The Effect of Message Framing on Preschoolers’ Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions: Influences of Age, Issue Involvement, and Delivery Method
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Effect of Age on Message Framing
1.2. The Effect of Issue Involvement on Message Framing
1.3. The Effect of Delivery Method on Message Framing
1.4. Purpose of the Study
- Are there main effects of each variable on the children’s attitudes and behavioral intentions toward persuasive messages?
- Are there interaction effects between variables for children’s attitudes and behavioral intentions toward persuasive messages?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
2.2.1. Persuasive Messages
2.2.2. Picture Cards
2.2.3. Attitudes and Behavioral Intention Questionnaire
2.3. Procedure
2.3.1. Preliminary Study
2.3.2. Main Study
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Effects of Age, Issue Involvement, Delivery Method, and Frame Type on Children’s Perception of Importance of Messages (Attitude)
3.2. Effects of Age, Issue Involvement, Delivery Method, and Frame Type on Children’s Tendency to Share Messages (Attitude)
3.3. Effects of Age, Issue Involvement, Delivery Method, and Frame Type on Children’s Behavioral Intention
3.4. Children’s Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions by Age, Issue Involvement, Delivery Method, and Frame Type
4. Discussion
4.1. Effects of Children’s Age on Their Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions
4.2. Effects of Issue Involvement on Children’s Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions
4.3. Effects of Delivery Method on Children’s Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions
4.4. Comprehensive Discussion of Effective Message Delivery for Children
4.5. Limitations and Suggestions for Future Researches
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 1979, 47, 263–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 1981, 211, 453–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abhyankar, P.; O’Connor, D.B.; Lawton, R. The role of message framing in promoting MMR vaccination: Evidence of a loss-frame advantage. Psychol. Health Med. 2008, 13, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Donovan, R.J.; Jalleh, G. Positive versus negative framing of a hypothetical infant immunization: The influence of involvement. Health Educ. Behav. 2000, 27, 82–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gallagher, K.M.; Updegraff, J.A. Health message framing effects on attitudes, intentions, and behavior: A meta-analytic review. Ann. Behav. Med. 2012, 4, 101–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Avineri, E.; Waygood, E.O.D. Applying valence framing to enhance the effect of information on transport-related carbon dioxide emissions. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2013, 48, 31–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Purewal, S.; van den Akker, O.B.A. A study of the effect of message framing on oocyte donation. Hum. Reprod. 2009, 24, 3136–3143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ganzach, Y.; Karsahi, N. Message framing and buying behavior: A field experiment. J. Bus. Res. 1995, 32, 11–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keller, P.A.; Limpkus, I.M.; Rimer, B.K. Affect, framing, and persuasion. J. Mark. Res. 2003, 40, 54–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerend, M.A.; Sias, T. Message framing and color priming: How subtle threat cues affect persuasion. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 45, 999–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyerowitz, B.E.; Chaiken, S. The effect of message framing on breast self-examination attitudes, intentions, and behavior. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 52, 500–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cherubini, P.; Rumiati, R.; Rossi, D.; Nigro, F.; Calabrò, A. Improving attitudes toward prostate examinations by loss-framed appeals. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 35, 732–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Detweiler, J.B.; Bedell, B.T.; Salovey, P.; Pronin, E.; Rothman, A.J. Message framing and sunscreen use: Gain-framed messages motivate beach-goers. Health Psychol. 1999, 18, 189–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rothman, A.J.; Salovey, P. Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: The role of message framing. Psychol. Bull. 1997, 121, 3–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hull, S.J. Perceived risk as a moderator of the effectiveness of framed HIV-test promotion messages among women: A randomized controlled trial. Health Psychol. 2012, 31, 114–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Venkatraman, S.; Aloysius, J.A.; Davis, F.D. Multiple prospect framing and decision behavior: The mediational roles of perceived riskiness and perceived ambiguity. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Processes 2006, 101, 59–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerend, M.A.; Shepherd, J.E. Using message framing to promote acceptance of the human papillomavirous vaccine. Health Psychol. 2007, 26, 745–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maheswaran, D.; Meyers-Levy, J. The influence of message framing and issue involvement. J. Mark. Res. 1990, 27, 361–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petty, R.E.; Cacioppo, J.T. The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 19, 123–205. [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Retamero, R.; Cokely, E.T. Effective communication of risks to young adults: Using message framing and visual aids to increase condom use and STD screening. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2011, 17, 270–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- White, V.; Webster, B.; Wakefield, M. Do graphic health warning labels have an impact on adolescents’ smoking-related beliefs and behaviours? Addiction 2008, 103, 1562–1571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Berko, J. The child’s learning of English morphology. Word 1958, 14, 50–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garon, N.; Bryson, S.E.; Smith, I.M. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 134, 31–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Guy, J.; Rogers, M.; Cornish, K. Developmental changes in visual and auditory inhibition in early childhood. Infant Child Dev. 2012, 21, 521–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hwang, M.H. A Longitudinal Study of a Korean Child’s Language Development: From 30 to 41 Months. Master’s Thesis, Dankook University, Yongin-si, Korea, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska, G.; Coy, K.C.; Murray, K.T. The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 1091–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kurdek, L.A. Perspective taking as the cognitive basis of children’s moral development: A review of the literature. Merrill-Palmer Q. Behav. Dev. 1978, 24, 3–28. [Google Scholar]
- Selman, R.L. The relation of role taking to the development of moral judgment in children. Child Dev. 1971, 42, 79–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simmering, V.R. The development of visual working memory capacity during early childhood. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2012, 111, 695–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vazsonyi, A.T.; Huang, L. Where self-control comes from: On the development of self-control and its relationship to deviance over time. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Danis, A.; Pêcheux, M.G.; Lefèvre, C.; Bourdais, C.; Serres-Ruel, J. A continuous performance task in preschool children: Relations between attention and performance. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 5, 401–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruff, H.A. Components of attention during infants’ manipulative exploration. Child Dev. 1986, 57, 105–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cheon, C.H. The Influence of Child’s Development of Theory of Mind and Social Desirability on Child’s Prosocial Behavior. Master’s Thesis, Sungkyunkwan University, Seoul, Korea, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Chung, J. A Developmental Lag between Children’s Understanding of First-Order and Second-Order Mental States. Korean J. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 26, 97–118. [Google Scholar]
- Hongwanishkul, D.; Happaney, K.R.; Lee, W.S.; Zelazo, P.D. Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2005, 28, 617–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rothbart, M.K.; Ellis, L.K.; Rosario Rueda, M.; Posner, M.I. Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. J. Personal. 2003, 71, 1113–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bindman, S.W.; Hindman, A.H.; Bowles, R.P.; Morrison, F.J. The contributions of parental management language to executive function in preschool children. Early Child. Res. Q. 2013, 28, 529–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kopp, C.B. Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Dev. Psychol. 1982, 18, 199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kohlberg, L. Stage and sequence: The cognitive developmental approach to socialization. In Handbook of Socialization on Theory and Research; Goslin, D., Ed.; Rand McNally: Chicago, IL, USA, 1969; pp. 347–480. [Google Scholar]
- Kohlberg, L. The cognitive-developmental approach to moral education. Phi Delta Kappan 1975, 56, 670–677. [Google Scholar]
- Walker, L.J.; Taylor, J.H. Family interactions and the development of moral reasoning. Child Dev. 1991, 62, 264–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seed, A.M.; Dickerson, K.L. Future thinking: Children but not apes consider multiple possibilities. Curr. Biol. 2016, 26, R525–R527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Seiver, E.; Gopnik, A.; Goodman, N.D. Did she jump because she was the big sister or because the trampoline was safe? Causal inference and the development of social attribution. Child Dev. 2013, 84, 443–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tecwyn, E.C.; Thorpe, S.K.S.; Chappell, J. Development of planning in 4- to 10-year-old children: Reducing inhibitory demands does not improve performance. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 125, 85–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Witelson, S.F. Neurobiological aspects of language in children. Child Dev. 1987, 58, 653–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kim, S.P.; Kim, H.J. The impact of message framing and theme type on public campaign advertising: Focused on elementary school student. J. Public Relat. Res. 2013, 17, 5–39. [Google Scholar]
- Improgo, L.V.F.; Inguito, J.S.; Ingusan, D.R.; Ingusan, D.R.; Jalandoni, J.A.J.; Jarabelo, L.E.; Empaynado-Porto, A. Loss versus gain: Integrating technology and message framing in promoting proper hand washing among grade 1 pupils. Int. J. Public Health Res. 2011, 2001, 103–114. [Google Scholar]
- Bannon, K.; Schwartz, M.B. Impact of nutrition messages on children’s food choice: Pilot study. Appetite 2006, 46, 124–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moorman, M.; van den Putte, B. The influence of message framing, intention to quit smoking, and nicotine dependence on the persuasiveness of smoking cessation messages. Addict. Behav. 2008, 33, 1267–1275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rothman, A.J.; Salovey, P.; Antone, C.; Keough, K.; Martin, C.D. The influence of message framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 29, 408–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fagerlin, A.; Wang, C.; Ubel, P.A. Reducing the influence of anecdotal reasoning on people’s health care decisions: Is a picture worth a thousand statistics? Med. Decis. Mak. 2005, 25, 398–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lipkus, I.M. Numeric, verbal, and visual formats of conveying health risks: Suggested best practices and future recommendations. Med. Decis. Mak. 2007, 27, 696–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Evans, A.T.; Peters, E.; Strasser, A.A.; Emery, L.F.; Sheerin, K.M.; Romer, D. Graphic warning labels elicit affective and thoughtful responses from smokers: Results of a randomized clinical trial. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0142879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Hegarty, M.; Pederson, L.L.; Nelson, D.E.; Mowery, P.; Gable, J.M.; Wortley, P. Reactions of young adult smokers to warning labels on cigarette packages. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2006, 30, 467–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hammond, D.; Fong, G.T.; McDonald, P.W.; Brown, K.S.; Cameron, R. Graphic Canadian cigarette warning labels and adverse outcomes: Evidence from Canadian smokers. Am. J. Public Health 2004, 94, 1442–1445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hammond, D.; Fong, G.T.; McDonald, P.W.; Cameron, R.; Brown, K.S. Impact of the graphic Canadian warning labels on adult smoking behavior. Tob. Control 2003, 12, 391–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Seo, K.; Dillard, J.P.; Shen, F. The effects of message framing and visual image on persuasion. Commun. Q. 2013, 61, 564–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cokely, E.T.; Kelley, C.M. Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: A protocol analysis and process model evaluation. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2009, 4, 20–33. [Google Scholar]
- Cho, Y.-S.; Hwang, J.-S. The effectiveness of PSA (Public Service Advertising) with the types of issues: Influence of the characteristics of messages and audiences on social v. individual issues. Korean J. Advert. Public Relat. 2007, 9, 71–104. [Google Scholar]
- O’Keefe, D.J.; Jensen, J.D. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed loss-framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: A meta-analytic review. J. Health Commun. 2007, 12, 623–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pakpour, A.H.; Yekaninejad, M.S.; Sniehotta, F.F.; Updegraff, J.A.; Dombrowski, S.U. The effectiveness of gain- versus loss-framed health messages in improving oral health in Iranian secondary schools: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. Ann. Behav. Med. 2014, 47, 376–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruff, H.A.; Lawson, K.R. Development of sustained, focused attention in young children during free play. Dev. Psychol. 1990, 26, 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Social Learning Theory; Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Ahn, S. The Teacher-Child Interaction and the Child-Peer Interaction in Free Play Time. Master’s Thesis, Ewha Womans University, Seoul, Korea, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, Y.-N. Affective Perspective Taking Ability and Peer Interaction: Their Tendency and Relationship in Preschoolers 4–5 Years-Old. Master’s Thesis, Ewha Womans University, Seoul, Korea, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, G.E. Framing in advertising and the moderating impact of consumer education. J. Advert. Res. 1996, 36, 49–64. [Google Scholar]
- Rose, S.A.; Feldman, J.F.; Jankowski, J.J.; Van Rossem, R. Information processing from infancy to 11 years: Continuities and prediction of IQ. Intelligence 2012, 40, 445–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mayer, R.E.; Heiser, J.; Lonn, S. Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more material results in less understanding. J. Educ. Psychol. 2001, 93, 187–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paivio, A. Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson, D.; Reed, V.; Walling, J. Pictorial superiority effect. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem. 1976, 2, 523–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slovic, P.; Finucane, M.; Peters, E.; MacGregor, D.G. Rational actors or rational fools: Implications of the affect heuristic for behavioral economics. J. Socio-Econ. 2002, 31, 329–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peters, E. The functions of affect in the construction of preferences. In The Construction of Preference; Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2006; pp. 454–463. [Google Scholar]
- Peters, E.; Lipkus, I.; Diefenbach, M.A. The functions of affect in health communications and in the construction of health preferences. J. Commun. 2006, 56, S140–S162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Block, L.G.; Keller, P.A. When to accentuate the negative: The effects of perceived efficacy and message framing on intentions to perform a health-related behavior. J. Mark. Res. 1995, 32, 192–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Issue | Message | Gain-Framed | Loss-Framed |
---|---|---|---|
Safety | Delivery | You should wear protective gear when riding a bike or kick scooter outside. | |
Reason | Because this protects the precious parts of your body, and you do not get hurt a lot even if you fall or hit something. | Because not wearing protective gear may hurt you a lot if you fall or hit something. Then you will bleed and it will hurt a lot, and you will have to go to the hospital. | |
Environment | Delivery | We should use only the right amount of soap and shampoo when bathing. | |
Reason | Because if we do so, the soap and shampoo will be naturally gone in the river (dissolved), so fish can live in clean water. | Because if we use a lot of soap or shampoo, it stays in the river (not dissolved). This pollutes the river and kills all the fish living in the river. |
Title 1 | 3 Years Old (n = 58) | 4 Years Old (n = 58) | 5 Years Old (n = 57) | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions in Gain/Loss Condition | High inv. | Low inv. | High inv. | Low inv. | High inv. | Low inv. | |||||||
Verb. M (SD) | Verb. + Pic. M (SD) | Verb. M (SD) | Verb. + Pic. M (SD) | Verb. M(SD) | Verb. + Pic. M (SD) | Verb. M (SD) | Verb. + Pic. M (SD) | Verb. M (SD) | Verb. + Pic. M (SD) | Verb. M (SD) | Verb. + Pic. M (SD) | ||
Perception of importance (attitude) | Gain | 4.27 (2.17) | 3.64 (2.17) | 4.53 (1.60) | 4.64 (2.10) | 5.27 (1.87) | 4.40 (2.29) | 5.40 (2.03) | 4.33 (2.26) | 5.29 (2.20) | 5.67 (0.90) | 5.43 (1.79) | 5.53 (1.81) |
3.97 (2.16) | 4.59 (1.82) | 4.83 (2.10) | 4.87 (2.18) | 5.48 (1.64) | 5.48 (1.77) | ||||||||
Loss | 3.79 (2.69) | 4.13 (2.26) | 2.50 (1.65) | 3.80 (2.46) | 4.53 (2.07) | 6.00 (1.41) | 4.27 (2.15) | 5.31 (2.10) | 5.77 (1.59) | 5.67 (1.84) | 5.92 (1.50) | 5.80 (2.08) | |
3.97 (2.44) | 3.17 (2.17) | 5.21 (1.91) | 4.75 (2.15) | 5.71 (1.70) | 5.86 (1.80) | ||||||||
Tendency to share the message (attitude) | Gain | 4.07 (2.12) | 4.86 (2.14) | 5.00 (2.07) | 4.57 (2.28) | 5.27 (2.05) | 3.80 (2.54) | 4.00 (2.10) | 4.07 (2.46) | 3.93 (2.53) | 4.87 (1.73) | 5.29 (1.86) | 5.60 (1.64) |
4.45 (2.13) | 4.79 (2.14) | 4.53 (2.39) | 4.03 (2.25) | 4.41 (2.16) | 5.45 (1.72) | ||||||||
Loss | 3.71 (2.40) | 4.47 (2.13) | 4.43 (2.34) | 3.87 (2.23) | 4.27 (2.37) | 6.31 (1.11) | 4.73 (2.22) | 4.69 (2.36) | 3.77 (2.31) | 5.87 (1.51) | 4.31 (2.25) | 6.00 (1.51) | |
4.10 (2.26) | 4.14 (2.26) | 5.21 (2.13) | 4.71 (2.24) | 4.89 (2.17) | 5.21 (2.04) | ||||||||
Behavioral intention | Gain | 4.47 (2.26) | 4.07 (2.30) | 3.60 (2.26) | 3.14 (2.41) | 5.73 (2.46) | 4.47 (2.59) | 3.13 (2.00) | 2.80 (2.11) | 6.14 (1.83) | 5.87 (1.51) | 3.93 (2.40) | 3.67 (2.02) |
4.28 (2.25) | 3.38 (2.31) | 5.10 (2.56) | 2.97 (2.03) | 6.00 (1.65) | 3.79 (2.18) | ||||||||
Loss | 3.43 (2.50) | 4.87 (2.56) | 2.29 (2.09) | 3.20 (2.43) | 5.33 (2.32) | 6.38 (0.96) | 2.73 (1.94) | 2.92 (2.25) | 5.54 (1.71) | 6.40 (1.59) | 1.92 (0.95) | 3.53 (2.53) | |
4.17 (2.59) | 2.76 (2.28) | 5.82 (1.87) | 2.82 (2.06) | 6.00 (1.68) | 2.79 (2.10) |
SS | Df | MS | F | ƞp2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (A) | 172.21 | 2 | 86.11 | 18.10 *** | 0.18 |
Issue involvement (B) | 0.54 | 1 | 0.54 | 0.17 | |
Delivery method (C) | 2.32 | 1 | 2.32 | 0.49 | |
Frame type (D) | 0.50 | 1 | 0.50 | 0.11 | |
A * B | 1.27 | 2 | 0.63 | 0.20 | |
A * C | 0.70 | 2 | 0.35 | 0.07 | |
A * D | 17.99 | 2 | 9.00 | 1.89 | |
B * C | 0.35 | 1 | 0.35 | 0.11 | |
B * D | 7.91 | 1 | 7.91 | 2.45 | |
C * D | 20.79 | 1 | 20.79 | 4.37 * | 0.03 |
A * B * C | 5.65 | 2 | 2.82 | 0.87 | |
A * B * D | 9.07 | 2 | 4.54 | 1.40 | |
A * C * D | 23.85 | 2 | 11.93 | 2.51 | |
B * C * D | 0.04 | 1 | 0.04 | 0.01 | |
A * B * C * D | 0.26 | 2 | 0.13 | 0.04 | |
Error (B) | 520.18 | 161 | 3.23 | ||
Error (between variables) | 766.12 | 161 | 4.76 |
SS | df | MS | F | ƞp2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (A) | 19.43 | 2 | 9.72 | 1.80 | |
Issue involvement (B) | 1.13 | 1 | 1.13 | 0.31 | |
Delivery method (C) | 22.98 | 1 | 22.98 | 4.27 * | 0.03 |
Frame type (D) | 0.74 | 1 | 0.74 | 0.14 | |
A * B | 21.86 | 2 | 10.93 | 3.01 (p = 0.052) | 0.04 |
A * C | 23.75 | 2 | 11.87 | 2.20 | |
A * D | 21.61 | 2 | 10.80 | 2.01 | |
B * C | 10.12 | 1 | 10.12 | 2.79 | |
B * D | 2.64 | 1 | 2.64 | 0.73 | |
C * D | 19.91 | 1 | 19.91 | 3.70 (p = 0.056) | 0.02 |
A * B * C | 3.87 | 2 | 1.94 | 0.53 | |
A * B * D | 1.52 | 2 | 0.76 | 0.21 | |
A * C * D | 12.55 | 2 | 6.28 | 1.17 | |
B * C * D | 7.30 | 1 | 7.30 | 2.01 | |
A * B * C * D | 16.31 | 2 | 8.16 | 2.25 | |
Error (B) | 584.02 | 161 | 3.63 | ||
Error (between variables) | 867.41 | 161 | 5.39 |
SS | df | MS | F | ƞp2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (A) | 56.79 | 2 | 28.40 | 5.58 ** | 0.07 |
Issue involvement (B) | 399.59 | 1 | 399.59 | 97.84 *** | 0.38 |
Delivery method (C) | 5.66 | 1 | 5.66 | 1.11 | |
Frame type (D) | 3.65 | 1 | 3.65 | 0.72 | |
A * B | 43.65 | 2 | 21.83 | 5.34 ** | 0.06 |
A * C | 5.33 | 2 | 2.67 | 0.52 | |
A * D | 11.94 | 2 | 5.97 | 1.17 | |
B * C | 0.04 | 1 | 0.04 | 0.01 | |
B * D | 14.25 | 1 | 14.25 | 3.49 (p = 0.064) | 0.02 |
C * D | 49.12 | 1 | 49.12 | 9.65 ** | 0.06 |
A * B * C | 1.63 | 2 | 0.82 | 0.20 | |
A * B * D | 1.08 | 2 | 0.54 | 0.13 | |
A * C * D | 0.12 | 2 | 0.06 | 0.01 | |
B * C * D | 1.39 | 1 | 1.39 | 0.34 | |
A * B * C * D | 5.73 | 2 | 2.87 | 0.70 | |
Error (B) | 657.52 | 161 | 4.08 | ||
Error (between variables) | 819.14 | 161 | 5.09 |
SS | df | MS | F | ƞp2 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (A) | 69.61 | 2 | 34.803 | 13.46 *** | 0.14 |
Issue involvement (B) | 42.94 | 1 | 42.94 | 28.36 *** | 0.15 |
Delivery method (C) | 8.40 | 1 | 8.40 | 3.25 | |
Frame type (D) | 0.34 | 1 | 0.34 | 0.13 | |
A * B | 8.57 | 2 | 4.29 | 2.83 (p = 0.062) | |
A * C | 4.19 | 2 | 2.09 | 0.81 | |
A * D | 12.60 | 2 | 6.30 | 2.44 | |
B * C | 0.64 | 1 | 0.64 | 0.42 | |
B * D | 7.49 | 1 | 7.49 | 4.95 * | 0.03 |
C * D | 28.55 | 1 | 28.55 | 11.04 ** | 0.06 |
A * B * C | 0.08 | 2 | 0.04 | 0.03 | |
A * B * D | 0.25 | 2 | 0.12 | 0.08 | |
A * C * D | 4.29 | 2 | 2.15 | 0.83 | |
B * C * D | 1.51 | 1 | 1.51 | 1.00 | |
A * B * C * D | 5.14 | 2 | 2.57 | 1.70 | |
Error (B) | 243.76 | 161 | 1.51 | ||
Error (between variables) | 416.42 | 161 | 2.59 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kim, B.; Pack, Y.H.; Yi, S.H. The Effect of Message Framing on Preschoolers’ Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions: Influences of Age, Issue Involvement, and Delivery Method. Educ. Sci. 2022, 12, 550. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12080550
Kim B, Pack YH, Yi SH. The Effect of Message Framing on Preschoolers’ Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions: Influences of Age, Issue Involvement, and Delivery Method. Education Sciences. 2022; 12(8):550. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12080550
Chicago/Turabian StyleKim, Bokyung, Yun Hyun Pack, and Soon Hyung Yi. 2022. "The Effect of Message Framing on Preschoolers’ Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions: Influences of Age, Issue Involvement, and Delivery Method" Education Sciences 12, no. 8: 550. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12080550
APA StyleKim, B., Pack, Y. H., & Yi, S. H. (2022). The Effect of Message Framing on Preschoolers’ Attitudes and Behavioral Intentions: Influences of Age, Issue Involvement, and Delivery Method. Education Sciences, 12(8), 550. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12080550