelligence 2008, 36, 681–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R. Ability tilt for whites and blacks: Support for differentiation and investment theories. Intelligence 2016, 56, 28–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R. Non-g residuals of group factors predict ability tilt, college majors, and jobs: A non-g nexus. Intelligence 2018, 67, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R.; Pillow, D.R. SAT and ACT predict college GPA after removing g. Intelligence 2008, 36, 719–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R.; Purcell, J.M.; Snyder, A.C.; Richmond, M.C. Ability tilt on the SAT and ACT predicts specific abilities and college majors. Intelligence 2014, 46, 18–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R.; Snyder, A.C.; Richmond, M.C. Sex differences in ability tilt: Support for investment theory. Intelligence 2015, 50, 209–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R.; Snyder, A.C.; Richmond, M.C.; Little, M. SAT non-g residuals predict course specific GPAs: Support for investment theory. Intelligence 2015, 51, 57–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kell, H.J.; Lubinski, D.; Benbow, C.P. Who rises to the top? Early indicators. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 648–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kell, H.J.; Lubinski, D.; Benbow, C.P.; Steiger, J.H. Creativity and technical innovation: Spatial ability’s unique role. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 1831–1836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lang, J.W.B.; Bliese, P.D. I–O psychology and progressive research programs on intelligence. Ind. Organ. Psychol. 2012, 5, 161–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Makel, M.C.; Kell, H.J.; Lubinski, D.; Putallaz, M.; Benbow, C.P. When lightning strikes twice: Profoundly gifted, profoundly accomplished. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 27, 1004–1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Park, G.; Lubinski, D.; Benbow, C.P. Contrasting intellectual patterns predict creativity in the arts and sciences: Tracking intellectually precocious youth over 25 years. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 18, 948–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stanhope, D.S.; Surface, E.A. Examining the incremental validity and relative importance of specific cognitive abilities in a training context. J. Pers. Psychol. 2014, 13, 146–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wai, J.; Lubinski, D.; Benbow, C.P. Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. J. Educ. Psychol. 2009, 101, 817–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ziegler, M.; Dietl, E.; Danay, E.; Vogel, M.; Bühner, M. Predicting training success with general mental ability, specific ability tests, and (Un) structured interviews: A meta-analysis with unique samples. Int. J. Sel. Assess. 2011, 19, 170–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lievens, F.; Reeve, C.L. Where I–O psychology should really (re)start its investigation of intelligence constructs and their measurement. Ind. Organ. Psychol. 2012, 5, 153–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, T.R. Predictive validity of non-g residuals of tests: More than g. J. Intell. 2014, 2, 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flynn, J.R. Reflections about Intelligence over 40 Years. Intelligence 2018. Available online: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289618300904?dgcid=raven_sd_aip_email (accessed on 31 August 2018).
- Reeve, C.L.; Scherbaum, C.; Goldstein, H. Manifestations of intelligence: Expanding the measurement space to reconsider specific cognitive abilities. Hum. Resour. Manag. Rev. 2015, 25, 28–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ritchie, S.J.; Bates, T.C.; Deary, I.J. Is education associated with improvements in general cognitive ability, or in specific skills? Devel. Psychol. 2015, 51, 573–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schneider, W.J.; Newman, D.A. Intelligence is multidimensional: Theoretical review and implications of specific cognitive abilities. Hum. Resour. Manag. Rev. 2015, 25, 12–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krumm, S.; Schmidt-Atzert, L.; Lipnevich, A.A. Insights beyond g: Specific cognitive abilities at work. J. Pers. Psychol. 2014, 13, 117–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wee, S.; Newman, D.A.; Song, Q.C. More than g-factors: Second-stratum factors should not be ignored. Ind. Organ. Psychol. 2015, 8, 482–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ryan, A.M.; Ployhart, R.E. A century of selection. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014, 65, 693–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gottfredson, L.S. A g theorist on why Kovacs and Conway’s Process Overlap Theory amplifies, not opposes, g theory. Psychol. Inq. 2016, 27, 210–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ree, M.J.; Carretta, T.R.; Teachout, M.S. Pervasiveness of dominant general factors in organizational measurement. Ind. Organ. Psychol. 2015, 8, 409–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bliese, P.D.; Halverson, R.R.; Schriesheim, C.A. Benchmarking multilevel methods in leadership: The articles, the model, and the data set. Leadersh. Quart. 2002, 13, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lang, J.W.B.; Lang, J. Priming competence diminishes the link between cognitive test anxiety and test performance: Implications for the interpretation of test scores. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 811–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kersting, M.; Althoff, K.; Jäger, A.O. Wilde-Intelligenz-Test 2: WIT-2; Hogrefe, Verlag für Psychologie: Göttingen, Germany, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, W. Some experimental results in the correlation of mental abilities. Br. J. Psychol. 1910, 3, 296–322. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, W.; Thomson, G.H. The Essentials of Mental Measurement; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1921. [Google Scholar]
- Thorndike, E.L.; Lay, W.; Dean, P.R. The relation of accuracy in sensory discrimination to general intelligence. Am. J. Psychol. 1909, 20, 364–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tryon, R.C. A theory of psychological components—An alternative to “mathematical factors”. Psychol. Rev. 1935, 42, 425–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tryon, R.C. Reliability and behavior domain validity: Reformulation and historical critique. Psychol. Bull. 1957, 54, 229–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bartholomew, D.J.; Allerhand, M.; Deary, I.J. Measuring mental capacity: Thomson’s Bonds model and Spearman’s g-model compared. Intelligence 2013, 41, 222–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dickens, W.T. What Is g? Available online: https://www.brookings.edu/wp-content/uploads/2016/06/20070503.pdf (accessed on 2 May 2018).
- Kievit, R.A.; Davis, S.W.; Griffiths, J.; Correia, M.M.; Henson, R.N. A watershed model of individual differences in fluid intelligence. Neuropsychologia 2016, 91, 186–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kovacs, K.; Conway, A.R. Process overlap theory: A unified account of the general factor of intelligence. Psychol. Inq. 2016, 27, 151–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lang, J.W.B.; Kersting, M.; Beauducel, A. Hierarchies of factor solutions in the intelligence domain: Applying methodology from personality psychology to gain insights into the nature of intelligence. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2016, 47, 37–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Der Maas, H.L.; Dolan, C.V.; Grasman, R.P.; Wicherts, J.M.; Huizenga, H.M.; Raijmakers, M.E. A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 113, 842–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Campbell, D.T.; Fiske, D.W. Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychol. Bull. 1959, 56, 81–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gould, S.J. The Mismeasure of Man, 2nd ed.; W. W. Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Howe, M.J. Separate skills or general intelligence: The autonomy of human abilities. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 1989, 59, 351–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schlinger, H.D. The myth of intelligence. Psychol. Record 2003, 53, 15–32. [Google Scholar]
- Schönemann, P.H. Jensen’s g: Outmoded theories and unconquered frontiers. In Arthur Jensen: Consensus and Controversy; Modgil, S., Modgil, C., Eds.; The Falmer Press: New York, NY, USA, 1987; pp. 313–328. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, W.; Bouchard, T.J. The structure of human intelligence: It is verbal, perceptual, and image rotation (VPR), not fluid and crystallized. Intelligence 2005, 33, 393–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGrew, K.S. CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence 2009, 37, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humphreys, L.G. The primary mental ability. In Intelligence and Learning; Friedman, M.P., Das, J.R., O’Connor, N., Eds.; Plenum: New York, NY, USA, 1981; pp. 87–102. [Google Scholar]
- Reise, S.P. The rediscovery of bifactor measurement models. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2012, 47, 667–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Murray, A.L.; Johnson, W. The limitations of model fit in comparing the bi-factor versus higher-order models of human cognitive ability structure. Intelligence 2013, 41, 407–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldberg, L.R. Doing it all bass-ackwards: The development of hierarchical factor structures from the top down. J. Res. Personal. 2006, 40, 347–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDonald, R.P. Behavior domains in theory and in practice. Alta. J. Educ. Res. 2003, 49, 212–230. [Google Scholar]
- Bollen, K.; Lennox, R. Conventional wisdom on measurement: A structural equation perspective. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 110, 305–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kievit, R.A.; Lindenberger, U.; Goodyer, I.M.; Jones, P.B.; Fonagy, P.; Bullmore, E.T.; Dolan, R.J. Mutualistic coupling between vocabulary and reasoning supports cognitive development during late adolescence and early adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 28, 1419–1431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Der Maas, H.L.; Kan, K.J.; Marsman, M.; Stevenson, C.E. Network models for cognitive development and intelligence. J. Intell. 2017, 5, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kell, H.J.; Lang, J.W.B. The Great Debate: General Ability and Specific Abilities in the Prediction of Important Outcomes. J. Intell. 2018, 6, 39. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6030039
Kell HJ, Lang JWB. The Great Debate: General Ability and Specific Abilities in the Prediction of Important Outcomes. Journal of Intelligence. 2018; 6(3):39. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6030039
Chicago/Turabian StyleKell, Harrison J., and Jonas W. B. Lang. 2018. "The Great Debate: General Ability and Specific Abilities in the Prediction of Important Outcomes" Journal of Intelligence 6, no. 3: 39. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6030039
APA StyleKell, H. J., & Lang, J. W. B. (2018). The Great Debate: General Ability and Specific Abilities in the Prediction of Important Outcomes. Journal of Intelligence, 6(3), 39. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6030039