Next Article in Journal
How to Get Better: Taking Notes Mediates the Effect of a Video Tutorial on Number Series
Next Article in Special Issue
Intelligence Can Be Used to Make a More Equitable Society but Only When Properly Defined and Applied
Previous Article in Journal
Examining the Use of Game-Based Assessments for Hiring Autistic Job Seekers
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Precipitous Decline in Reasoning and Other Key Abilities with Age and Its Implications for Federal Judges
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Beyond IQ: The Importance of Metacognition for the Promotion of Global Wellbeing

by
Lav R. Varshney
1,2 and
Aron K. Barbey
2,3,*
1
Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
2
Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61801, USA
3
Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61801, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 10 September 2021 / Revised: 4 November 2021 / Accepted: 17 November 2021 / Published: 18 November 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue How Intelligence Can Be a Solution to Consequential World Problems)

Abstract

:
Global policy makers increasingly adopt subjective wellbeing as a framework within which to measure and address human development challenges, including policies to mitigate consequential societal problems. In this review, we take a systems-level perspective to assemble evidence from studies of wellbeing, of collective intelligence, and of metacognition and argue for a virtuous cycle for health promotion in which the increased collective intelligence of groups: (1) enhances the ability of such groups to address consequential societal problems; (2) promotes the wellbeing of societies and the individual wellbeing of people within groups; and, finally, (3) enables prosocial actions that further promote collective problem-solving and global wellbeing. Notably, evidence demonstrates that effective collaboration and teamwork largely depend on social skills for metacognitive awareness—the capacity to evaluate and control our own mental processes in the service of social problem-solving. Yet, despite their importance, metacognitive skills may not be well-captured by measures of general intelligence. These skills have instead been the focus of decades of research in the psychology of human judgment and decision-making. This literature provides well-validated tests of metacognitive awareness and demonstrates that the capacity to use analysis and deliberation to evaluate intuitive responses is an important source of individual differences in decision-making. Research in network neuroscience further elucidates the topology and dynamics of brain networks that enable metacognitive awareness, providing key targets for intervention. As such, we further discuss emerging scientific interventions to enhance metacognitive skills (e.g., based on mindfulness meditation, and physical activity and aerobic fitness), and how such interventions may catalyze the virtuous cycle to improve collective intelligence, societal problem-solving, and global wellbeing.

