Next Article in Journal
Molecular Detection and Genetic Variability of Hepatozoon canis in Golden Jackals (Canis aureus L. 1758) in Serbia
Next Article in Special Issue
Preliminary Study of Scent Rolling in Captive Wolves (Canis lupus L. 1758) in Response to Olfactory Enrichment
Previous Article in Journal
Immunonutrition, Metabolism, and Programmed Cell Death in Lung Cancer: Translating Bench to Bedside
Previous Article in Special Issue
Raising an Eye at Facial Muscle Morphology in Canids
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Predatory Dogs as Drivers of Social Behavior Changes in the Central Himalayan Langur (Semnopithecus schistaceus) in Agro-Forest Landscapes

1
College of Medicine, Howard University, 520 W St, NW, Washington, DC 20059, USA
2
Department of Anthropology, University of Toronto, Scarborough 1265 Military Trail, Toronto, ON M1C 1A4, Canada
3
Bharatiya Vidya Bhavan’s, Bhavan’s College, University of Mumbai, Andheri (w), Mumbai 400 058, India
4
Department of Geography and Environmental Studies, Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, ON K1S 5B6, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Biology 2024, 13(6), 410; https://doi.org/10.3390/biology13060410
Submission received: 23 April 2024 / Revised: 28 May 2024 / Accepted: 30 May 2024 / Published: 4 June 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Biology, Ecology, Management and Conservation of Canidae)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

In human-modified landscapes, wildlife species are increasingly pushed into unfamiliar territories due to habitat fragmentation caused by urbanization and agricultural development. This shift poses various challenges for these species, including interactions with humans and predators like free-ranging dogs. To understand how wildlife adapts to such circumstances, we examined the behavioral strategies of the Central Himalayan langur (CHL) in response to the presence of predatory dogs and humans. Over two years, we observed a group of CHLs living in a fragmented landscape, frequently foraging on agricultural and fodder crops, and thus encountering villagers and free-ranging dogs. Our research found significant changes in the CHLs’ major activities, such as resting, feeding, moving, and socializing in the presence of dogs and humans. Specifically, CHLs performed increased movement and feeding and reduced social activity. These behavioral adaptations likely aim to optimize survival in a challenging habitat. However, such modifications in the long term could lead to issues for both humans and wildlife. Increased movement and feeding in agricultural areas may lead to high negative interactions with farmers and dogs, while reduced social activity could impact social dynamics and reproductive success. Implementing sustainable agricultural practices, providing alternative livelihood options, promoting coexistence strategies, and engaging local communities in conservation efforts will be essential steps toward minimizing these concerns.

Abstract

Globally, habitat fragmentation has increased the proximity between wildlife, humans, and emerging predators such as free-ranging dogs. In these fragmented landscapes, encounters between primates and dogs are escalating, with primates often falling victim to dog attacks while navigating patchy landscapes and fragmented forests. We aim to investigate how these primates deal with the simultaneous threats posed by humans and predators, specifically focusing on the adaptive strategies of Central Himalayan langur (CHL) in the landscape of fear. To address this, we conducted a behavioral study on the CHL in an agro-forest landscape, studying them for a total of 3912 h over two consecutive years. Our results indicate that, compared to their most common resting behavior, CHLs allocate more time to feeding and locomotion, and less time to socializing in the presence of humans and predatory dogs. Additionally, they exhibit increased feeding and locomotion and reduced social behavior in agro-forest or open habitats. These behavioral patterns reflect adaptive responses to the landscape of fear, where the presence of predators significantly influences their behavior and resource utilization. This study suggests measures to promote coexistence between humans and wildlife through the integration of effective management strategies that incorporate both ecological and social dimensions of human–wildlife interactions.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Domestic dogs Canis familiaris are globally the most widespread human-introduced carnivores [1]. Originating roughly 14,000 years ago [2], the dog population grew alongside the human population, with estimates nearing a billion worldwide [3]. Through their long-term association with humans, dogs have become integral members of human society, fulfilling various roles, such as pets, protectors, and guards for agriculture and livestock [4,5]. Across the globe, dogs exhibit varying degrees of free-ranging behavior, bringing them into interactions with wildlife on multiple levels [1]. Particularly in developing nations, the threat of free-ranging dogs escalates as human populations expand into natural areas in search of suitable agricultural land and natural resources [6]. Despite their long history as human companions and workers, the impact of dogs on wildlife hinges on their management practices, whether they are fully domesticated and leashed, roam freely, rely on human-provided food, or lead independent lives. Additionally, the proximity of dogs to natural reserves or urban pockets of wildlife plays a significant role in the frequency and nature of interactions with wildlife [6,7].
Despite their morphological differences from wolves due to domestication, dogs retain characteristics of the carnivore guild [1,4]. In certain regions, dogs have emerged as the most abundant carnivores, disrupting ecosystems [8] and negatively impacting wildlife. Dog behaviors, including direct predation, harassment, and competition, can lead to fear-induced behavioral changes in wildlife and affect their resource base and ultimately their fitness. In addition, the mere presence of dogs can lead to disease transmission [9,10,11]. Prominent review articles provide insightful analyses of the complex dynamics of human–dog–wildlife interactions, highlighting the need for effective management strategies to mitigate adverse effects on biodiversity [1,4,7,12,13]. However, there remains a need to understand how dogs are increasingly impacting individual species of wildlife as humans expand their agricultural areas.
Primate species represent one of the most diverse groups of mammals, totaling 504 species [14]. Unfortunately, approximately 65% of these species are currently under threat of extinction due to habitat loss and resource depletion [15]. Primate habitats are becoming significantly fragmented as a result of rising human demand for agricultural land, industrial growth, and the extraction of natural resources (e.g., mining, fuelwood), [16,17]. Over 50% of tropical and subtropical forest habitats, which are home to a large diversity of primates worldwide, have become fragmented [16]. In these fragmented landscapes, encounters between primates and invasive species, notably dogs, are growing, as primates often fall victim to dog attacks while navigating patchy landscapes and fragmented forests [18,19]. For example, dogs have been hunting and eating white-tailed small-eared Galagos Otolemur garnettii lasiotis in Kenya’s fragmented landscapes [19]. Similarly, in Madagascar, dogs have also been observed preying on endangered wild ring-tailed lemurs Lemur catta [20], and similar incidents involving langurs and macaques have been reported from comparable landscapes in Asia [21,22,23,24]. The presence of predators, including dogs, profoundly influences primate behavior and activity patterns. Primates, like many other species, have adapted to minimize the risk of predation through behavioral strategies such as alarm calls, vigilance, and counter-attacking [25,26]. Studies conducted in various ecosystems have shown how the perceived risk of predation alters vigilance behavior [27], movement patterns [28], foraging strategies [29], and social dynamics [30].
Anthropogenic impacts on animal behavior result from both direct human disturbances, like fragmentation, and indirect disturbances, such as the introduction of alien species [31]. The initial reactions of animals to these changes are often behavioral, helping them adapt to new conditions [32,33,34]. Primates living in anthropogenically disturbed areas regularly show changes in habitat use, foraging, and activity budget [35,36]. For instance, common chimpanzees Pan troglodytes verus in Guinea, West Africa, have shown a preference for traveling, resting, and socializing in habitat types with lower levels of human-induced pressure [35]. Compared to natural processes, anthropogenic pressures expose animals to environments they have not experienced in their evolutionary history, and behavioral changes are occurring at faster rates [37]. Even though these behavioral adaptations might have short-term benefits, they may also disrupt other long-evolved, fitness-related behaviors (e.g., social structure, reproductive strategies), which could negatively impact species persistence and have cascading effects on ecosystems [38,39,40].
Shared agroforest environments offer a complex web of opportunities and costs to wildlife. Our study species, Central Himalayan langurs (CHLs), inhabit Himalayan high-altitude agroforest landscapes. CHLs dwell within fragmented natural oak forests and rely on seasonal crops (May–June and September–October), as well as on the leaves of fodder trees that grow along the edges of agricultural fields year-round [41,42]. However, foraging within agricultural fields exposes langurs to their main predator, feral dogs [18,43], and increases the likelihood of frequent negative encounters with local people [40,43]. Predator presence can create a ‘landscape of fear’ and can influence the behavior and distribution of prey species within an ecosystem. Thus, ecological dynamics for CHLs in such agroforest landscapes are not solely driven by the availability of resources, but also by the fear of predation and the strategies animals employ to mitigate that risk [44,45,46,47]. By understanding how animals adapt their behavior to minimize risks while maximizing opportunities in shared landscapes, we can develop more effective approaches for managing human–wildlife interactions and foster sustainable coexistence.
While a growing number of case studies describe behavioral responses to human-induced rapid environmental change in specific systems [46], our understanding of such changes due to predatory feral dogs remains limited. It is well established that the presence of dogs alters the spatio-temporal behavior of wildlife [47], escalating competition for space and resources [48], which leads to broader negative consequences such as predation, the transmission of diseases to wildlife, the disturbance of natural habitats, and the potential for hybridization with native species [4,7,12,13,17]. However, to our knowledge, the direct influence of these impacts on animal behavior patterns has not been extensively explored in previous studies.
This study explores how the fear landscape, shaped by both human and predatory dog presence, influences the activity patterns of CHLs in agroforest environments. Our goal is to gain insight into the complex structure of fear behavior and identify specific adaptations in response to perceived threats. We hypothesize that (1) the presence of dogs and humans induces a disturbance in the behavior of CHLs, and that (2) habitats associated with these fear landscapes impact CHL behavior patterns. Consequently, we predict that in fear landscapes, CHLs will modify their activity patterns to reduce the likelihood of encounters with dogs and humans.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Site and Population

The study was conducted from May 2022 to December 2023 in Mandal Valley, Chamoli, Uttarakhand, Garhwal Himalayas, India (coordinates: 30°28′04″ N 79°16′31″ E; elevation: 1500–1800 m above sea level). The habituation process for the study group, “S Group”, began in 2014. Since then, this group has remained the subject of continuous studies, up to the present day. Consequently, the study group has become habituated to the presence of researchers. The total home range of the S Group encompasses both natural forests and agricultural fields, frequently bringing them into proximity with humans and predatory domestic dogs (Figure 1). For further details on previous studies conducted on the S Group, please refer to the following studies [18,40,41,42,49,50]. Every member of the group, as well as each newcomer, was individually identified and monitored throughout the duration of the study. During the observation time for this study (May 2022 to December 2023), the group consisted of 64 individuals, comprising 12 infants, 9 juveniles, 16 subadults, 20 adult females, and 7 adult males.

2.2. Behavioral Data Collection

Data were collected on 43 adult and subadult (age more than 3 years) langurs of both sexes for two consecutive years. Data collection was carried out over eight months in 2022 (May to December) and ten months in 2023 (January to December). The S Group was tracked from 06:00 to 18:00 by three observers (the authors HN and VM, and a field assistant) for a total of 326 days, averaging 20 days per month. Three times each observation hour (i.e., every 20 min), we conducted an instantaneous scan sample that could last up to 15 min [51], for a total of 3912 h of observation—240 h on average per month (2052 h in 2022 and 1860 h in 2023).
Throughout the 15 min scan, all state behaviors of the visible S Group members were recorded, noting the activity that individuals were engaged in when first spotted. Observations were conducted using binoculars from a distance of more than 10 m. Two observers (one author and the field assistant) were consistently present at the study site during observations. To ensure the reliability of our data, we assessed inter-observer agreement between the two observers, which yielded a high level of agreement (Cohen’s kappa = 0.70). Agreement values falling between 0.60 and 0.74 were considered indicative of good agreement [52] Given that observers were consistently present during all recorded behaviors, they were highly unlikely to have influenced the observed behavioral changes.
The dogs were fully habituated to the presence of humans/researchers. Both dog and human presence were documented during each scan. If dogs and humans were within approximately 10 m of any S Group individual during a particular scan, they were noted as present; otherwise, they were considered absent. The selection of the 10 m distance for our study is the culmination of a decade-long research effort. We found that the S Group frequently inhabits areas where human activities, along with the presence of dogs, are common, such as villages, roads, and agricultural fields. At distances beyond 10 m, langurs tend to exhibit neutral behavioral responses in such settings. However, when humans and predatory dogs come into close proximity with langurs, their behavior is significantly affected. Therefore, we concentrated our investigation within this critical range to better understand these behavioral dynamics. Habitat-type codes were recorded for each scan sample, and further details of these codes are presented in the Supplementary Information.

2.3. Behavioral Definitions

Using the comprehensive ethogram outlined by Dolhinow [53], we categorized langur behavior into four broad state behaviors, as detailed below:
  • Feeding: When an animal is ingesting or masticating food.
  • Locomotion: An animal moves, at any pace, from one location to another for a distance of more than 1 m.
  • Resting: An animal remains stationary, hindquarters on the supporting surface, either asleep (i.e., eyes closed) or awake (i.e., alert, with eyes open). It may simultaneously engage in l vigilance behavior.
  • Social: Social interactions like grooming, play, or sexual activity.

2.4. Study Variables

Presence of predatory dog(s): In all the models, a binary value of this variable is used to denote whether one or more dogs were present within a 10 m radius of the S Group or not. These free-roaming dogs may have belonged to villagers or been stray dogs within the S Group’s territory. Presence of human(s): In all the models, a binary value of this variable is used to denote whether one or more humans were present within a 10 m radius of the S Group or not. Humans usually included villagers or occasional tourists within the S Group’s territory. Habitat: The study group’s habitat encompasses various areas, including dense forest patches, open patches near human settlements, and agricultural fields (Figure 1). Initially, the entire study area was categorized based on location and vegetation, which were later combined into two forest types—open forests and dense forests—across all models. Further details regarding these classifications are presented in Supplementary Tables S1 and S2.

2.5. Model Formulation and Statistical Analysis

To investigate the potential impact of our study variables on the four major behaviors of CHLs, we conducted a Multinomial Logistic Regression Mixed Model (MLRMM) analysis using the nnet package [54,55] in R version 4.2.2 [56]. In MLRMM, the log odds provide an estimate of the relationship between predictor variables (habitat, presence of dogs, presence of humans) and the probabilities of each categorical outcome variable (the four types of behavior) in comparison to the reference category. Specifically, we used the log odds of feeding, locomotion, and social behavior relative to resting behavior as the baseline for all models. The selection of the reference category should be driven by theoretical or substantive considerations [57]. Thus, we use resting behavior as the reference category, given it is a dominant behavior in colobines for digesting their folivorous diet [58]. Additionally, selecting a reference category with a substantial number of observations can enhance the precision of estimates [59]. To control for the systematic variation in the presence and absence of humans and dogs that occurs over time, such as seasonal patterns or changes in human activity, the month was included as a random factor in all models. We conducted tests for outliers, the homogeneity of residuals, and variance inflation factors using the car package in R [60]. The results indicated no violations of the model assumptions. Specifically, all VIF values were below 3, suggesting no significant multicollinearity among the predictor variables [61]. Furthermore, we confirmed the validity and reliability of our modeling approach by using squared Mahalanobis distance residuals to diagnose various types of overdispersion [62], as well as conducting a score test to assess the goodness of fit of the most complex model [63].
MLR serves as an extension of binary logistic regression, accommodating more than two categories of dependent variables. MLRMM, like binary logistic regression, models the log odds of the outcomes as a linear combination of the predictor variables and employs maximum likelihood estimation to assess the probability of categorical membership, generating a unique set of parameters for each level of the dependent variable, referred to as regression coefficients (log odds) [64]. To understand how different factors affect the likelihood of each category of the outcome variable, we identified three predictor variables: presence of dog, presence of human, and habitat. We constructed nine potential models based on these variables and compared them based on Akaike’s information criteria (AIC). We applied information theory (IT) to identify the best model [65,66,67,68]. This method’s fundamental principle uses ΔAIC values to rank the model among the candidate set of models (Δi = AIC(i) − AIC (min)) [65]. Standardized model weights express the probability that a given model is the best among those in a set of models (Table 1). With the “aictab” function from the AICcmodavg package, we assessed the Akaike’s weight, or relative likelihood, and the cumulative weight of each model [69]. These metrics indicate the degree to which one model is more likely than another to explain variance in the data. AIC is the preferred method for model selection in ecology and evolution due to its effectiveness in addressing methodological challenges [70,71,72]. This method has been widely adopted in primate behavior studies across various species, including Japanese macaques [73] Macaca fuscata fuscata, vervet monkeys [74] (Chlorocebus pygerythrus), common chimpanzees [75]) Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, black howler monkeys [76] Alouatta pigra, and mountain gorillas [77] Gorilla beringei beringei.

3. Results

Selection of Activity Pattern

Among the nine models that were tested, the integrated model emerged as the most accurate in explaining the factors influencing the activity patterns of CHLs. This model demonstrated the lowest AIC value and the highest model probability, with a weight of 1 (as shown in Table 1). The integrated model incorporates all independent variables (dog presence, human presence, habitat type). In contrast, the remaining eight models, including the null model (as presented in Table 1), collectively exhibited a probability of less than 0.01.
In the presence of predatory dogs, the log odds value of adult CHLs engaging in feeding activities over resting activities was 0.006, whereas for locomotion and social activities, it was 0.202 and −0.455, respectively (see Table 2). Free ranging dogs significantly influenced the activity patterns of adult langurs, resulting in increased feeding and locomotion but decreased social activity when dogs were present in their habitat (see Figure 2a, Table 2; z = 1.698, z = 6.113, z = −11.375; p < 0.01).
Similarly, in the presence of humans, the log odds value of adult CHLs engaging in feeding activities over resting activities was 0.166, whereas for locomotion and social activities, it was 0.249 and −0.267, respectively (see Table 2). Adult langurs exhibited increased feeding and locomotion but decreased social activity when humans were present (Figure 2b, Table 2; z = 4.582, z = 7.875, z = −7.334; p < 0.01).
In open habitats, the log odds value of adult CHLs engaging in feeding activities over resting activities was 0.464, whereas for locomotion and social activities, it was 0.182 and −0.622, respectively (see Table 2). Habitat type significantly influenced the activity patterns of adult langurs, resulting in increased feeding and locomotion but decreased social activity in open habitats (Figure 2c, Table 2; z = 15.734, z = 6.261, z = −18.306; p < 0.01).

4. Discussion

4.1. Behavior Modifications in the Predator-Induced Landscape of Fear

Our results demonstrate that the presence of predatory dogs causes CHLs to alter their behavior, increasing their feeding and movement activity while lowering their social interaction. Colobine populations can differ in feeding and resting patterns, and these differences are more likely to be a result of behavioral adaptations than a byproduct of their shared evolutionary history [78]. By allocating maximum time to resting, colobines optimize their digestive efficiency and maximize the extraction of nutrients from their folivorous diet [58]. Under a landscape of fear, the presence of predators can shape the behavior and distribution of prey species even when predation itself does not occur [79,80,81]. Adjusting the allocation of foraging time is a common strategy for animals to mitigate the risk of predation [82]. Because animals respond to predators through complex, compensatory behavioral strategies, it is difficult to predict how these behaviors will ultimately impact fitness [83]. Prey species facing higher levels of predation risk may allocate more energy toward developing and maintaining anti-predator defenses or reducing other energetically expensive activities [84,85].
The natural Banj oak Quercus leucotrichophora forest, which is the preferred habitat for langurs [41], has become fragmented due to the high dependency of local people on the surrounding natural forest [86,87]. Langurs are increasingly dedicating more time to feeding on fodder crops in agricultural land, primarily due to the fragmentation of their preferred habitat [41,42,43]. Our results suggest that the differences in resource availability in modified agricultural landscapes relative to their preferred oak forests leads to greater feeding and moving, requiring them to expend more energy to forage and navigate their surroundings. These areas also do not provide many safe areas to rest and socialize. Based on 312 langur–dog interactions, all successful predatory attacks by dogs on S Group individuals occurred in agricultural fields, where langurs are notably vulnerable due to the lack of connected tree cover, providing a low chance of escape [18,43]. Our results suggest that despite the threat of predation, langurs increase their feeding time in predator-rich environments because these areas provide crucial resources for them. Similar behavior is also observed among herbivores in Africa; due to the distribution of grasslands and waterholes, grazers often have no choice but to utilize habitats located near the few available waterholes and grasslands, despite the high predation risk associated with these areas [88]. This behavioral adjustment reflects their prioritization of foraging and movement over social activities to maximize their fitness. Additionally, langurs employ high-cost anti-predator strategies such as giving alarm calls and engaging in fights with predators to mitigate the risk of predation [18]. Overall, our findings highlight the complex interactions between habitat fragmentation, foraging behavior, and predation risk in langurs, emphasizing their adaptive responses to changes in their environment to achieve optimal fitness.

4.2. Behavior Modifications in the Human-Induced Landscape of Fear

Similar to the presence of predatory dogs, the presence of humans caused CHLs to alter their behavior, increasing their feeding and movement activity while lowering their social interaction. This effect was partially spatial, as langurs were attempting to forage on crops and thus moving into agricultural areas where they were likely to encounter humans. Human presence can have significant effects on the behavior of various animals, for example, changing the foraging and vigilance behavior of elk Cervus elephus [89] and Nubian ibex Capra nubiana [90]. Human activities such as hunting, habitat modification, and disturbance can create perceived threats for wildlife, for example, in the case of red deer Cervus elaphus and wild boar Sus scrofa, human presence can influence their use of habitat and behavior [44]. The livelihood of local villagers in this region depends on agro-forestry crops [91] such as Grewia optiva, Celtis australis, and Ficus virens, which are also important parts of the CHL diet [49]. In response to the perceived threat posed by langurs to their crops and livelihoods, villagers engage in various negative interactions with the langurs, including throwing stones, using loud noises and firecrackers to chase them away, and even resorting to poisoning [41]. Human behavior and tolerance toward wildlife play crucial roles in shaping the landscape of fear in human-influenced environments [92]. In agroforest contexts in Africa, the economic value of cultivated food crops and the extent of damage caused by common chimpanzees Pan troglodytes can significantly influence human attitudes, tolerance, and behavior toward them [93]. Human tolerance toward CHLs can also influence the effectiveness of management and conservation efforts [41]. The negative attitude of the local community towards langurs is due to the langurs’ frequent consumption of their agricultural and fodder crops. To promote tolerance, it is critical to address the livelihood challenges faced by the local community. By integrating the livelihood needs of local communities into mitigation models, we can enhance their value perception of wildlife. This, in turn, facilitates community support for mitigating human–wildlife interactions through educational initiatives, habitat restoration, and the promotion of coexistence. Our findings contribute toward the understanding of how human behavior and tolerance affect the spatial distribution of CHLs and CHL behavior (i.e., the landscape of fear). This evidence can inform land-use planning and management strategies, such as the identification and establishment of corridors within the territory of the S Group. Consequently, we recommend integrating effective management strategies incorporating ecological and social dimensions of human–wildlife interactions. We can develop comprehensive and socially acceptable interventions by considering factors such as human behavior and tolerance alongside ecological variables like species ecology, population dynamics, and habitat preferences.

4.3. Behavior Modifications in Habitats Associated with the Landscape of Fear

We found that CHLs exhibit an increase in feeding and locomotion activity and a decrease in social interaction and resting in open forest habitats. Primates in fragmented landscapes spend more time feeding to compensate for limited resources [94]. The home range of the S Group includes fragmented patches of dense forest, agricultural land, and human settlements, and they often move between the forest and agricultural fields for crop foraging [41]. Interactions with dogs typically occur in open forest patches or agricultural fields where continuous canopy cover is absent [18,42]. The fragmentation and loss of habitats is a major factor leading to the decline in many species, partly due to increases in predation by human-introduced predators [33]. Primates show changes in their activity budget, foraging and ranging in anthropogenically disturbed environments; for example, common chimpanzees Pan troglodytes verus prefer to rest, travel, and socialize in habitats with less human activity [35]. In anthropogenically disturbed areas, vervet monkeys spend significantly less time socializing and more time engaged in movement [95]. Similarly, toque macaques Macaca sinica in human-modified habitats allocated more time to moving and vigilance and less time to resting [96]. Based on the evidence presented here, we propose that the availability of food resources in open habitats plays a crucial role in shaping the behavior and ecology of CHLs, specifically by increasing feeding and locomotion and decreasing resting and social interactions. Future studies focusing on the relationship between food availability and primate behavior in agroforest landscapes have the potential to generate valuable insights that can contribute to conservation, management, and coexistence strategies. Identifying tree species and landscape features that sustain primate food sources while preserving crop yields can help agroforestry management practices establish a balance between agricultural productivity and biodiversity conservation efforts.

5. Conclusions

Our findings highlight that CHLs allocate more time to feeding and locomotion and less time to socializing in the presence of humans and predatory dogs. This behavioral shift is anticipated as an adaptive strategy aimed at increasing individual fitness in environments where predation risk and human disturbances are prevalent. These behaviors demonstrate their adaptive responses to the landscape of fear, where the mere presence of predators influences their behavior and resource utilization. Overall, our findings provide valuable insights into how CHLs adapt their behavior to deal with threats from humans and predators, ultimately contributing to our understanding of wildlife responses to anthropogenic pressures in shared habitats. In the long run, this adaptation may lead to a shift in CHL habitat use patterns, social dynamics, and foraging behaviors. Therefore, we propose the following practices. 1—Create safe corridors and protected areas where CHLs can access essential resources without facing significant threats from humans or dogs. 2—Conduct comprehensive research to gain deeper insights into CHL social structures, behaviors, and dynamics. 3—Engage local stakeholders in collaborative wildlife management and conflict resolution efforts. By implementing these measures, we can promote coexistence between humans and wildlife while mitigating the long-term impact of human disturbances and predation risk on wildlife populations. Overall, by establishing partnerships with communities, conservation organizations can promote sustainable practices, raise conservation awareness, and empower local communities to take ownership of conservation initiatives.
It is also essential to address the limitations of this study and propose opportunities for future research that could enhance our understanding and inform management practices more effectively. While the study reveals behavioral changes occurring within langurs in response to human and dog presence, it lacks direct evidence regarding the adaptability or long-term benefits of these changes for langurs. Therefore, further investigation comparing the fitness and survival outcomes of langur troops in various habitats (e.g., forest-dwelling vs. semi-urban) is crucial to assess the adaptive nature of these behavioral shifts. Such research could evaluate factors like reproductive success, survival rates, and overall population health to determine whether langurs in urban or semi-urban environments survive better despite the risks associated with human and dog interactions. These approaches would contribute significantly to strengthening management approaches and promoting coexistence.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/biology13060410/s1, Table S1: Classification of open forests within the S Group territory (See Figure 1); Table S2: Classification of dense forests within the S Group Territory (See Figure 1).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.N., V.M. and K.H.G.; methodology, H.N.; software, H.N. and K.H.G.; formal analysis, H.N.; investigation, H.N. and V.M.; resources, H.N. and J.T.; data curation, H.N., V.M. and K.H.G.; writing—original draft preparation, H.N. and K.H.G.; writing—review and editing, H.N., V.M., K.H.G., J.T., D.S. and R.D.; visualization, H.N., V.M. and D.S.; supervision, J.T., D.S. and R.D.; funding acquisition, H.N., V.M. and J.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Rufford Small Grant, grant number 37193-B, and University of Toronto field research funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Our study adhered to Indian wildlife protection laws, complied with the principles for the Ethical Treatment of Non-human Primates set forth by the American Society of Primatologists, and followed the Code of Best Practices for Field Primatology as outlined by the American Society of Primatologists. The entire data collection process was noninvasive, and we conducted our research in close collaboration with Indian Forest authorities and the National Biodiversity Board.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

All data needed to evaluate the conclusions in the paper are present in the paper and/or the Supplementary Materials. The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge Anindya Sinha and the National Institute of Advanced Studies, Bengaluru, India, for their constant support and assistance in acquiring the necessary permits to conduct the study in India. We are grateful to the Forest Department of Uttarakhand for permitting us to complete research in the Kedarnath Wildlife Sanctuary. Lastly, we warmly thank our field assistants, Suraj, Harish, and Mohit, and research assistants, Diganta, Soumalya, Simran, and Ryan, for their unwavering support.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Vanak, A.T.; Gompper, M.E. Dogs Canis Familiaris as Carnivores: Their Role and Function in Intraguild Competition. Mammal Rev. 2009, 39, 265–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Vila, C. Phylogenetic Relationships, Evolution, and Genetic Diversity of the Domestic Dog. J. Hered. 1999, 90, 71–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Gompper, M.E. Free-Ranging Dogs and Wildlife Conservation, 1st ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2014; ISBN 978-0-19-966321-7. [Google Scholar]
  4. Hughes, J.; Macdonald, D.W. A Review of the Interactions between Free-Roaming Domestic Dogs and Wildlife. Biol. Conserv. 2013, 157, 341–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Driscoll, C.A.; Macdonald, D.W. Top Dogs: Wolf Domestication and Wealth. J. Biol. 2010, 9, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Sepúlveda, M.A.; Singer, R.S.; Silva-Rodríguez, E.; Stowhas, P.; Pelican, K. Domestic Dogs in Rural Communities around Protected Areas: Conservation Problem or Conflict Solution? PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e86152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Home, C.; Bhatnagar, Y.V.; Vanak, A.T. Canine Conundrum: Domestic Dogs as an Invasive Species and Their Impacts on Wildlife in India. Anim. Conserv. 2018, 21, 275–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Feldmann, B.M. The Problem of Urban Dogs. Science 1974, 185, 903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Ritchie, E.G.; Dickman, C.R.; Letnic, M.; Vanak, A.T. Dogs as Predators and Trophic Regulators. In Free-Ranging Dogs and Wildlife Conservation; Gompper, M.E., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013; pp. 55–68. ISBN 978-0-19-966321-7. [Google Scholar]
  10. Banks, P.B.; Bryant, J.V. Four-Legged Friend or Foe? Dog Walking Displaces Native Birds from Natural Areas. Biol. Lett. 2007, 3, 611–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Doherty, T.S.; Dickman, C.R.; Glen, A.S.; Newsome, T.M.; Nimmo, D.G.; Ritchie, E.G.; Vanak, A.T.; Wirsing, A.J. The Global Impacts of Domestic Dogs on Threatened Vertebrates. Biol. Conserv. 2017, 210, 56–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Young, J.K.; Olson, K.A.; Reading, R.P.; Amgalanbaatar, S.; Berger, J. Is Wildlife Going to the Dogs? Impacts of Feral and Free-Roaming Dogs on Wildlife Populations. BioScience 2011, 61, 125–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Gompper, M.E. Adding Nuance to Our Understanding of Dog–Wildlife Interactions and the Need for Management. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2021, 61, 93–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Fernández, D.; Kerhoas, D.; Dempsey, A.; Billany, J.; McCabe, G.; Argirova, E. The Current Status of the World’s Primates: Mapping Threats to Understand Priorities for Primate Conservation. Int. J. Primatol. 2022, 43, 15–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Estrada, A.; Garber, P.A.; Chaudhary, A. Current and Future Trends in Socio-Economic, Demographic and Governance Factors Affecting Global Primate Conservation. PeerJ 2020, 8, e9816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Estrada, A.; Garber, P.A.; Rylands, A.B.; Roos, C.; Fernandez-Duque, E.; Di Fiore, A.; Nekaris, K.A.-I.; Nijman, V.; Heymann, E.W.; Lambert, J.E.; et al. Impending Extinction Crisis of the World’s Primates: Why Primates Matter. Sci. Adv. 2017, 3, e1600946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Waters, S.; Watson, T.; Farris, Z.J.; Bornbusch, S.; Valenta, K.; Clarke, T.A.; Chetry, D.; Randriana, Z.; Owen, J.R.; El Harrad, A.; et al. Dogs, Primates, and People: A Review. In Primates in Anthropogenic Landscapes; McKinney, T., Waters, S., Rodrigues, M.A., Eds.; Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2023; pp. 61–81. ISBN 978-3-031-11735-0. [Google Scholar]
  18. Nautiyal, H.; Tanaka, H.; Huffman, M.A. Anti-Predator Strategies of Adult Male Central Himalayan Langurs (Semnopithecus schistaceus) in Response to Domestic Dogs in a Human-Dominated Landscape. Primates 2023, 64, 361–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Pihlström, H.; Rosti, H.; Lombo, B.M.; Pellikka, P. Domestic Dog Predation on White-Tailed Small-Eared Galago (Otolemur garnettii lasiotis) in the Taita Hills, Kenya. Afr. Primates 2021, 15, 31–38. [Google Scholar]
  20. Moresco, A.; Larsen, R.S.; Sauther, M.L.; Cuozzo, F.P.; Jacky, A.Y.; Millette, J.B. Survival of a Wild Ring-Tailed Lemur (Lemur catta) with Abdominal Trauma in an Anthropogenically Disturbed Habitat. Madag. Conserv. Dev. 2012, 7, 49–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Knight, J. Herding Monkeys to Paradise: How Macaque Troops Are Managed for Tourism in Japan. In Herding Monkeys to Paradise; Brill: Leiden, The Nederlands, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  22. Najmuddin, M.F.; Haris, H.; Norazlimi, N.; Md-Zain, M.; Mohd-Ridwan, A.R.; Shahrool-Anuar, R.; Husna, H.A.; Abdul-Latiff, M.A.B. Predation of Domestic Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) on Schlegel’s Banded Langur (Presbytis neglectus) and Crested Hawk-Eagle (Nisaetus cirrhatus) on Dusky Leaf Monkey (Trachypithecus obscurus) in Malaysia. J. Sustain. Sci. Manag. 2019, 14, 39–50. [Google Scholar]
  23. Bishop, N.; Hrdy, S.B.; Teas, J.; Moore, J. Measures of Human Influence in Habitats of South Asian Monkeys. Int. J. Primatol. 1981, 2, 153–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Chakraborty, B.; Pithva, K.; Mohanty, S.; McCowan, B. Lethal Dog Attacks on Adult Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) in an Anthropogenic Landscape. Primates 2024, 65, 151–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Isbell, L.A. Predation on Primates: Ecological Patterns and Evolutionary Consequences. Evol. Anthropol. Issues News Rev. 1994, 3, 61–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hart, D. Predation on Primates: A Biogeographical Analysis. In Primate Anti-Predator Strategies; Gursky, S.L., Nekaris, K.A.I., Eds.; Springer US: Boston, MA, USA, 2007; pp. 27–59. ISBN 978-0-387-34810-0. [Google Scholar]
  27. Campos, F.A.; Fedigan, L.M. Spatial Ecology of Perceived Predation Risk and Vigilance Behavior in White-Faced Capuchins. Behav. Ecol. 2014, 25, 477–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Willems, E.P.; Hill, R.A. Predator-Specific Landscapes of Fear and Resource Distribution: Effects on Spatial Range Use. Ecology 2009, 90, 546–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Cowlishaw, G. Trade-Offs between Foraging and Predation Risk Determine Habitat Use in a Desert Baboon Population. Anim. Behav. 1997, 53, 667–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bidner, L.R. Primates on the menu: Direct and indirect effects of predation on primate communities. Int. J. Primatol. 2014, 35, 1164–1177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Robertson, B.A.; Hutto, R.L. A Framework for Understanding Ecological Traps and an Evaluation of Existing Evidence. Ecology 2006, 87, 1075–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Wong, B.B.M.; Candolin, U. Behavioral Responses to Changing Environments. Behav. Ecol. 2015, 26, 665–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Sih, A.; Ferrari, M.C.O.; Harris, D.J. Evolution and Behavioural Responses to Human-Induced Rapid Environmental Change. Evol. Appl. 2011, 4, 367–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Tuomainen, U.; Candolin, U. Behavioural Responses to Human-Induced Environmental Change. Biol. Rev. 2011, 86, 640–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Bryson-Morrison, N.; Tzanopoulos, J.; Matsuzawa, T.; Humle, T. Activity and Habitat Use of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in the Anthropogenic Landscape of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Int. J. Primatol. 2017, 38, 282–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Negrín, A.R.; Fuentes, A.C.; Espinosa, D.C.; Dias, P.A.D. The Loss of Behavioral Diversity as a Consequence of Anthropogenic Habitat Disturbance: The Social Interactions of Black Howler Monkeys. Primates 2016, 57, 9–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. McKinney, T. A Classification System for Describing Anthropogenic Influence on Nonhuman Primate Populations. Am. J. Primatol. 2015, 77, 715–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Sih, A. Understanding Variation in Behavioural Responses to Human-Induced Rapid Environmental Change: A Conceptual Overview. Anim. Behav. 2013, 85, 1077–1088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Ehrlich, P.R. Intervening in Evolution: Ethics and Actions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 5477–5480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Nautiyal, H. Behavioral Ecology of the Central Himalayan Langur (Semnopithecus schistaceus) in a Human Dominated Landscape: Multi-Species Interactions and Conservation Implications. Ph.D. Thesis, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  41. Nautiyal, H.; Mathur, V.; Sinha, A.; Huffman, M.A. The Banj Oak Quercus leucotrichophora as a Potential Mitigating Factor for Human-Langur Interactions in the Garhwal Himalayas, India: People’s Perceptions and Ecological Importance. Glob. Ecol. Conserv. 2020, 22, e00985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Gajare, K.H. Interaction between Feral Dogs and Central Himalayan Langur (Semnopithecus schistaceus); Mumbai University: Mumbai, India, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  43. Mathur, V.; Teichroeb, J.; Nautiyal, H. Understanding Variability in Crop Foraging by Himalayan Langurs (Semnopithecus schistacesus) and Other Wildlife Species Using Direct Observation and Camera Traps in An Anthropogenic Landscape. In Proceedings of the American Socity of Primatology Conference, Reno, NV, USA, 21 June 2023. [Google Scholar]
  44. Theuerkauf, J.; Rouys, S. Habitat Selection by Ungulates in Relation to Predation Risk by Wolves and Humans in the Białowieża Forest, Poland. For. Ecol. Manag. 2008, 256, 1325–1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Nowak, K.; Wimberger, K.; Richards, S.A.; Hill, R.A.; le Roux, A. Samango Monkeys (Cercopithecus albogularis labiatus) Manage Risk in a Highly Seasonal, Human-Modified Landscape in Amathole Mountains, South Africa. Int. J. Primatol. 2017, 38, 194–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Candolin, U.; Wong, B.B.M. Behavioural Responses to a Changing World: Mechanisms and Consequences; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012; ISBN 978-0-19-960256-8. [Google Scholar]
  47. Mori, E. Porcupines in the Landscape of Fear: Effect of Hunting with Dogs on the Behaviour of a Non-Target Species. Mammal Res. 2017, 62, 251–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Atickem, A.; Bekele, A.; Williams, S.D. Competition between Domestic Dogs and Ethiopian Wolf (Canis simensis) in the Bale Mountains National Park, Ethiopia. Afr. J. Ecol. 2010, 48, 401–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Nautiyal, H. Life in the Extreme: Time-Activity Budgets and Foraging Ecology of Central Himalayan Langur (Semnopithecus schistaceus) in the Kedarnath Wildlife Sanctuary, Uttarakhand India. Master’s Thesis, Bharathidasan University, Tiruchirappalli, India, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  50. Mathur, V. Choice and Characteristics of Sleeping Sites in a Troop of Central Himalayan Langurs (Semnopithecus schistaceus). Ph.D. Thesis, Indian Institute of Science Education and Research Mohali, Mohali, India, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  51. Altmann, J. Observational Study of Behavior: Sampling Methods. Behaviour 1974, 49, 227–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Watkins, M.W.; Pacheco, M. Interobserver Agreement in Behavioral Research: Importance and Calculation. J. Behav. Educ. 2000, 10, 205–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Dolhinow, P. A Behavior Repertoire for the Indian Langur Monkey (Presbytis entellus). Primates 1978, 19, 449–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Venables, W.; Ripley, B. Modern Applied Statistics with S; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  55. Ripley, B.; Venables, W. Nnet: Feed-Forward Neural Networks and Multinomial Log-Linear Models 2023. Available online: http://www.stats.ox.ac.uk/pub/MASS4/ (accessed on 3 February 2024).
  56. R A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 2022. Available online: http://www.r-project.org/ (accessed on 5 February 2024).
  57. Long, J.S.; Freese, J. Regression Models for Categorical Dependent Variables Using Stata, 2nd ed.; Stata Press: College Station, TX, USA, 2006; ISBN 978-1-59718-011-5. [Google Scholar]
  58. Davies, G.; Oates, J. Colobine Monkeys: Their Ecology, Behaviour and Evolution; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1994; ISBN 978-0-521-33153-1. [Google Scholar]
  59. Agresti, A. Categorical Data Analysis; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012; ISBN 978-0-470-46363-5. [Google Scholar]
  60. Zuur, A.F.; Ieno, E.N.; Walker, N.; Saveliev, A.A.; Smith, G.M. Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R; Statistics for Biology and Health; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2009; ISBN 978-0-387-87457-9. [Google Scholar]
  61. Cohen, J.; Cohen, P.; West, S.G.; Aiken, L.S. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 3rd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2002; ISBN 978-0-203-77444-1. [Google Scholar]
  62. Gerber, E.A.E.; Craig, B.A. Residuals and Diagnostics for Multinomial Regression Models. Stat. Anal. Data Min. ASA Data Sci. J. 2024, 17, e11645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Goeman, J.J.; le Cessie, S. A Goodness-of-Fit Test for Multinomial Logistic Regression. Biometrics 2006, 62, 980–985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Anderson, C.J.; Rutkowski, L. Multinomial Logistic Regression. In Best Practices in Quantitative Methods; Osborne, J., Ed.; SAGE Publishing: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2008; pp. 390–409. ISBN 978-1-4129-4065-8. [Google Scholar]
  65. Burnham, K.P.; Anderson, D.R. (Eds.) Advanced Issues and Deeper Insights. In Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 267–351. ISBN 978-0-387-22456-5. [Google Scholar]
  66. Burnham, K.P.; Anderson, D.R.; Huyvaert, K.P. AIC Model Selection and Multimodel Inference in Behavioral Ecology: Some Background, Observations, and Comparisons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011, 65, 23–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Symonds, M.R.E.; Moussalli, A. A Brief Guide to Model Selection, Multimodel Inference and Model Averaging in Behavioural Ecology Using Akaike’s Information Criterion. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011, 65, 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Richards, S.A.; Whittingham, M.J.; Stephens, P.A. Model Selection and Model Averaging in Behavioural Ecology: The Utility of the IT-AIC Framework. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2011, 65, 77–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Mazerolle, M.J. AICcmodavg: Model Selection and Multimodel Inference Based on (Q)AIC(c). R Package Version 2.3.3. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=AICcmodavg (accessed on 15 January 2024).
  70. Rushton, S.P.; Ormerod, S.J.; Kerby, G. New Paradigms for Modelling Species Distributions? J. Appl. Ecol. 2004, 41, 193–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Whittingham, M.J.; Stephens, P.A.; Bradbury, R.B.; Freckleton, R.P. Why Do We Still Use Stepwise Modelling in Ecology and Behaviour? J. Anim. Ecol. 2006, 75, 1182–1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Johnson, J.B.; Omland, K.S. Model Selection in Ecology and Evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2004, 19, 101–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Duboscq, J.; Romano, V.; Sueur, C.; MacIntosh, A.J.J. Scratch That Itch: Revisiting Links between Self-Directed Behaviour and Parasitological, Social and Environmental Factors in a Free-Ranging Primate. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2016, 3, 160571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Mercier, S.; Neumann, C.; van de Waal, E.; Chollet, E.; Meric de Bellefon, J.; Zuberbühler, K. Vervet Monkeys Greet Adult Males during High-Risk Situations. Anim. Behav. 2017, 132, 229–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Lonsdorf, E.V.; Wilson, M.L.; Boehm, E.; Delaney-Soesman, J.; Grebey, T.; Murray, C.; Wellens, K.; Pusey, A.E. Why Chimpanzees Carry Dead Infants: An Empirical Assessment of Existing Hypotheses. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2020, 7, 200931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Rangel-Negrín, A.; Coyohua-Fuentes, A.; Chavira, R.; Canales-Espinosa, D.; Dias, P.A.D. Primates Living Outside Protected Habitats Are More Stressed: The Case of Black Howler Monkeys in the Yucatán Peninsula. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e112329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Galbany, J.; Muhire, T.; Vecellio, V.; Mudakikwa, A.; Nyiramana, A.; Cranfield, M.R.; Stoinski, T.S.; McFarlin, S.C. Incisor Tooth Wear and Age Determination in Mountain Gorillas from Volcanoes National Park, Rwanda. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2018, 167, 930–935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Kraus, J.B.; Strier, K.B. Geographic, Climatic, and Phylogenetic Drivers of Variation in Colobine Activity Budgets. Primates 2022, 63, 647–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Khater, M.; Murariu, D.; Gras, R. Predation Risk Tradeoffs in Prey: Effects on Energy and Behaviour. Theor. Ecol. 2016, 9, 251–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Brown, J.S.; Laundré, J.W.; Gurung, M. The Ecology of Fear: Optimal Foraging, Game Theory, and Trophic Interactions. J. Mammal. 1999, 80, 385–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Gaynor, K.M.; Brown, J.S.; Middleton, A.D.; Power, M.E.; Brashares, J.S. Landscapes of Fear: Spatial Patterns of Risk Perception and Response. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2019, 34, 355–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Kotler, B.P.; Brown, J.S.; Hasson, O. Factors Affecting Gerbil Foraging Behavior and Rates of Owl Predation. Ecology 1991, 72, 2249–2260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Lind, J.; Cresswell, W. Determining the Fitness Consequences of Antipredation Behavior. Behav. Ecol. 2005, 16, 945–956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Trussell, G.C.; Matassa, C.M.; Luttbeg, B. The Effects of Variable Predation Risk on Foraging and Growth: Less Risk Is Not Necessarily Better. Ecology 2011, 92, 1799–1806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Wisenden, B.D. Female Convict Cichlids Adjust Gonadal Investment in Current Reproduction in Response to Relative Risk of Brood Predation. Can. J. Zool. 1993, 71, 252–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Sharma, C.M.; Gairola, S.; Ghildiyal, S.K.; Suyal, S. Forest Resource Use Patterns in Relation to Socioeconomic Status. Mt. Res. Dev. 2009, 29, 308–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Makino, Y. Lopping of Oaks in Central Himalaya, India. Mt. Res. Dev. 2011, 31, 35–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Valeix, M.; Loveridge, A.J.; Chamaillé-Jammes, S.; Davidson, Z.; Murindagomo, F.; Fritz, H.; Macdonald, D.W. Behavioral Adjustments of African Herbivores to Predation Risk by Lions: Spatiotemporal Variations Influence Habitat Use. Ecology 2009, 90, 23–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Ciuti, S.; Northrup, J.M.; Muhly, T.B.; Simi, S.; Musiani, M.; Pitt, J.A.; Boyce, M.S. Effects of Humans on Behaviour of Wildlife Exceed Those of Natural Predators in a Landscape of Fear. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e50611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Tadesse, S.A.; Kotler, B.P. Impact of Tourism on Nubian Ibex (Capra nubiana) Revealed through Assessment of Behavioral Indicators. Behav. Ecol. 2012, 23, 1257–1262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Mahato, S.; Dasgupta, S.; Todaria, N.P.; Singh, V.P. Agroforestry Mapping and Characterization in Four Districts of Garhwal Himalaya. Energy Ecol. Environ. 2016, 1, 86–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Bersacola, E.; Hill, C.M.; Hockings, K.J. Chimpanzees Balance Resources and Risk in an Anthropogenic Landscape of Fear. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 4569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. McLennan, M.R.; Hill, C.M. Troublesome Neighbours: Changing Attitudes towards Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in a Human-Dominated Landscape in Uganda. J. Nat. Conserv. 2012, 20, 219–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Huang, Z.; Yuan, P.; Huang, H.; Tang, X.; Xu, W.; Huang, C.; Zhou, Q. Effect of Habitat Fragmentation on Ranging Behavior of White-Headed Langurs in Limestone Forests in Southwest China. Primates 2017, 58, 423–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Thatcher, H.R.; Downs, C.T.; Koyama, N.F. Anthropogenic Influences on the Time Budgets of Urban Vervet Monkeys. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2019, 181, 38–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Jayapali, U.; Perera, P.; Cresswell, J.; Dayawansa, N. Does Anthropogenic Influence on Habitats Alter the Activity Budget and Home Range Size of Toque Macaques (Macaca sinica)? Insight into the Human-Macaque Conflict. Trees For. People 2023, 13, 100412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. A study area map illustrating the spatial distribution of forested areas, villages, and agricultural land within the habitat of the S Group.
Figure 1. A study area map illustrating the spatial distribution of forested areas, villages, and agricultural land within the habitat of the S Group.
Biology 13 00410 g001
Figure 2. Predictor variables of (a) presence of dogs, (b) presence of humans, and (c) habitat type in relation to activity patterns.
Figure 2. Predictor variables of (a) presence of dogs, (b) presence of humans, and (c) habitat type in relation to activity patterns.
Biology 13 00410 g002
Table 1. Model selection based on AIC.
Table 1. Model selection based on AIC.
MLR ModelsKAICΔAICWeight
PD + PH + H-OF + (1|Month)24154,160.801
PD + PH + PD*H-OF + PD*H-OF
+ H-OF (1|Month)
18154,232.671.760
PH + H-OF + (1|Month)9154,564.3403.520
PD + H-OF + (1|Month)9154,569.3408.460
H-OF + (1|Month)6154,959.4798.540
PD + PH + (1|Month)9155,579.51418.680
PD + (1|Month)6155,833.31672.460
PH + (1|Month)6155,888.21727.330
MLR~1 + (1|Month)3156,371.42210.530
AIC: The AIC value for each model; ΔAIC: difference in the AIC between the model with the lowest AIC and the following one; H-OF: habitat type—open forest; K: number of free parameters in the model; PD: presence of dog; PH: presence of human; Weight: model probabilities; *: interaction term.
Table 2. Results from a multinomial logistic regression model.
Table 2. Results from a multinomial logistic regression model.
Predictor VariablePresence of Dogs Presence of HumansHabitat—Open Forest
Feedingz value0.60111.25525.641
p value<0.01<0.01<0.01
*Coef (SE)0.016 (0.027)0.288 (0.026)0.585 (0.023)
Locomotionz value6.1358.1224.717
p value<0.01<0.01<0.01
Coef (SE)0.156 (0.025)0.202 (0.025)0.106 (0.022)
Socialz value−11.963−5.407−17.872
p value<0.01<0.01<0.01
Coef (SE)−0.386 (0.032)−0.163 (0.030)−0.492 (0.028)
*Coef (SE): Regression coefficient (standard error).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Nautiyal, H.; Mathur, V.; Gajare, K.H.; Teichroeb, J.; Sarkar, D.; Diogo, R. Predatory Dogs as Drivers of Social Behavior Changes in the Central Himalayan Langur (Semnopithecus schistaceus) in Agro-Forest Landscapes. Biology 2024, 13, 410. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology13060410

AMA Style

Nautiyal H, Mathur V, Gajare KH, Teichroeb J, Sarkar D, Diogo R. Predatory Dogs as Drivers of Social Behavior Changes in the Central Himalayan Langur (Semnopithecus schistaceus) in Agro-Forest Landscapes. Biology. 2024; 13(6):410. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology13060410

Chicago/Turabian Style

Nautiyal, Himani, Virendra Mathur, Kimaya Hemant Gajare, Julie Teichroeb, Dipto Sarkar, and Rui Diogo. 2024. "Predatory Dogs as Drivers of Social Behavior Changes in the Central Himalayan Langur (Semnopithecus schistaceus) in Agro-Forest Landscapes" Biology 13, no. 6: 410. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology13060410

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop