Advancing the Understanding of the Factor Structure of Executive Functioning
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Relationship between EF and Relevant Cognitive Constructs
1.2. The Current Study
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
2.3. Procedures
3. Statistical Analyses
3.1. Data Trimming and Transformation
3.2. Data Analyses
4. Results
4.1. Preliminary Data Analysis
4.2. Five-Factor Model
4.3. Hierarchical Model
4.4. Additional Model: Two-Layer Six-Factor Model
4.5. Model Comparison
5. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Factor Loadings for the Exploratory Factor Analysis of EF Variables
Measures | Factors | |
---|---|---|
1 | 2 | |
Number–letter | .56 | |
Color–shape | .35 | |
Category switch | .77 | |
Keep track | .72 | |
Letter memory | −.19 | .59 |
Nonverbal n-back | .63 | |
Antisaccade | .22 | .49 |
Stopsignal | .25 | |
Correlation | ||
Factor 1 | - | |
Factor 2 | .34 | - |
References
- Adrover-Roig, Daniel, Albert Sesé, Francisco Barceló, and Alfonso Palmer. 2012. A latent variable approach to executive control in healthy ageing. Brain and Cognition 78: 284–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Agostino, Alba, Janica Johnson, and Juan Pascual-Leone. 2010. Executive functions underlying multiplicative reasoning: Problem type matters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 105: 286–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christopher, Micaela E., Janice M. Keenan, Jacqueline Hulslander, John C. DeFries, Akira Miyake, Sally J. Wadsworth, Erik Willcutt, Bruce Pennington, and Richard K. Olson. 2016. The genetic and environmental etiologies of the relations between cognitive skills and components of reading ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 145: 451–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chuderski, Adam, Maciej Taraday, Edward Nêcka, and Tomasz Smoleñ. 2012. Storage capacity explains fluid intelligence but executive control does not. Intelligence 40: 278–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowan, Nelson. 2001. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 87–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ettenhofer, Mark L., David Z. Hambrick, and Norman Abeles. 2006. Reliability and stability of executive functioning in older adults. Neuropsychology 20: 607–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fleming, Kimberly A., Samantha J. Heintzelman, and Bruce D. Bartholow. 2016. Specifying associations between conscientiousness and executive functioning: Mental set shifting, not prepotent response inhibition or working memory updating. Journal of Personality 84: 348–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fournier-Vicente, Siobhan, Pascale Larigauderie, and Daniel Gaonac’h. 2008. More dissociations and interactions within central executive functioning: A comprehensive latent-variable analysis. Acta Psychologica 129: 32–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friedman, Naomi P., Akira Miyake, Lee J. Altamirano, Robin P. Corley, Susan E. Young, Sally A. Rhea, and John K. Hewitt. 2016. Stability and change in executive function abilities from late adolescence to early adulthood: A longitudinal twin study. Developmental Psychology 52: 326–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathmann, Bettina, Matthias Brand, and Johannes Schiebener. 2017. One executive function never comes alone: Monitoring and its relation to working memory, reasoning, and different executive functions. Cognitive Processing 18: 13–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halford, Graeme S., William H. Wilson, and Steven Phillips. 1998. Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 831–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hilbert, Sven, Tristan T. Nakagawa, Manuela Bindl, and Markus Bühner. 2014. The spatial Stroop effect: A comparison of color-word and position-word interference. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 21: 1509–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Himi, Samsad A., Markus Bühner, Matthias Schwaighofer, Anna Klapetek, and Sven Hilbert. 2019. Multitasking behavior and its related constructs: Executive functions, working memory capacity, relational integration, and divided attention. Cognition 189: 275–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horn, John L., and John J. McArdle. 2007. Understanding human intelligence since Spearman. In Factor Analysis at 100: Historical Developments and Future Directions. Edited by Robert Cudeck and Robert C. MacCallum. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, pp. 205–47. [Google Scholar]
- Hu, Li-tze, and Peter M. Bentler. 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 6: 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Im-Bolter, Nancie, Janica Johnson, and Juan Pascual-Leone. 2006. Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development 77: 1822–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ito, Tiffany A., Naomi P. Friedman, Bruce D. Bartholow, Joshua Correll, Chris Loersch, Lee J. Altamirano, and Akira Miyake. 2015. Toward a comprehensive understanding of executive cognitive function in implicit racial bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 108: 187–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaiser, Stefan, Steffen Aschenbrenner, UTe Pfüller, Daniela Roesch-Ely, and Matthias Weisbrod. 2010. Manual Response Inhibition (INHIB). Mödling: Schuhfried Gmbh. [Google Scholar]
- Kane, Michael J., and Randall W. Engle. 2000. Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26: 336–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klauer, Karl C., Florian Schmitz, Sarah Teige-Mocigemba, and Andreas Voss. 2010. Understanding the role of executive control in the implicit association test: Why flexible people have small IAT effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 63: 595–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kovacs, Kristof, and Andrew R. A. Conway. 2016. Process overlap theory: A unified account of the general Factor of intelligence. Psychological Inquiry 27: 151–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCabe, David P., Henry L. Roediger III, Mark A. McDaniel, David A. Balota, and David Z. Hambrick. 2010. The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: Evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology 24: 222–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miyake, Akira, Naomi P. Friedman, Michael J. Emerson, Alexander H. Witzki, Amy Howerter, and Tor D. Wager. 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Morra, Sergio, Sabrina Panesi, Laura Traverso, and Maria C. Usai. 2018. Which tasks measure what? Reflections on executive function development and a commentary on Podjarny, Kamawar, and Andrews. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 167: 246–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oberauer, Klaus, Heinz-Martin Süß, Oliver Wilhelm, and Nicolas Sander. 2007. Individual differences in working memory capacity and reasoning ability. In Variation in Working Memory. Edited by Andrew R. A. Conway, Christopher Jarrold, Michael J. Kane, Akira Miyake and John N. Towse. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 49–75. [Google Scholar]
- Osaka, Mariko, Naoyuki Osaka, Hirohito Kondo, Masanao Morishita, Hidenao Fukuyama, Toshihiko Aso, and Hiroshi Shibasaki. 2003. The neural basis of individual differences in working memory capacity: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 18: 789–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oswald, Frederick L., Samuel T. McAbee, Thomas S. Redick, and David Z. Hambrick. 2015. The development of a short domain-general measure of working memory capacity. Behavior Research Methods 47: 1343–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panesi, Sabrina, and Sergio Morra. 2020. Executive functions and mental attentional capacity in preschoolers. Journal of Cognition and Development 21: 72–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Development Core Team. 2020. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Available online: http://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 2 November 2015).
- Revelle, William. 2020. Psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research, R Package Version 2.0.8. [Computer Software]; Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych (accessed on 7 August 2020).
- Schellig, D., Uwe Schuri, and Martin Arendasy. 2011. Manual N-Back Non-Verbal (NBN). Mödling: Schuhfried Gmbh. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder, Hannah R., Akira Miyake, and Benjamin L. Hankin. 2015. Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: Bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Spearman, Charles. 1904. ‘General intelligence,’ objectively determined and measured. The American Journal of Psychology 15: 201–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sturm, Walter. 2008. Manual Perception and Attention Functions: Divided Attention (WAFG). Mödling: Schuhfried Gmbh. [Google Scholar]
- von Bastian, Claudia C., and Klaus Oberauer. 2013. Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of working memory capacity. Journal of Memory and Language 69: 36–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- von Bastian, Claudia C., Chris Blais, Gene A. Brewer, Máté Gyurkovics, Craig Hedge, Patrycja Kałamała, Matt E. Meier, Klaus Oberauer, Alodie Rey-Mermet, Jeffrey N. Rouder, and et al. 2020. Advancing the understanding of individual differences in attentional control: Theoretical, methodological, and analytical considerations. Manuscript submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
- Waltz, James A., Barbara J. Knowlton, Keith J. Holyoak, Kyle B. Boone, Carly Back-Madruga, Susan McPherson, Donna Masterman, Tiffany Chow, Jeffrey L. Cummings, and Bruse L. Miller. 2004. Relational integration and executive function in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 18: 296–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tasks | Authors | Task Description | Dependent Variables |
---|---|---|---|
Shifting | Friedman et al. (2016) | ||
Number–letter | When a number–letter pair appears in the top half of the matrix, participants have to classify the number as odd or even; but when the pair appears in the bottom half of the matrix, they should classify the letter as vowel or consonant. | Switch cost: the difference between the mean reaction time (RT) of correct switch trials and the mean RT of correct repeat (nonswitch) trials in random mixed blocks | |
Color–shape | Participants need to classify the color (green vs. red) or the geometric shape (circle vs. triangle) of the target stimulus. | ||
Category switch | Participants are instructed to switch back and forth regarding the dimension of animacy (living or nonliving) or size of the target stimulus (smaller or larger than a soccer ball). | ||
Updating | Friedman et al. (2016) | ||
Keep track | Participants remember the last exemplar of each of the five target categories. | Accuracy (i.e., the proportion of correct trials) | |
Letter memory | Participants remember the last four letters in the letter string. | ||
Nonverbal n-back | Schellig et al. (2011) | Participants identify the stimulus if the stimulus matches the stimulus n-times back. | The average of the z-scores across the 2-back and 3-back tasks |
Inhibition | Friedman et al. (2016) Kaiser et al. (2010) | ||
Antisaccade | Participants have to look in the opposite direction of visual cues to detect a briefly presented target. | The proportion of correct target discrimination responses across three antisaccade blocks. | |
Stop signal | Participants have to categorize and respond to stimuli until a stop signal appears for withholding a response. | The mean stop signal delay is subtracted from the median RT on go trials | |
WMC | Oswald et al. (2015) | ||
Operation span | Participants have to solve a series of math problems while remembering letters in correct serial order. | The partial-credit score | |
Reading span | Participants have to identify whether the sentences are meaningful while remembering letters in correct serial order. | ||
Symmetry span | Participants have to identify whether the patterns are symmetrical while remembering the correct presentation order of red squares in the 4×4 matrix. | ||
Relational integration | von Bastian and Oberauer (2013) | The dependent variable is the discriminability index (d′), reflecting the sensitivity of target detection. It is computed by relating the hit rate and false alarm rate (d′ = z (hit rate)—z (false alarm rate)). | |
Numerical version | Participants have to respond when three identical last digits appear either in a row, column, or diagonal line in a 3 × 3 matrix. | ||
Verbal version | Participants are asked to respond when three rhyming words are shown either in a row, column, or diagonal line within the 3 × 3 matrix. | ||
Figural version | Participants are asked to respond when four black dots form a square in a 3 × 3 matrix. | ||
Divided attention | Sturm (2008) | ||
Unimodal version | Participants have to monitor two visual stimulus presentation conditions. Whenever the same shape (either square or circle) gets noticeably lighter twice in a row, participants should respond. | The logarithmic mean RT of the given responses | |
Crossmodal version | Participants are required to monitor one visual and one auditory stimulus presentation conditions. Whenever the square gets noticeably lighter or the sound gets noticeably softer twice in a row, participants are asked to respond. |
Tests | Mean | SD | Skewness | Kurtosis | Reliability |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Executive functioning | |||||
Shifting | |||||
Number–letter | 457.88 | 157.64 | −0.75 | 0.25 | .89 c |
Color–shape | 828.06 | 275.50 | −0.80 | 0.92 | .92 c |
Category switch | 592.94 | 186.20 | −0.96 | 1.30 | .83 c |
Updating | |||||
Keep track | 0.75 | 0.10 | −0.68 | 0.20 | .72 b/.73 d |
Letter memory | 0.69 | 0.19 | −0.41 | −0.37 | .59 b/.59 d |
Nonverbal n-back | |||||
Nonverbal 2-back a | 1.27 | 0.10 | −0.37 | 0.73 | .84 b |
Nonverbal 3-back a | 1.22 | 0.09 | −0.36 | 0.11 | .86 b |
Inhibition | |||||
Antisaccade | 0.65 | 0.17 | −0.62 | 0.03 | .94 a |
Stop signal | 165.93 | 55.48 | 0.60 | 0.25 | .94 e |
WMC | |||||
Operation span | 0.82 | 0.19 | −1.39 | 1.56 | .72 b/.73 d |
Reading span | 0.66 | 0.23 | −0.66 | −0.11 | .73 b/.73 d |
Symmetry span | 0.65 | 0.20 | −0.56 | −0.03 | .55 b/.55 d |
Relational integration | |||||
Numerical | 2.43 | 0.73 | −0.22 | −0.13 | .77 c |
Verbal | 2.51 | 0.71 | 0.00 | −0.31 | .72 c |
Figural | 2.48 | 0.42 | −0.59 | 0.36 | .59 c |
Divided attention | |||||
Unimodal | 481.60 | 151.06 | −1.36 | 2.07 | .96 b |
Crossmodal | 492.11 | 171.42 | −0.82 | 0.39 | .96 b |
Model | χ2 | df | CFI | RMSEA | SRMR | AIC | BIC |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
A. Five-factor EF model | 130.10 | 94 | .95 | .04 | .05 | 214.10 | 353.05 |
B. Hierarchical model | 136.44 | 99 | .95 | .04 | .05 | 210.44 | 332.85 |
C. Two-layer six-factor model | 168.42 | 97 | .91 | .06 | .06 | 246.42 | 375.44 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Himi, S.A.; Bühner, M.; Hilbert, S. Advancing the Understanding of the Factor Structure of Executive Functioning. J. Intell. 2021, 9, 16. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9010016
Himi SA, Bühner M, Hilbert S. Advancing the Understanding of the Factor Structure of Executive Functioning. Journal of Intelligence. 2021; 9(1):16. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9010016
Chicago/Turabian StyleHimi, Samsad Afrin, Markus Bühner, and Sven Hilbert. 2021. "Advancing the Understanding of the Factor Structure of Executive Functioning" Journal of Intelligence 9, no. 1: 16. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9010016
APA StyleHimi, S. A., Bühner, M., & Hilbert, S. (2021). Advancing the Understanding of the Factor Structure of Executive Functioning. Journal of Intelligence, 9(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9010016