1. Collective Intelligence, Metacognition, and Wellbeing

Collective intelligence, metacognition, and wellbeing are three constructs that have developed largely independently from one another in psychology and allied fields, which we review here. Yet, emerging evidence indicates there are potentially powerful relationships among them that can be leveraged to catalyze collective problem-solving at a systems level to address consequential societal troubles such as the coronavirus pandemic, systemic racism, and poverty. In particular, social problem-solving is largely driven by social engagement (Mao 2016) and the collective intelligence of a community (Tovey 2015; Malone and Woolley 2020), which itself depends on effective collaboration and teamwork due to social skills from metacognitive awareness (Gupta and Woolley 2020). Further, social problem-solving can equitably promote public wellbeing, which may be necessary for promoting individual wellbeing (Lambert et al. 2020) and in turn lead to people that are more socially engaged (Mehl et al. 2010; Richards and Huppert 2011). Here we argue that scientific interventions to enhance metacognitive awareness may improve collective intelligence, social problem-solving, and global wellbeing.
Global policy makers increasingly adopt subjective wellbeing as a framework within which to measure and address human development challenges, extending their traditional focus from material wellbeing and economic development to include the impact policies have on how people think and feel about their lives (Dolan and White 2007). Further, there is growing agreement among scientists, economists, and policy experts that the gross domestic product (GDP) should no longer be a primary performance indicator for national prosperity. The World Happiness Report (WHR) has garnered attention for its national happiness rankings; it assesses wellbeing using the Cantril Self-Anchoring Striving Scale (Cantril 1965), also called Cantril’s Ladder, which rates respondents on their current and future perceived quality or satisfaction with life. Yet, as captured in the United Nations 2030 Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs), an emerging global agenda for development is recognizing the coupling of social, environmental, and economic dynamics and in particular, the interconnectedness of several dimensions of human and ecosystem wellbeing (Fioramonti et al. 2019). Moreover, the science of wellbeing is starting to reflect a richer view of wellbeing than life satisfaction alone by including hedonic and eudaemonic facets of wellbeing, social wellbeing, as well as the role of culture, community, nature, and governance (Lambert et al. 2020). Assessment tools beyond Cantril’s Ladder that draw not just on psychology, but also neighboring fields such as organizational design, health, education, and economics, are being developed for this broader conception of wellbeing.
Given that subjective wellbeing in this broader conception is not only an individual consideration but is intertwined with social and environmental life, it follows that equitably promoting public wellbeing is necessary for promoting individual wellbeing. Quite interestingly, equality not just of income but also of life satisfaction is associated with higher average subjective wellbeing in countries around the world (Diener and Tay 2015). Further, the positive impact of income on subjective wellbeing is greater in more equal societies (Ng and Diener 2019). Due to the positively correlated relationships between life satisfaction and subjective wellbeing as well as between income and subjective wellbeing, we can therefore infer that subjective wellbeing is not a conserved, extensive quantity like energy or mass, but actually increases in total as it is equalized in societies. This indicates that redistribution is not a zero-sum activity.
It further follows that public wellbeing is inextricably linked with consequential societal problems such as the coronavirus pandemic, systemic racism, and poverty. Public wellbeing can be promoted through collective action on social determinants of wellbeing (Fisher 2019), especially since a key driver of wellbeing is the capacity to distribute and share the cognitive, social, and economic demands of life, providing a powerful mechanism for mitigating loss and the effects of adversity.
