How Is Intelligence Test Performance Associated with Creative Achievement? A Meta-Analysis
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Creative Achievement in the Four-C Framework
1.2. Is a New Meta-Analysis Needed?
1.3. The Intelligence–Creative-Achievement Relationship and Its Moderators
1.3.1. The Domain of Creativity
1.3.2. The Theoretical Status of Intelligence and Creative Achievement
1.3.3. CAQ and ICAA—A Closer Look
2. The Present Study
3. Materials and Methods
3.1. Literature Search and Initial Screening
3.2. Screening and Eligibility Criteria
3.3. Coding Procedures
3.4. Study Selection
3.5. Statistical Procedure
4. Results
4.1. Overall Effect
4.2. Moderator Analysis
4.2.1. Study Characteristics: Year and Sample Composition
4.2.2. Creativity Domain
4.2.3. Creative Achievement Measure
4.2.4. Intelligence Facet and Test Modality
4.3. Publication Bias
5. Discussion
6. Limitations
7. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Notably, studies with a focus on creative activities may have assessed creative achievements as another variable. In some rare cases, we expected to find such studies only when the search term also includes “creative activities” (see Section 3.1). |
2 | Nine additional studies focused on the relationships between intelligence and creative activity rather than creative achievement. We decided not to include these studies, but we did note that the effect size for the activity-intelligence links observed in these studies was weaker: r = .09, 95% CI: .05, .13, p < .001 than reported. |
3 | When estimating this overall effect, only the total effect was included in studies that reported both the correlation between the total (aggregated) creative achievement and intelligence and correlations between intelligence and specific creative domains. Three reasons drove this decision. First, the total score is more reliable than the scores obtained in shorter scales of specific domains. Second, it is less likely that the total score suffers from restriction of range and floor effects—a common problem of domain scores. Third and final, in such studies, we omitted domain-based correlations to avoid including them twice (as already covered by the total score). The domain-level correlations were used to analyze the role of the domain moderator, presented later on. |
4 | It is fascinating to note that our point estimate of r = .16 (95% CI: .12, .19) perfectly overlaps with the correlation between IQ estimates and eminence in a sample of geniuses in Cox (1926) study. More specifically, after controlling for IQ scores reliability, Cox obtained a correlation of r = .16, with a 95% CI: .12, .20 (Cox 1926, p. 55). We are grateful to Dean Keith Simonton for bringing this to our attention. |
5 | We appreciate the thoughtful comment of the anonymous reviewer who pushed us to re-think this issue. |
References
- Adams, Richard J., Palie Smart, and Anne S. Huff. 2017. Shades of grey: Guidelines for working with the grey literature in systematic reviews for management and organizational studies. International Journal of Management Reviews 19: 432–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Araki, Michael E. 2018. Polymathy: A new outlook. Journal of Genius and Eminence 3: 66–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Avitia, Maria J., and James C. Kaufman. 2014. Beyond g and c: The relationship of rated creativity to long-term storage and retrieval (Glr). Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 8: 293–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baer, John. 2015. The importance of domain-specific expertise in creativity. Roeper Review: A Journal on Gifted Education 37: 165–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Batey, Mark D. 2007. A Psychometric Investigation of Everyday Creativity. London: University of London, University College London. [Google Scholar]
- Batey, Mark, and Adrian Furnham. 2008. The relationship between measures of creativity and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences 45: 816–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beaty, Roger E., Emily C. Nusbaum, and Paul J. Silvia. 2014. Does insight problem solving predict real-world creativity? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 8: 287–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beaty, Roger E., Mathias Benedek, Scott B. Kaufman, and Paul J. Silvia. 2015. Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Scientific Reports 5: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Beghetto, Ronald A., Maciej Karwowski, and Roni Reiter-Palmon. 2020. Intellectual risk taking: A moderating link between creative confidence and creative behavior? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benbow, Camilla P. 1992. Academic achievement in mathematics and science of students between ages 13 and 23: Are there differences among students in the top one percent of mathematical ability? Journal of Educational Psychology 84: 51–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benedek, Mathias, Franz Fabiola, Moritz Heene, and Aljoscha C. Neubauer. 2012. Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences 53: 480–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carroll, John B. 1993. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Carson, Shelley H., Jordan B. Peterson, and Daniel M. Higgins. 2003. Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85: 499–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carson, Shelley H., Jordan B. Peterson, and Daniel M. Higgins. 2005. Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the creative achievement questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal 17: 37–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, Rachel. 2015. Neuroticism and Its Associations with Higher Cognitive Functions. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Digital Conservancy. [Google Scholar]
- Condon, David M., and William Revelle. 2014. The International Cognitive Ability Resource: Development and initial validation of a public-domain measure. Intelligence 43: 52–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corazza, Giovanni E. 2016. Potential originality and effectiveness: The dynamic definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal 28: 258–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Corazza, Giovanni E., and Todd Lubart. 2021. Intelligence and Creativity: Mapping Constructs on the Space-Time Continuum. Journal of Intelligence 9: 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cox, Catharine M. 1926. The Early Mental Traits of Three Hundred Geniuses. Stanford: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly. 1988. Motivation and creativity: Toward a synthesis of structural and energistic approaches to cognition. New Ideas in Psychology 6: 159–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Manzano, Örjan, and Frederik Ullén. 2018. Genetic and environmental influences on the phenotypic associations between intelligence, personality, and creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Intelligence 69: 123–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deary, Ian J., Steve Strand, Pauline Smith, and Cres Fernandes. 2007. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence 35: 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diedrich, Jennifer, Emanuel Jauk, Paul J. Silvia, Jeffrey M. Gredlein, Aljoscha C. Neubauer, and Mathias Benedek. 2018. Assessment of real-life creativity: The Inventory of Creative Activities and Achievements (ICAA). Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 12: 304–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dollinger, Stephen J. 2003. Need for uniqueness, need for cognition, and creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior 37: 99–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duval, Sue, and Richard Tweedie. 2000. A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association 95: 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dworak, Elisabeth M., William Revelle, Philip Doebler, and David M. Condon. 2021. Using the International Cognitive Ability Resource as an open source tool to explore individual differences in cognitive ability. Personality and Individual Differences 169: 109906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ericsson, K. Anders. 2014. The Road to Excellence: The Acquisition of Expert Performance in the Arts and Sciences, Sports, and Games. Mahwah: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Feist, Gregory. J. 1998. A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review 2: 290–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Feist, Gregory J., and Frank X. Barron. 2003. Predicting creativity from early to late adulthood: Intellect, potential, and personality. Journal of Research in Personality 37: 62–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Florida, Richard. 2019. The Rise of the Creative Class. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- Forthmann, Boris, David Jendryczko, Jana Scharfen, Ruben Kleinkorres, Mathias Benedek, and Heinz Holling. 2019. Creative ideation, broad retrieval ability, and processing speed: A confirmatory study of nested cognitive abilities. Intelligence 75: 59–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerwig, Anne, Kirill G. Miroshnik, Boris Forthmann, Mathias Benedek, Maciej Karwowski, and Heinz Holling. 2021. The relationship of intelligence and divergent thinking—A meta-analytic update. Journal of Intelligence 9: 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gignac, Gilles E., and Eva T. Szodorai. 2016. Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Personality and Individual Differences 102: 74–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glăveanu, Vlad. 2013. Rewriting the language of creativity: The Five A’s framework. Review of General Psychology 17: 69–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gonzalez, Miguel C. 2020. The Predictive Power of Personality, Intelligence, and Divergent Thinking for Creative Achievement. San Antonio: The University of Texas at San Antonio, Wake County: ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Gough, Harrison G. 1975. A new scientific uses test and its relationship to creativity in research. Journal of Creative Behavior 9: 245–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gough, Harrison G. 1976. Studying creativity by means of word association tests. Journal of Applied Psychology 61: 348–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greengross, Gil, and Geoffrey F. Miller. 2009. The Big Five personality traits of professional comedians compared to amateur comedians, comedy writers, and college students. Personality and Individual Differences 47: 79–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harris, Alexandra M., Rachel L. Williamson, and Nathan T. Carter. 2019. A conditional threshold hypothesis for creative achievement: On the interaction between intelligence and openness. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 13: 322–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hocevar, Dennis. 1976. Dimensionality of Creativity. Psychological Reports 39: 869–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunter, John E., and Ronda F. Hunter. 1984. Validity and utility of alternative predictors of job performance. Psychological Bulletin 96: 72–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ivcevic, Zorana, and John D. Mayer. 2009. Mapping dimensions of creativity in the life-space. Creativity Research Journal 21: 152–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jauk, Emanuel, Mathias Benedek, Beate Dunst, and Aljoscha C. Neubauer. 2013. The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence 41: 212–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jauk, Emanuel, Mathias Benedek, and Aljoscha C. Neubauer. 2014. The road to creative achievement: A latent variable model of ability and personality predictors. European Journal of Personality 28: 95–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karwowski, Maciej. 2020. Intelligence and creative achievement. Unpublished data. [Google Scholar]
- Karwowski, Maciej, and Ronald A. Beghetto. 2019. Creative behavior as agentic action. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 13: 402–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karwowski, Maciej, and Jacek Gralewski. 2013. Threshold hypothesis: Fact or artifact? Thinking Skills and Creativity 8: 25–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karwowski, Maciej, Jan Dul, Jacek Gralewski, Emanuel Jauk, Dorota M. Jankowska, Aleksandra Gajda, Michał H. Chruszczewski, and Mathias Benedek. 2016. Is creativity without intelligence possible? A necessary condition analysis. Intelligence 57: 105–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karwowski, Maciej, James C. Kaufman, Izabela Lebuda, Grzegorz Szumski, and Anna Firkowska-Mankiewicz. 2017. Intelligence in childhood and creative achievements in middle-age: The necessary condition approach. Intelligence 64: 36–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karwowski, Maciej, Marta Czerwonka, and James C. Kaufman. 2020. Does intelligence strengthen creative metacognition? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 14: 353–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaufman, James C., and Ronald A. Beghetto. 2009. Beyond big and little: The four c model of creativity. Review of General Psychology 13: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kaufman, Scott B., and James C. Kaufman. 2007. Ten years to expertise, many more to greatness: An investigation of modern writers. The Journal of Creative Behavior 41: 114–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaufman, Scott B., Aaron Kozbelt, Paul J. Silvia, James C. Kaufman, Scheela Ramesh, and Gregory J. Feist. 2014. Who finds Bill Gates sexy? Creative mate preferences as a function of cognitive ability, personality, and creative achievement. The Journal of Creative Behavior 50: 294–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaufman, Scott B., Lena C. Quilty, Rachael G. Grazioplene, Jacob B. Hirsh, Jeremy R. Gray, Jordan R. Peterson, and Colin G. DeYoung. 2015. Openness to experience and intellect differentially predict creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of Personality 82: 248–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kell, Harrison J., David Lubinski, and Camilla P. Benbow. 2013. Who rises to the top? Early indicators. Psychological Science 24: 648–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kellner, Raphaela, and Mathias Benedek. 2017. The role of creative potential and intelligence for humor production. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 1: 52–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kéri, Szabolcs. 2011. Solitary minds and social capital: Latent inhibition, general intellectual functions and social network size predict creative achievements. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 5: 215–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kim, Kyung H. 2008. Meta-analyses of the relationship of creative achievement to both IQ and divergent thinking test scores. Journal of Creative Behavior 42: 106–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Konstantopoulos, Spyros. 2011. Fixed effects and variance components estimation in three-level meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods 2: 61–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kossmeier, Michael, Ulrich S. Tran, and Martin Voracek. 2020. metaviz: Forest Plots, Funnel Plots, and Visual Funnel Plot Inference for Meta-Analysis. R package version 0.3.1. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=metaviz (accessed on 11 April 2021).
- Krampe, Ralf T., and Neil Charness. 2018. Aging and expertise. In The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, 2nd ed. Edited by K. Anders Ericsson, Robert R. Hoffman, Aaron Kozbelt and Mark A. Williams. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 835–56. [Google Scholar]
- Kuncel, Nathan R., Sarah A. Hezlett, and Deniz S. Ones. 2004. Academic performance, career potential, creativity, and job performance: Can one construct predict them all? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86: 148–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kuncel, Nathan R., Marcus Credé, and Lisa L. Thomas. 2005. The validity of self-reported grade point averages, class ranks, and test scores: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. Review of Educational Research 75: 63–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lebuda, Izabela. 2016. Political pathologies and Big-C creativity: Eminent Polish creators’ experience of restrictions under the Communist regime. In The Palgrave Handbook of Creativity and Culture Research. Edited by Vlad P. Glăveanu. London: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 329–54. [Google Scholar]
- Lebuda, Izabela, and Maciej Karwowski. Forthcoming. Personality of Nobel Prize Laureates: Differences Across Domains and Relationship to Public Recognition. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts.
- Lunke, Katrin, and Beat Meier. 2016. Disentangling the impact of artistic creativity on creative thinking, working memory, attention, and intelligence: Evidence for domain-specific relationships with a new self-report questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lunneborg, Clifford E., and Patricia W. Lunneborg. 1968. Architecture School Performance Predicted from ASAT, Intellective and Nonintellective Measures. Available online: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED023350 (accessed on 15 May 2021).
- Makel, Matthew C., Harrison J. Kell, David Lubinski, Martha Putallaz, and Camilla P. Benbow. 2016. When lightning strikes twice: Profoundly gifted, profoundly accomplished. Psychological Science 27: 1004–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mar, Raymond A., Colin G. DeYoung, Daniel M. Higgins, and Jordan B. Peterson. 2006. Self-liking and self-competence separate self-evaluation from self-deception: Associations with personality, ability, and achievement. Journal of Personality 74: 1047–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martin, Nathan D., and Alexandre Frenette. 2017. Lost in transition: College resources and the unequal early-career trajectories of arts alumni. American Behavioral Scientist 61: 1487–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDermid, Charles D. 1965. Some correlates of creativity in engineering personnel. Journal of Applied Psychology 49: 14–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McGrew, Kevin S. 2009. CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence 37: 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McKay, Alexander S., Maciej Karwowski, and James C. Kaufman. 2017. Measuring the muses: Validating the Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale (K-DOCS). Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 11: 216–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Milgram, Roberta M., and Norman A. Milgram. 1976. Creative thinking and creative performance in Israeli students. Journal of Educational Psychology 68: 255–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miroshnik, Kirill G., and Olga V. Shcherbakova. 2019. The proportion and creativity of “old” and “new” ideas: Are they related to fluid intelligence? Intelligence 76: 101384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moher, David, Alessandro Liberati, Jennifer Tetzlaff, Douglas G. Altman, and The PRISMA Group. 2009. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine 6: e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nemiro, Jill. 1997. Interpretive artists: A qualitative exploration of the creative process of actors. Creativity Research Journal 10: 229–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nusbaum, Emily C., and Paul J. Silvia. 2011. Are intelligence and creativity really so different? Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence 39: 36–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nusbaum, Emily C., Paul J. Silvia, and Roger E. Beaty. 2014. Ready, set, create: What instructing people to “be creative” reveals about the meaning and mechanisms of divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 8: 423–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, Gregory, David Lubinski, and Camilla P. Benbow. 2007. Contrasting intellectual patterns predict creativity in the arts and sciences: Tracking intellectually precocious youth over 25 years. Psychological Science 18: 948–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, Gregory, David Lubinski, and Camilla P. Benbow. 2008. Ability differences among people who have commensurate degrees matter for scientific creativity. Psychological Science 19: 957–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plucker, Jonathan A. 1998. Beware of simple conclusions: The case for content generality of creativity. Creativity Research Journal 11: 179–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plucker, Jonathan A. 1999. Is the proof in the pudding? Reanalyses of Torrance’s (1958 to present) longitudinal data. Creativity Research Journal 12: 103–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plucker, Jonathan. A., Maciej Karwowski, and James C. Kaufman. 2019. Intelligence and creativity. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1087–105. [Google Scholar]
- Prabhakaran, Ranjani, Adam E. Green, and Jeremy R. Gray. 2014. Thin slices of creativity: Using single-word utterances to assess creative cognition. Behavior Research Methods 46: 641–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- R Core Team. 2016. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Core Team. [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes, Mel. 1961. An analysis of creativity. The Phi Delta Kappan 42: 305–10. [Google Scholar]
- Robertson, Kimberley F., Stijn Smeets, David Lubinski, and Camilla P. Benbow. 2010. Beyond the threshold hypothesis: Even among the gifted and top math/science graduate students, cognitive abilities, vocational interests, and lifestyle preferences matter for career choice, performance, and persistence. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19: 346–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schmucker, Christine M., Anette Blümle, Lisa K. Schell, Guido Schwarzer, Patrick Oeller, Laura Cabrera, Erik von Elm, Matthias Briel, and Joerg J. Meerpohl. 2017. Systematic review finds that study data not published in full text articles have unclear impact on meta-analyses results in medical research. PLoS ONE 12: e0176210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schönbrodt, Felix D., and Marco Perugini. 2013. At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality 47: 609–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sharpe, Donald. 1997. Of apples and oranges, file drawers and garbage: Why validity issues in meta-analysis will not go away. Clinical Psychology Review 17: 881–901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shurkin, Joel N. 1992. Terman’s Kids: The Groundbreaking Study of How the Gifted Grow Up. Boston, Toronto and London: Little Brown. [Google Scholar]
- Silvia, Paul J., Emily C. Nusbaum, Christopher Berg, Christopher Martin, and Alejandra O’Connor. 2009a. Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity: Exploring lower-order, high-order, and interactive effects. Journal of Research in Personality 43: 1087–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvia, Paul J., James C. Kaufman, and Jean E. Pretz. 2009b. Is creativity domain-specific? Latent class models of creative accomplishments and creative self-descriptions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 3: 139–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Silvia, Paul J., Benjamin Wigert, Roni Reiter-Palmon, and James C. Kaufman. 2012. Assessing creativity with self-report scales: A review and empirical evaluation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 6: 19–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvia, Paul J., Rebekah M. Rodriguez, James C. Kaufman, Roni Reiter-Palmon, and Jeb S. Puryear. 2021a. Taking Inventory of the Creative Behavior Inventory: An Item Response Theory Analysis of the CBI. Preprint. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvia, Paul J., Rebekah M. Rodriguez, Roger E. Beaty, Emily Frith, James C. Kaufman, Paul Loprinzi, and Roni Reiter-Palmon. 2021b. Measuring everyday creativity: A Rasch model analysis of the Biographical Inventory of Creative Behaviors (BICB) scale. Thinking Skills and Creativity 39: 100797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simonton, Dean. K. 1977. Creative productivity, age, and stress: A biographical time-series analysis of 10 classical composers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35: 791–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simonton, Dean K. 2013. After Einstein: Scientific genius is extinct. Nature 493: 602–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sligh, Allison C., Frances A. Conners, and Beverly Roskos-Ewoldsen. 2005. Relation of creativity to fluid and crystallized intelligence. The Journal of Creative Behavior 39: 123–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1985. Beyond IQ: A Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2002. Creativity as a decision: Comment. American Psychologist 57: 376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert. J., and Todd I. Lubart. 1996. Investing in creativity. American Psychologist 51: 677–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert. J., James C. Kaufman, and Jean E. Pretz. 2002. The Creativity Conundrum: A Propulsion Model of Kinds of Creative Contributions. New York: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Vlad P. Glăveanu, Sareh Karami, James C. Kaufman, Shane N. Phillipson, and David D. Preiss. 2021. Meta-Intelligence: Understanding, Control, and Interactivity between Creative, Analytical, Practical, and Wisdom-Based Approaches in Problem Solving. Journal of Intelligence 9: 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sutu, Andreea, Surizaday Serrano, Leah H. Schultz, Joshua J. Jackson, and Rodica I. Damian. 2019. Creating through deviancy or adjustment? The link between personality profile normativeness and creativity. European Journal of Personality 33: 565–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Szen-Ziemiańska, Joanna, Izabela Lebuda, and Maciej Karwowski. 2017. Mix and Match: Cross-domain creativity. In Cambridge Handbook of Creativity across Domains. Edited by James C. Kaufman, Vlad P. Glăveanu and John Baer. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 18–40. [Google Scholar]
- Vellante, Marcello Z., Giulia Preti, Antonio Sisti, Davide Rocchi, Marco B. L. Akiskal, and Kareen K. Akiskal. 2011. Creativity and affective temperaments in non-clinical professional artists: An empirical psychometric investigation. Journal of Affective Disorders 135: 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Viechtbauer, Wolfgang. 2005. Bias and efficiency of meta-analytic variance estimators in the random-effects model. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics 30: 261–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Viechtbauer, Wolfgang. 2010. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software 36: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Waltz, Emily. 2020. AI takes its best shot: What AI can—and can’t—do in the race for a coronavirus vaccine. IEEE Spectrum 57: 24–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Warne, Russell T., Ross A. A. Larsen, and Jonathan Clark. 2020. Low base rates and a high IQ selection threshold prevented Terman from identifying future Nobelists. Intelligence 82: 101488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weiss, Selina, Diana Steger, Kaur Yadwinder, Andrea Hildebrandt, Ulrich Schroeders, and Oliver Wilhelm. 2020a. On the trail of creativity: Dimensionality of divergent thinking and its relation with cognitive abilities and personality. European Journal of Personality. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weiss, Selina, Diana Steger, Ulrich Schroeders, and Oliver Wilhelm. 2020b. A Reappraisal of the Threshold Hypothesis of Creativity and Intelligence. Journal of Intelligence 8: 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zabelina, Darya L., and Giorgio Ganis. 2018. Creativity and cognitive control: Behavioral and ERP evidence that divergent thinking, but not real-life creative achievement, relates to better cognitive control. Neuropsychologia 118: 20–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Zabelina, Darya. L., Naomi P. Friedman, and Jessica Andrews-Hanna. 2019. Unity and diversity of executive functions in creativity. Consciousness and Cognition 68: 47–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zabelina, Darya L., Elina Zaonegina, William Revelle, and David M. Condon. 2020. Creative Achievement and Individual Differences: Associations across and within the Domains of Creativity. Preprint. Available online: https://osf.io/4g92k/?pid=8d6zh (accessed on 15 May 2021).
- Zaboski, Brian A., 2nd, John H. Kranzler, and Nicholas A. Gage. 2018. Meta-analysis of the relationship between academic achievement and broad abilities of the Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory. Journal of School Psychology 71: 42–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zielińska, Aleksandra, Izabela Lebuda, Zorana Ivcevic, and Maciej Karwowski. 2020. How Adolescents Develop and Implement Their Ideas? On Self-Regulation of Creative Action. Preprint. Available online: https://psyarxiv.com/z85fc/ (accessed on 15 May 2021).
95%-CI | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effects | Estimate | LB | UB | P | k | m | N |
Total Achievement | .16 | .13 | .19 | <.001 | 31 | 22 | 19,983 |
Achievement—Arts | .09 | .06 | .12 | <.001 | 69 | 13 | 15,317 |
Achievement—Science | .19 | .16 | .22 | <.001 | 56 | 16 | 15,444 |
Achievement—Everyday | .06 | .03 | .10 | <.001 | 26 | 6 | 3482 |
95%-CI | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effects | Estimate | LB | UB | p | k | m | N |
CAQ | .14 | .10 | .17 | <.001 | 86 | 20 | 18,849 |
ICAA | .20 | .11 | .29 | <.001 | 7 | 4 | 2002 |
Other | .20 | .12 | .28 | <.001 | 23 | 6 | 752 |
95%-CI | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effects | Estimate | LB | UB | p | k | m | N |
g | .17 | .13 | .21 | <.001 | 36 | 20 | 9600 |
Gf | .17 | .12 | .22 | <.001 | 46 | 7 | 12,761 |
Gc | .11 | .06 | .17 | <.001 | 25 | 7 | 2169 |
Gr | .20 | .11 | .29 | <.001 | 10 | 1 | 116 |
Verbal | .17 | .13 | .21 | <.001 | 43 | 10 | 2527 |
Figural | .17 | .13 | .22 | <.001 | 27 | 7 | 12,761 |
Both | .15 | .11 | .18 | <.001 | 47 | 20 | 9801 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Karwowski, M.; Czerwonka, M.; Wiśniewska, E.; Forthmann, B. How Is Intelligence Test Performance Associated with Creative Achievement? A Meta-Analysis. J. Intell. 2021, 9, 28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9020028
Karwowski M, Czerwonka M, Wiśniewska E, Forthmann B. How Is Intelligence Test Performance Associated with Creative Achievement? A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Intelligence. 2021; 9(2):28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9020028
Chicago/Turabian StyleKarwowski, Maciej, Marta Czerwonka, Ewa Wiśniewska, and Boris Forthmann. 2021. "How Is Intelligence Test Performance Associated with Creative Achievement? A Meta-Analysis" Journal of Intelligence 9, no. 2: 28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9020028