Evidence demonstrates that people with greater individual wellbeing are more likely to show compassion and empathy (Shanafelt et al. 2005; Nelson 2009; Rand et al. 2015), make ethical decisions (James and Chymis 2004; Fernando and Chowdhury 2010), live healthier and longer lives (Wiest et al. 2011; Boehm and Kubzansky 2012), and perhaps most importantly for our discussion, be socially engaged (Mehl et al. 2010; Richards and Huppert 2011) and perform prosocial behavior (Son and Wilson 2012; Yang et al. 2017).
At the same time, recent findings suggest that addressing consequential societal problems and enhancing prosperity are largely driven by social problem-solving and the collective intelligence of a community (Tovey 2015; Malone and Woolley 2020), rather than the individual intelligence of community members. Contemporary methods to enhance collective intelligence have focused on the distribution and processing of knowledge within a social network, as measured by transactive memory and attentional systems (Gupta and Woolley 2020). Notably, the collective intelligence of a group is strongly associated with abilities such as emotional intelligence and theory of mind of group members (Woolley et al. 2010). Further, effective collaboration and teamwork largely depend on social skills for metacognitive awareness (Gupta and Woolley 2020), the capacity to evaluate and control our own mental processes in the service of social problem-solving.
To summarize the logic that emerges from our review, we see a virtuous cycle for health promotion in which the increased collective intelligence of groups: (1) enhances the ability of such groups to address consequential societal problems; (2) promotes the wellbeing of societies and the individual wellbeing of people within groups; and, finally, (3) enables prosocial actions that further promote collective problem-solving and global wellbeing.
Jintelligence 09 00054 i001
Is it possible to accelerate this virtuous cycle and enhance global wellbeing through science-based interventions that focus on particular metacognitive abilities, which have known neural correlates in brain networks?

2. Construct Validity

By analogy with the individual intelligence factor g, Woolley et al. (2010) defined a group’s collective intelligence (c) as the general ability of the group to perform a wide variety of tasks. From a psychometric perspective, c emerges when the ability of a group to perform one task is correlated with that group’s ability to perform a wide range of other tasks. This kind of collective intelligence is a property of the group itself, not just the individuals in it. Although the individual intelligence factors of group members may play a role in collective intelligence (Bates and Gupta 2017), a meta-analysis that examined 22 studies, with 5279 individuals comprising 1356 groups, showed that c is predicted by the proportion of women in the group, mediated by average social perceptiveness of group members, and that it is validated in the sense of predicting performance on various out-of-sample criterion tasks (Riedl et al. 2021). Yet, c is not nearly as well-validated as g (Coyle 2021).
Metacognitive awareness, with a key dimension of metacognitive knowledge, is defined as beliefs about one’s own mental states and processes as well as beliefs about those of other people (Jost et al. 1998). Aptitude in these specific metacognitive skills is the focus of our analysis. There is some agreement on the general theoretical structure of metacognition, which has informed the development of the Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (MAI), a commonly used instrument for its measurement (Schraw and Dennison 1994). Although self-report instruments such as MAI may raise validity concerns, it is widely used in both research and practice; further refinements show between-group and time invariance (Harrison and Vallin 2018).
As noted above, subjective wellbeing is defined as how people experience and evaluate their lives, usually measured through questionnaires that have been well-validated using large-scale studies (Nima et al. 2020). An example instrument is the Satisfaction With Life Scale (Pavot and Diener 2008), which has been validated and widely used. Wellbeing is increasingly being constructed to also include hedonic and eudaemonic facets of wellbeing, social wellbeing, as well as the role of culture, community, nature, and governance (Lambert et al. 2020). This validity of this broader construct of subjective wellbeing is still under investigation (Pancheva et al. 2021).

3. Interventions to Improve Wellbeing via Improved Metacognitive Skills

Despite their importance for collective intelligence (Gupta and Woolley 2020) and wellbeing (Kiaei and Reio 2014), metacognitive skills are not typically captured by measures of general intelligence (and are in fact weakly associated with intelligence (Ohtani and Hisasaka 2018)). These skills have instead been the focus of research in educational psychology and the psychology of human judgment and decision-making (Koriat 2015). This literature provides well-validated tests of metacognitive awareness (Schraw and Dennison 1994) and demonstrates that the capacity to use analysis and deliberation to evaluate intuitive responses is an important source of individual differences in decision-making (Kleitman et al. 2019). Moreover, this literature demonstrates that metacognitive training aids decision-making (Batha and Carroll 2007).
Research in network neuroscience further elucidates the topology and dynamics of brain networks that enable metacognitive awareness, providing key targets for intervention (Paul et al. 2015). Evidence indicates that the precuneus has differential connectivity within the default mode network of the brain across individuals’ lifespans (Yang et al. 2014) and has specialized roles in self-related cognition and awareness (Philippi et al. 2012; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al. 2011). These attributes of the precuneus suggest it may be linked with over/underconfidence in decision-making or other metacognitive abilities. In our recent work, we collected resting-state fMRI data (n = 304) to perform connectome-wide association to characterize individual differences in decision-making competence, identifying regions within the frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital cortex that demonstrated significant associations (Talukdar et al. 2018). In assessing whether functional interactions between brain regions sensitive to decision-making competence and particular intrinsic connectivity networks (ICNs) were predictive of specific facets of decision-making, we further found individual differences in specific facets of decision-making competence are mediated by ICNs that support executive, social, and perceptual processes. More broadly, these findings motivate an integrative framework for understanding the neural basis of individual differences in decision-making competence (Talukdar et al. 2018).
Research in cognitive psychology has investigated several pathways to improve metacognitive skills including skill-based cognitive training and mindfulness meditation. A large empirical literature on the efficacy of cognitive training programs, involving the guided practice of specific cognitive tests to enhance executive functions and decision-making (Zwilling et al. 2019), indicates that training of executive functions can promote specific cognitive skills (e.g., inhibition, interference control, and working memory) but typically does not generalize to improvements beyond the trained tasks (Au et al. 2014; Simons et al. 2016; Soveri et al. 2017; Butler et al. 2018). Although still very much an active area of research, transfer of metacognitive training may be more effective (Jones et al. 2020; Schaeffner et al. 2021) than transfer of cognitive training, which demonstrates limited generalization (Sala and Gobet 2017, 2019).
Accumulating evidence indicates that the neural mechanisms underlying improvements in executive function from cognitive training are due to changes in functional connectivity, such as increased neural synchrony between frontal and parietal regions (Constantinidis and Klingberg 2016). Potential drivers of this increased functional connectivity include stronger synaptic connections (Gibson et al. 2014), increased myelination of the connecting axons (Yeung et al. 2014), or increased release rate of dopamine (Backman et al. 2011). The central role of executive functions in metacognitive skills (del Missier et al. 2012) motivates the application of interventions from cognitive psychology—which are designed to target the cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying executive functions—to enhance metacognitive awareness. This represents a promising direction for future interventions and studies that aim to confer benefits that generalize beyond the training context.
An emerging area of research in psychology investigates the beneficial effects of mindfulness meditation on metacognition. Evidence indicates that mindfulness training cultivates moment-to-moment awareness of the self and environment (Wallace 2006) and to this extent, mindfulness training heightens metacognition (Austin 1998). This provides a pathway to explain robust experimental findings on the impact of mindfulness training on metacognition (Zeidan et al. 2010; Solem et al. 2015). Neuroscience evidence further demonstrates that mindfulness meditation induces changes in functional brain connectivity within the ventral attention network (Brefczynski-Lewis et al. 2007), dorsal anterior cingulate (Tang et al. 2009, 2010), medial frontal cortex (Hölzel et al. 2007), temporal parietal junction, and pons (Hölzel et al. 2011). These findings suggest that mindfulness meditation enhances the cognitive and neural mechanisms of executive function (which is an independent but highly overlapping construct with metacognitive awareness (Fernandez-Duque et al. 2000)) and motivates applications to metacognition.
A complementary literature in health psychology investigates the efficacy of moderate intensity physical activity and aerobic fitness training to enhance executive functions (for reviews, see Hillman et al. 2008; Voss et al. 2011; Guiney and Machado 2013). A recent meta-analysis demonstrates that physical activity and aerobic fitness enhances executive functions, observing an effect size gain of 0.34 across 36 studies (Northey et al. 2017). Neuroscience evidence further indicates that the observed improvements in executive functions are linked to the effects of physical activity and aerobic fitness on brain structure and function. A growing body of evidence indicates that aerobic fitness promotes efficient functional connectivity within brain networks for executive function, primarily within the fronto-parietal network (Colcombe et al. 2004; Voss et al. 2010a, 2011). For example, Voss et al. (2010b) demonstrated that a 1-year walking intervention was associated with increased functional connectivity within the fronto-parietal network of healthy older adults. Taken together, evidence from health psychology and neuroscience demonstrates that aerobic fitness improves executive functions, suggesting that physical fitness may have beneficial effects on associated skills for metacognitive awareness.
The reviewed findings support the efficacy of modern interventions from psychology to enhance metacognitive awareness and associated executive functions. Although unimodal approaches to intervention represent the most commonly applied method, an emerging body of evidence examines the efficacy of multi-modal interventions designed to leverage the beneficial effects of multiple intervention modalities. For example, recent evidence demonstrates that multi-modal cognitive and physical fitness training produces greater improvements in executive functions compared to unimodal training (Fabre et al. 2002; Oswald et al. 2006; Ward et al. 2017; Daugherty et al. 2018; Zwilling et al. 2019). A meta-analysis investigating the combined effects of cognitive and physical fitness training across 20 studies concluded that multi-modal training delivers synergistic effects that enhance performance more than physical fitness training or cognitive training alone (Lauenroth et al. 2016). Although the mechanisms underlying the beneficial effects of multi-modal training are still under investigation, animal models suggest that both cognitive and physical fitness training may promote neural plasticity and stimulate neurogenesis (Fabel 2009). These findings support the efficacy of multi-modal interventions—providing evidence that this approach can enhance performance on tests of executive function and further motivating their potential for the promotion of metacognitive awareness, collective intelligence, and subjective wellbeing.

4. Summary

In this short review article, we brought together findings from psychology, cognitive neuroscience, and the social science of intelligence to argue for a virtual cycle of global wellbeing that can be accelerated via interventions to enhance individual metacognition (due its central role in collective intelligence). Notably, there are methods from cognitive training, mindfulness meditation, and kinesiology that have been demonstrated to enhance metacognitive awareness, which can be deployed as large-scale interventions. As noted, promoting global population-level and individual wellbeing via improved metacognition will be fundamentally intertwined with addressing vexing societal problems such as inequity.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.R.V. and A.K.B.; writing—original draft preparation, L.R.V.; writing—review and editing, L.R.V. and A.K.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Au, Jacky, Ellen Sheehan, Nancy Tsai, Greg J. Duncan, Martin Buschkuehl, and Susanne M. Jaeggi. 2014. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22: 366–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Austin, James H. 1998. Zen and the Brain: Toward an Understanding of Meditation and Consciousness. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Backman, Lars, Lars Nyberg, Anna Soveri, Jarkko Johansson, Micael Andersson, Erika Dahlin, Anna Stigsdotter Neely, Jere R. Virta, Matti Laine, and Juha O. Rinne. 2011. Effects of working-memory training on striatal dopamine release. Science 333: 718–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Batha, Kate, and Marie Carroll. 2007. Metacognitive training aids decision-making. Australian Journal of Psychology 59: 64–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bates, Timothy C., and Shivani Gupta. 2017. Smart groups of smart people: Evidence for IQ as the origin of collective intelligence in the performance of human groups. Intelligence 60: 46–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Boehm, Julia K., and Laura D. Kubzansky. 2012. The heart’s content: The association between positive psychological well-being and cardiovascular health. Psychological Bulletin 138: 655–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Brefczynski-Lewis, Julie A., Antoine Lutz, Hillary S. Schaefer, Daniel B. Levinson, and Richard J. Davidson. 2007. Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104: 11483–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Butler, Mary, Ellen McCreedy, Victoria A. Nelson, Priyanka Desai, Edward Ratner, Howard A. Fink, Laura S. Hemmy, J. Riley McCarten, Terry R. Barclay, Michelle Brasure, and et al. 2018. Does cognitive training prevent cognitive decline? Annals of Internal Medicine 168: 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Cantril, Hadley. 1965. The Pattern of Human Concerns. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. [Google Scholar]
  10. Colcombe, Stanley J., Arthur F. Kramer, Kirk I. Erickson, Paige Scalf, Edward McAuley, Neal J. Cohen, Andrew Webb, Gerry J. Jerome, David X. Marquez, and Steriani Elavsky. 2004. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 101: 3316–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Constantinidis, Christos, and Torkel Klingberg. 2016. The neuroscience of working memory capacity and training. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 17: 438–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Coyle, Thomas R. 2021. Defining and measuring intelligence: The psychometrics and neuroscience of g. In The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence and Cognitive Neuroscience. Edited by Aron K. Barbey, Sherif Karama and Richard J. Haier. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Daugherty, Ana M., Christopher Zwilling, Erick J. Paul, Nikolai Sherepa, Courtney Allen, Arthur F. Kramer, Charles H. Hillman, Neal J. Cohen, and Aron K. Barbey. 2018. Multi-modal fitness and cognitive training to enhance fluid intelligence. Intelligence 66: 32–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. del Missier, Fabio, Timo Mäntylä, and Waendi Bruine De Bruin. 2012. Decision-making competence, executive functioning, and general cognitive abilities. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 25: 331–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Diener, Ed, and Louis Tay. 2015. Subjective well-being and human welfare around the world as reflected in the Gallup World Poll. International Journal of Psychology 50: 135–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Dolan, Paul, and Mathew P. White. 2007. How can measures of subjective well-being be used to inform public policy? Perspectives on Psychological Science 2: 71–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Fabel, Klaus. 2009. Additive effects of physical exercise and environmental enrichment on adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice. Frontiers in Neuroscience 3: 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Fabre, Claudine, Karim Chamari, Patrick Mucci, J. Masse-Biron, and C. Prefaut. 2002. Improvement of cognitive function by mental and/or individualized aerobic training in healthy elderly subjects. International Journal of Sports Medicine 23: 415–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Fernandez-Duque, Diego, Jodie A. Baird, and Michael I. Posner. 2000. Executive attention and metacognitive regulation. Consciousness and Cognition 9: 288–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Fernando, Mario, and Rafi M. M. I. Chowdhury. 2010. The relationship between spiritual well-being and ethical orientations in decision-making: An empirical study with business executives in Australia. Journal of Business Ethics 95: 211–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fioramonti, Lorenzo, Luca Coscieme, and Lars F. Mortensen. 2019. From gross domestic product to well-being: How alternative indicators can help connect the new economy with the Sustainable Development Goals. The Anthropocene Review 6: 207–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Fisher, Matthew. 2019. A theory of public well-being. BMC Public Health 19: 1283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Gibson, Erin M., David Purger, Christopher W. Mount, Andrea K. Goldstein, Grant L. Lin, Lauren S. Wood, Ingrid Inema, Sarah E. Miller, Gregor Bieri, J. Bradley Zuchero, and et al. 2014. Neuronal activity promotes oligodendrogenesis and adaptive myelination in the mammalian brain. Science 344: 1252304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Guiney, Hayley, and Liana Machado. 2013. Benefits of regular aerobic exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20: 73–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Gupta, Pranav, and Anita Williams Woolley. 2020. The emergence of collective intelligence behavior. In Proceedings of the 2020 ACM Collective Intelligence Conference. New York: ACM. [Google Scholar]
  26. Harrison, George M., and Lisa M. Vallin. 2018. Evaluating the metacognitive awareness inventory using empirical factor-structure evidence. Metacognition Learning 13: 15–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hillman, Charles H., Kirk I. Erickson, and Arthur F. Kramer. 2008. Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Hölzel, Britta K., James Carmody, Mark Vangel, Christina Congleton, Sita M. Yerramsetti, Tim Gard, and Sara W. Lazar. 2011. Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 191: 36–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Hölzel, Britta K., Ulrich Ott, Hannes Hempel, Andrea Hackl, Katharina Wolf, Rudolf Stark, and Dieter Vaitl. 2007. Differential engagement of anterior cingulate and adjacent medial frontal cortex in adept meditators and non-meditators. Neuroscience Letters 421: 16–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. James, Harvey S., Jr., and Athanasios Chymis. 2004. Are Happy People Ethical People? Evidence from North America and Europe. Department of Agricultural Economics Working Paper No. AEWP 2004-08. Columbia: University of Missouri Columbia. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Jones, Jonathan S., Fraser Milton, Mohammod Mostazir, and Anna R. Adlam. 2020. The academic outcomes of working memory and metacognitive strategy training in children: A double-blind randomized controlled trial. Developmental Science 23: e12870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Jost, John T., Arie W. Kruglanski, and Thomas O. Nelson. 1998. Social metacognition: An expansionist review. Personality and Social Psychology Review 2: 137–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Kiaei, Yalda Amir, and Thomas G. Reio Jr. 2014. Goal pursuit and eudaimonic well-being among university students: Metacognition as the mediator. Behavioral Development Bulletin 19: 91–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Kleitman, Sabina, Jessica Sik-Wai Hui, and Yixin Jiang. 2019. Confidence to spare: Individual differences in cognitive and metacognitive arrogance and competence. Metacognition and Learning 14: 479–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Koriat, Asher. 2015. Metacognition: Decision making processes in self-monitoring and self-regulation. In The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making, II. Edited by Gideon Keren and George Wu. Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 356–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lambert, Louise, Tim Lomas, Margot P. van de Weijer, Holli Anne Passmore, Mohsen Joshanloo, Jim Harter, Yoshiki Ishikawa, Alden Lai, Takuya Kitagawa, Dominique Chen, and et al. 2020. Towards a greater global understanding of well-being: A proposal for a more inclusive measure. International Journal of Wellbeing 10: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lauenroth, Andreas, Anestis E. Ioannidis, and Birgit Teichmann. 2016. Influence of combined physical and cognitive training on cognition: A systematic review. BMC Geriatrics 16: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Malone, Thomas W., and Anita Williams Woolley. 2020. Collective intelligence. In The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 780–801. [Google Scholar]
  39. Mao, Andrew. 2016. Toward a quantitative understanding of teamwork and collective intelligence. In Proceedings of the 2016 AAAI Spring Symposium. Palo Alto: AAAI Press. [Google Scholar]
  40. Mehl, Matthias R., Simine Vazire, Shannon E. Holleran, and C. Shelby Clark. 2010. Eavesdropping on happiness: Wellbeing is related to having less small talk and more substantive conversations. Psychological Science 21: 539–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Nelson, Donna Webster. 2009. Feeling good and open-minded: The impact of positive affect on cross cultural empathic responding. Journal of Positive Psychology 4: 53–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ng, Weiting, and Ed Diener. 2019. Affluence and subjective well-being: Does income inequality moderate their associations? Applied Research Quality Life 14: 155–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Nima, Ali Al, Kevin M. Cloninger, Björn N. Persson, Sverker Sikström, and Danilo Garcia. 2020. Validation of subjective well-being measures using item response theory. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 3036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Northey, Joseph Michael, Nicolas Cherbuin, Kate Louise Pumpa, Disa Jane Smee, and Ben Rattray. 2017. Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: A systematic review with meta-analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine 52: 154–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ohtani, Kazuhiro, and Tetsuya Hisasaka. 2018. Beyond intelligence: A meta-analytic review of the relationship among metacognition, intelligence, and academic performance. Metacognition and Learning 13: 179–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Oswald, Wolf D., Thomas Gunzelmann, Roland Rupprecht, and Bernd Hagen. 2006. Differential effects of single versus combined cognitive and physical training with older adults: The SimA study in a 5-year perspective. European Journal of Ageing 3: 179–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Pancheva, Marta G., Carol D. Ryff, and Mario Lucchini. 2021. An integrated look at well-being: Topological clustering of combinations and correlates of hedonia and eudaimonia. Journal of Happiness Studies 22: 2275–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Paul, Erick J., J. David Smith, Vivian V. Valentin, Benjamin O. Turner, Aron K. Barbey, and F. Gregory Ashby. 2015. Neural networks underlying the metacognitive uncertainty response. Cortex 71: 306–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Pavot, William, and Ed Diener. 2008. The Satisfaction With Life Scale and the emerging construct of life satisfaction. The Journal of Positive Psychology 3: 137–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Philippi, Carissa L., Justin S. Feinstein, Sahib S. Khalsa, Antonio Damasio, Daniel Tranel, Gregory Landini, Kenneth Williford, and David Rudrauf. 2012. Preserved self-awareness following extensive bilateral brain damage to the insula, anterior cingulate, and medial prefrontal cortices. PLoS ONE 7: e38413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Rand, David G., Gordon Kraft-Todd, and June Gruber. 2015. The collective benefits of feeling good and letting go: Positive emotion and (dis)inhibition interact to predict cooperative behavior. PLoS ONE 10: e0117426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Richards, Marcus, and Felicia A. Huppert. 2011. Do positive children become positive adults? Evidence from a longitudinal birth cohort study. The Journal of Positive Psychology 6: 75–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Riedl, Christoph, Young Ji Kim, Pranav Gupta, Thomas W. Malone, and Anita Williams Woolley. 2021. Quantifying collective intelligence in human groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 118: e2005737118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Sala, Giovanni, and Fernand Gobet. 2017. Does far transfer exist? Negative evidence from chess, music, and working memory training. Current Directions in Psychological Science 26: 515–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Sala, Giovanni, and Fernand Gobet. 2019. Cognitive training does not enhance general cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23: 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Schaeffner, Simone, Nicolas Chevalier, Maki Kubota, and Julia Karbach. 2021. Metacognitive training. In Cognitive Training. Edited by Tilo Strobach and Julia Karbach. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 255–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Schraw, Gregory, and Rayne Sperling Dennison. 1994. Assessing metacognitive awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology 19: 460–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Shanafelt, Tait D., Colin West, Xinghua Zhao, Paul Novotny, Joseph Kolars, Thomas Habermann, and Jeff Sloan. 2005. Relationship between increased personal well-being and enhanced empathy among internal medicine residents. Journal of General Internal Medicine 20: 559–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Simons, Daniel J., Walter R. Boot, Neil Charness, Susan E. Gathercole, Christopher F. Chabris, David Z. Hambrick, and Elizabeth A. L. Stine-Morrow. 2016. Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychological Science in the Public Interest 17: 103–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Solem, Stian, Susanne Semb Thunes, Odin Hjemdal, Roger Hagen, and Adrian Wells. 2015. A metacognitive perspective on mindfulness: An empirical investigation. BMC Psychology 3: 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Son, Joonmo, and John Wilson. 2012. Volunteer work and hedonic, eudemonic, and social well-being. Sociological Forum 27: 658–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Soveri, Anna, Jan Antfolk, Linda Karlsson, Benny Salo, and Matti Laine. 2017. Working memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24: 1077–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Talukdar, Tanveer, Francisco J. Román, Joachim T. Operskalski, Christopher E. Zwilling, and Aron K. Barbey. 2018. Individual differences in decision-making competence revealed by multivariate fMRI. Human Brain Mapping 39: 2664–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Tang, Yi-Yuan, Qilin Lu, Xiujuan Geng, Elliot A. Stein, Yihong Yang, and Michael I. Posner. 2010. Short-term meditation induces white matter changes in the anterior cingulate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107: 15649–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Tang, Yi-Yuan, Yinghua Ma, Yaxin Fan, Hongbo Feng, Junhong Wang, Shigang Feng, Qilin Lu, Bing Hu, Yao Lin, Jian Li, and et al. 2009. Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106: 8865–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Tovey, Mark, ed. 2015. Collective Intelligence: Creating a Prosperous World at Peace. Scotts Valley: CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform. [Google Scholar]
  67. Voss, Michelle W., Kirk I. Erickson, Ruchika S. Prakash, Laura Chaddock, Edward Malkowski, Heloisa Alves, Jennifer S. Kim, Katherine S. Morris, Siobhan M. White, Thomas R. Wójcicki, and et al. 2010a. Functional connectivity: A source of variance in the association between cardiorespiratory fitness and cognition? Neuropsychologia 48: 1394–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Voss, Michelle W., Kirk I. Erickson, Ruchika S. Prakash, Laura Chaddock, Edward Malkowski, Heloisa Alves, Jennifer S. Kim, Katherine S. Morris, Siobhan M. White, Thomas R. Wójcicki, and et al. 2010b. Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 2: 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. Voss, Michelle W., Lindsay S. Nagamatsu, Teresa Liu-Ambrose, and Arthur F. Kramer. 2011. Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. Journal of Applied Physiology 111: 1505–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Wallace, B. Alan. 2006. The Attention Revolution: Unlocking the Power of the Focused Mind. Boston: Wisdom Publications. [Google Scholar]
  71. Ward, Nathan, Erick J. Paul, Patrick D. Watson, Gillian Cooke, Charles Hillman, Neal J. Cohen, Arthur F. Kramer, and Aron K. Barbey. 2017. Enhanced learning through multimodal training: Evidence from a comprehensive cognitive, physical fitness, and neuroscience intervention. Scientific Reports 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Whitfield-Gabrieli, Susan, Joseph M. Moran, Alfonso Nieto-Castañón, Christina Triantafyllou, Rebecca Saxe, and John D. E. Gabrieli. 2011. Associations and dissociations between default and self-reference networks in the human brain. NeuroImage 55: 225–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Wiest, Maja, Benjamin Schüz, Noah Webster, and Susanne Wurm. 2011. Subjective well-being and mortality revisited: Differential effects of cognitive and emotional facets of well-being on mortality. Health Psychology 30: 728–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Woolley, Anita Williams, Christopher F. Chabris, Alex Pentland, Nada Hashmi, and Thomas W. Malone. 2010. Evidence for a collective intelligence factor in the performance of human groups. Science 330: 686–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Yang, Ying, Peipei Li, Xinyuan Fu, and Yu Kou. 2017. Orientations to happiness and subjective well-being in Chinese adolescents: The roles of prosocial behavior and internet addictive behavior. Journal of Happiness Studies 18: 1747–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Yang, Zhi, Catie Chang, Ting Xu, Lili Jiang, Daniel A. Handwerker, F. Xavier Castellanos, Michael P. Milham, Peter A. Bandettini, and Xi-Nian Zuo. 2014. Connectivity trajectory across lifespan differentiates the precuneus from the default network. NeuroImage 89: 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Yeung, Maggie S. Y., Sofia Zdunek, Olaf Bergmann, Samuel Bernard, Mehran Salehpour, Kanar Alkass, Shira Perl, John Tisdale, Göran Possnert, Lou Brundin, and et al. 2014. Dynamics of oligodendrocyte generation and myelination in the human brain. Cell 159: 766–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  78. Zeidan, Fadel, Susan K. Johnson, Bruce J. Diamond, Zhanna David, and Paula Goolkasian. 2010. Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition 19: 597–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Zwilling, Christopher E., Ana M. Daugherty, Charles H. Hillman, Arthur F. Kramer, Neal J. Cohen, and Aron K. Barbey. 2019. Enhanced decision-making through multimodal training. Science of Learning 4: 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Varshney, L.R.; Barbey, A.K. Beyond IQ: The Importance of Metacognition for the Promotion of Global Wellbeing. J. Intell. 2021, 9, 54. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9040054

AMA Style

Varshney LR, Barbey AK. Beyond IQ: The Importance of Metacognition for the Promotion of Global Wellbeing. Journal of Intelligence. 2021; 9(4):54. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9040054

Chicago/Turabian Style

Varshney, Lav R., and Aron K. Barbey. 2021. "Beyond IQ: The Importance of Metacognition for the Promotion of Global Wellbeing" Journal of Intelligence 9, no. 4: 54. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence9040054

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop