Next Article in Journal
Teachers’ Pedagogical Competence in Finnish Early Childhood Education—A Narrative Literature Review
Previous Article in Journal
What Competencies and Capabilities Identify a Good Teacher? Design of an Instrument to Measure Preservice Teachers’ Perceptions
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

An Exploratory Analysis of Transactive Interaction Patterns in Cooperative Learning Using Sequential Analysis

1
Institute of Educational Science, Department of Research on Teaching and Learning, University of Kassel, 34127 Kassel, Germany
2
Infrastructure Management Consultants, 68163 Mannheim, Germany
3
Hessian University for Public Management and Security, 34134 Kassel, Germany
4
Department of Student and Academic Affairs, University of Kassel, 34109 Kassel, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Educ. Sci. 2023, 13(8), 790; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci13080790
Submission received: 11 May 2023 / Revised: 14 July 2023 / Accepted: 24 July 2023 / Published: 3 August 2023

Abstract

:
For cooperative learning to be effective, the quality of student–student interaction is crucial. Interactions, which are transactive in nature, are positively related to students’ learning success during cooperative learning. However, little is known about typical interaction patterns during transactive interaction in face-to-face cooperative learning. Therefore, the current study aims to analyze typical interaction patterns of transactive interaction in cooperative learning. Sixty-eight students from seventh to tenth grade were randomly assigned to a total of 23 groups in their classes. The groups were videotaped while solving the same open-ended mathematical modelling task. The interaction behavior was coded, and interaction patterns were analyzed using sequential analysis with first- and second-order Markov chains. The results indicate that the likelihood that students confirm and pick up correct proposals is relatively high, indicating transactive interaction. However, it is almost equally likely that incorrect proposals are confirmed erroneously, as students barely correct them. Still, students do frequently engage in transactive interaction by discussing incorrect proposals, even though these discussions rarely lead to correct solution approaches. Limitations of these results, as well as the practical implications for cooperative learning in classroom settings, are discussed.

1. Introduction

Cooperative learning methods are widely used in classroom settings, in which students solve a given problem together in small groups. Key elements of cooperative learning are positive interdependence between the students, meaning that the students can reach their goal only together, and their individual responsibility, that is, each student feeling responsibility for making their individual contribution to the solution of the task [1]. Various studies have shown that cooperative learning can lead to better learning outcomes than individual work [2,3,4,5]. However, when comparing the individual studies included in the meta-analysis by Kyndt et al. [4], a heterogenous pattern of results regarding the effectiveness of cooperative learning emerges—reaching positive to even negative effects. These differences in the effectiveness of cooperative learning are partially attributed to the specific methods of cooperative learning used in the different studies (e.g., with or without incentives, task specialization). However, effects vary even within a single method of cooperative learning, so other factors seem to influence the effectiveness as well, such as student age, the domain, the complexity of the task, and the quality of interaction during cooperative learning [4,6,7,8].

1.1. Interaction during Cooperative Learning

Referring to the offer-and-use model of instruction [8,9,10,11], offering students to solve a task together in small groups is a learning opportunity they have to use by engaging in cognitive activities. These cognitive activities manifest in student behavior, e.g., in meaningful interaction with their peers in cooperative learning. Numerous studies have examined students’ interaction behavior during cooperative learning and have identified various activities that are effective for learning. According to Kaendler et al. [12], the quality of interaction in cooperative learning can be described on the basis of three dimensions: collaborative, cognitive, and metacognitive activities. Collaborative activities refer, among other things, to the extent to which learners listen to each other, share information, cooperate with each other, and encourage other group members. Cognitive activities include activities such as asking task-related questions, giving elaborative explanations [13,14,15], and explaining and reasoning about their solution approach [16]. Such cognitive activities have been shown to foster learning as they promote the integration of the to-be-learned content into existing knowledge structures, as well as recognizing and correcting prior misconceptions [17,18]. Finally, metacognitive activities include the planning, monitoring, and evaluation of the solution process and individual understanding.

1.2. Transactive Interaction in Cooperative Learning

Notably, cooperative learning allows students not only to engage in interaction activities that enable students to elaborate and reflect on their own knowledge but also to engage in transactive interaction [19]. Berkowitz and Gibbs [20] define transactive interaction as “reasoning that operates on the reasoning of another” (p. 402), meaning that students refer to each other’s ideas and transform them into more elaborate ones. Thus, transactive interaction exceeds constructive activities in the sense of Chi and Wylie [21], who define constructive activities as self-constructing knowledge from the learning material without taking the ideas of others into account (e.g., taking notes while reading a text). However, in transactive interaction, students engage in interactive activities [22], meaning that all persons in this interaction contribute to it constructively, leading to newly generated knowledge by interacting. Thus, transactive interaction includes contributions such as paraphrases, critiques, extensions, integrations of different ideas, requests for clarification or elaboration, and answers to such transactive questions [20,23,24]. Former research has shown that students during cooperative learning vividly engage in transactive interaction, but there are distinct differences regarding the engagement in such interaction on the group and individual level [10,25]. Transactive interaction favors deep-level cognitive processing, i.e., it can lead to a deeper elaboration of the learning content and to better learning outcomes for both the actor who elaborated their partner’s solution approach and for their partner whose idea has been elaborated [24]. Research studies provide evidence for the relevance of transactive interaction during cooperative learning for learning: paraphrasing group members’ ideas; sharing solution approaches; asking for more detailed explanations; and discussing, correcting, and integrating the different solution ideas, as well as further developing them, have been shown to be related to learning success [26,27,28,29,30,31].
Hänze and Jurkowski [22] subdivide transactive utterances into low-transactive and high-transactive ones. Low-transactive utterances include, e.g., paraphrases and inquiries, which can benefit the comprehension of all group members. High-transactive utterances, such as questioning or rejecting others’ solution ideas and further developing the solution approaches of group members, can explicitly help students to move forward in their solution process. Vogel et al. [26] have shown that the amount of engagement in dialogic transactivity during computer-supported peer learning, i.e., taking the idea of a group member into account by using or extending it, is not related to learning outcome, while dialectic transactivity, i.e., critiquing or integrating different contributions, is positively related to learning. The results provide evidence that the effect of engaging in transactive interaction might vary regarding the concrete transactive activity and how far this activity leads to more profound elaboration.
Barron’s [27] findings add a more subject-related perspective to the rather structural perspective on transactive interaction and support the relevance of interactions, which are transactive in nature. She has shown that neither sixth graders’ prior knowledge nor the total amount of incorrect and correct proposals during small-group work in mathematics are related to the learning success of the groups. However, the number of solution approaches linked to one another differentiated low-achieving from high-achieving groups. Moreover, she showed that in successful groups, correct proposals were more often accepted and discussed than in less successful groups. In contrast, correct proposals were more often discarded or ignored in less successful groups than in successful ones. In addition, the correct solution approaches should not only be discussed and integrated; how students deal with incorrect solution approaches is also relevant, as the results of Barnes’ [32] qualitative study with eleventh graders working in small groups in mathematics illustrate: “For collaborative learning to be effective, groups need to contain some people who question the usefulness of suggestions or the validity of arguments, look for alternative methods, seek clarification of inadequate explanations, and point out flaws in reasoning and inaccuracies in calculations” (p. 9).
In sum, transactive interaction during cooperative learning—in which students, e.g., share and discuss their solution approaches, further develop each other’s ideas, integrate useful solution approaches, and question and correct incorrect ones—is positively related to individuals’ learning success as well as to group outcome. However, transactive utterances, and especially high-transactive utterances, are cognitively challenging, and it remains unclear under which conditions students are willing to engage in these transactive interactions [22]. Moreover, there is a lack of research regarding interaction patterns in transactive interaction in face-to-face cooperative learning.

1.3. Analyzing Student Interaction in Cooperative Learning with Sequential Analysis

In most cases, the analysis of interaction in cooperative learning is based on the categorization of single events [33]. However, the structure of interactions themselves is sequential: by interacting, individuals influence each other. Thus, one event in an interaction process cannot be considered without the former one(s). Moreover, single-event analyses do not provide the possibility to analyze interaction patterns and, thus, to obtain an insight into transactive interaction during cooperative learning. With single-event analyses, it is not possible to analyze how students deal with competing ideas and arising conflicts, how far they question each other’s solution approaches, and how far they build upon each other’s ideas.
Next to, for example, network analysis [10,34], sequential analysis can help to analyze interaction sequences quantitatively. With this method, transitional probabilities from one event to another can be determined [35]. Markov chains provide the opportunity to describe typical interaction patterns and how individuals refer to each other. Thus, it is a suitable method to analyze interaction patterns in transactive interaction in cooperative learning. In the context of computer-based collaborative learning (CSCL) settings, this method has already been successfully applied to explore interaction patterns [36,37,38,39], while research on using this method on face-to-face peer interaction in school settings is scarce. In the following two sections, we will further introduce the application and potential benefit of using first- and second-order Markov chains when analyzing interaction patterns in cooperative learning.

1.3.1. First-Order Markov Chains

First-order Markov chains calculate the probability of the occurrence of the target event depending on the given event. Thus, typical sequential patterns can be determined. Initially, the absolute frequencies of each response type to each type of event are counted (for a detailed explanation of the procedure, see [35,40]). Based on these absolute frequencies, the transitional probabilities, that is, the relative frequencies of one response type to one type of event, are calculated. For example, when ten questions were asked during cooperative learning, of which four were answered correctly and six incorrectly, the transitional probability of a correct answer following a question would be 4/10 = 0.40, and for incorrect answers, 6/10 = 0.60. A shortcoming of such first-order Markov chains is that the probability of the occurrence of the target event only depends on the event that immediately occurred previously (given event). However, interactions do not only consist of two-event sequences. It can be expected that the target event is dependent not only on the previous one but also on longer sequences. Therefore, an extension to nth-order Markov chains is promising. The higher the number of events in an analyzed sequence, the more rapidly the required sample size increases, so we will only elaborate on second-order Markov chains in the following.

1.3.2. Second-Order Markov Chains

In contrast to first-order Markov chains, second-order Markov chains do not only trace the occurrence of the target event back to the event that occurred immediately previously. Three-event sequences are the basis of the analysis using second-order Markov chains. The transitional probability for the occurrence of the target event following the given event is contingent on the former event. The expected advantage of second-order Markov chains is demonstrated in the following example:
Using first-order Markov chains as in the fictive example above, the probability of a correct answer after a question was 40%, and the probability of an incorrect answer was 60%. However, it can be assumed that the transitional probabilities for correct and incorrect answers are related to whether the question is asked following a correct or incorrect declaration, as the notional example in Figure 1 shows:
In this fictive example, the likelihood of correct answers (target event) to a question (given event) that was asked following a correct declaration (former event) was 70% and thus higher than for questions asked after an incorrect declaration (20%). In contrast, the likelihood of incorrect answers after a question following an incorrect declaration was 80% and therefore higher than the likelihood of incorrect answers to questions asked after correct declarations (30%). Similar conditional dependencies on whether the former event is correct or incorrect might also apply to the transitional probabilities between other interaction events.

1.4. The Present Study

Transactive interaction, including discussing conflicting views, clarifying flaws in reasoning, questioning the ideas of other group members, and finally, integrating the (correct) ideas of others in the ongoing solution process, seems to be crucial for promoting learning in cooperative learning settings. Even though much research has been performed on learning-supportive interaction behavior, less is known about typical interaction patterns in cooperative learning. It is unclear how students engage in transactive interaction during face-to-face cooperative learning. Under which conditions do students further develop each other’s ideas, reject or confirm solution approaches by other group members, and ask questions and provide answers, and how do students deal with correct or incorrect utterances? In order to fill this research gap, this study aims to exploratively take a closer look at interaction patterns during transactive interaction in cooperative learning using sequential analysis.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design and Participants

The study took place in the middle of the 2015/2016 school year [41]. The sample consisted of 68 seventh- to tenth-graders from one comprehensive school in Germany. The students were videotaped while working face-to-face on the same mathematical modelling task in small groups of two to four students. It was a convenience sample, which should be considered when interpreting the results of this study.
Of the students, 28% were in seventh grade, 21% in eighth grade, 22% in ninth grade, and 29% in tenth grade. About half of the students were female (45%). On average, the students were 14.09 (SD = 1.29) years old. The students were randomly assigned to groups in their classes. In total, 23 groups were formed: two groups of two, twenty groups of three, and one group of four students. The group compositions were gender-heterogeneous (73%) and homogeneous (27%).
Cooperative learning was structured using the think–pair–share method [42]. The lessons were held by a teacher of the research team who did not know the classes and who was instructed to intervene as little as possible during the group work. Although previous research emphasizes the role of teachers during cooperative learning in the form of adaptive teacher interventions [43,44], we chose to minimize teacher interventions because we were interested in students’ transactive interaction behavior without teacher interventions. The lesson was structured through an introductory phase, in which the teacher explained the task; an individual work phase for about 10 min, in which the students dealt with the task individually and developed initial solution approaches (think phase); a subsequent group phase, in which the students shared their understanding of the task and their solution approaches and jointly solved the task (pair phase); and a plenum phase, in which some of the groups presented their solutions (share phase). The mathematical modelling task shown in Figure 2 was taken from Herget [45] (pp. 189–190).
The different levels of mathematical competence between the grades were compensated for by providing additional task-relevant information for students from grades seven and eight, whereas students from grades nine and ten had to gather this information by themselves (see Figure 2). Only the pair phase (M = 25.56 min, SD = 3.67 min) was considered in the following analyses.

2.2. Video-Based Analysis of Student–Student Interaction

The videos of the groups solving the task cooperatively were used to categorize the students’ interaction behavior. The analysis was conducted by two trained coders and a master coder who developed the coding manual. They coded the videos independently using the program Videograph [48]. In the first step, the pair phase was coded by separating it from the think and share phases. Subsequently, each student’s turn during the pair phase was identified and categorized following a standardized coding manual concerning the following categories: task relation, function, reference, directness of reference, syntactic classification, type of question, type of declaration, and correctness. Following Jurkowski and Hänze’s understanding of transactive interaction during cooperative learning, self-referential turns were not considered as having a reference to another turn [29,30]. The coding scheme is systematically shown in Figure 3.
The categories and their coding rules were self-developed or partially adapted from already existing coding systems as shown in Table 1. Each coding system was supplemented by a residual category, including those turns that could not unambiguously be assigned to one of the subcategories.
Four randomly chosen videos were coded by the master coder and the two coders, while the remaining 19 videos were randomly distributed between the two coders. The intercoder reliabilities of the categories were calculated using the percentage of agreement and Cohen’s κ over the four videos all three coders coded. The calculation of Cohen’s κ was not possible for the categories pair phase and turn since the event only had to be identified and not categorized. The intercoder reliabilities were satisfactory for all categories/coding systems, as shown in Table 2.

2.3. Data Preparation

Before the analysis was performed, the data had to be prepared. The coded turns of the students were restructured and merged into one variable to be able to detect typical interaction patterns. In sum, 10060 turns in 23 groups were identified. However, only task-related utterances were considered (8329 turns). Because we were interested in how students respond to each other in transactive interactions, all kinds of utterances had to have a direct reference. Thus, it was ensured that the target event did indeed relate to the given event, i.e., the previously occurred event. The only exception were statements, which were also considered when they had no reference. Statements without a reference could indicate new solution approaches so that the transitional probabilities could be used to analyze how new solutions are handled, while statements with a direct reference indicate a pick-up of the given turn into the ongoing solution process. In total, 4851 of 6679 declarations, i.e., statements, answers, agreements, and disagreements, had a direct reference and 1028 of 6679 did not have a direct reference. In addition, it was considered whether the declarations were correct or incorrect, which led to another reduction of the declarations that were considered in the analyses. A total of 3970 out of the 5879 declarations with reference or statements without a reference could be coded as correct or incorrect. Regarding the coding system type of question, only the content-specific questions were part of the analysis since they could represent critical questions (1179 of 1540 questions in total). Those content-specific questions also had to have a direct reference so that they could constitute an immediate response to the former event, which reduced the total number to 772 content-specific questions with a direct reference.
This data restructuring led to 11 category combinations that were part of the following analyses (Table 3). Pick-ups, disagreements, and questions illustrate indicators for transactive interaction. Pick-ups include paraphrasing ideas from other group members, further developments of ideas, and the integration of different solution approaches. By disagreeing or asking content-related questions that indicate critical inquiries, students could contribute to a change in or clarification of the current solution approach. Questions as coded in this study, furthermore, include questions with the goal of clarifying if one has understood the idea of a group member appropriately, questions to gather further information, and critical inquiries. Although the other categories do not represent transactive interaction by definition, they take an important role in the solution process: new ideas and answers can be the starting point of transactive interaction, i.e., by picking them up or disagreeing. Agreements can reinforce the pursuit of a solution approach. Table 3 shows the frequencies of the different categories. Only incorrect pick-ups were not observed, so this category was excluded from the following analyses.

2.4. Data Analyses

To gain insight into students’ transactive interaction during cooperative learning, first- and second-order Markov chains (see Section 1.3) using the described category combinations (Table 3) were calculated. With the first-order Markov chains, the probability of one event following another event (e.g., the probability of a correct answer following a question or a correct disagreement following an incorrect answer) was analyzed. In addition to first-order Markov chains, second-order Markov chains were used, as it can be expected that students’ interaction behavior does not only depend on the immediately previous utterance but also on the former event (former event → given event → target event). However, we only included the former event differentiated by its correctness and did not consider the specific category because a much higher number of coded utterances would be necessary to fill the 113 = 1331 possible different three-event-sequences. By considering only whether the former event is correct or incorrect, the number of possible three-event-sequences was reduced to 242.
For both first- and second-order Markov chains, z-scores were calculated to detect whether the transitional probabilities were on a 5%-level significantly lower (z-score < −1.96) or higher (z-score > 1.96) than expected using the formula by Bakeman and Gottman [35] (p. 109).

3. Results

3.1. Results of the First-Order Markov Chains

In the first step, the transitional probabilities between the different interaction categories were calculated using first-order Markov chains. Because 4742 category combinations were identified (see Table 3), a total number of 4741 event sequences for the following analysis would have been expected. However, only 3214 event sequences were part of the analysis. This reduction in event sequences is due to incomplete sequences because two-event sequences are needed to calculate the transitional probabilities. These lacks in the data are due to non-completely codable turns (see Section 2.2).
The results are shown in Table 4: The number in each cell represents the transitional probability for each possible event sequence based on the total number of responses to this specific category (last cell in each row). Bold numbers represent transitional probabilities that were significantly higher than expected, and underlined numbers identify transitional probabilities that were significantly lower than expected. For instance, a correct new idea was followed in 23% of cases by a correct pick-up and in 32% of cases by a correct agreement. Both transitional probabilities were significantly higher than statistically expected, while, for example, the transitional probability from a correct new idea to an incorrect disagreement was with 15% significantly lower than expected.
To illustrate the striking interaction patterns, Figure 4 only contains those transitional probabilities between the interaction events that were significantly higher than expected. The following patterns were apparent regarding the handling of correct solution approaches: a correct new idea (given event) was followed in 32% of cases by a correct agreement (target event). The high probability of agreements after correct declarations could also be shown for the other forms of declarations: correct agreements could be determined with 29% after a correct pick-up, with 26% after a correct disagreement, with 26% after a correct answer, and with 23% after a correct agreement. Moreover, correct declarations led to a high rate of transactive interaction in the form of pick-ups: students picked up correct new ideas in 23% of cases, correct agreements in 35% of cases, correct disagreements in 22% of cases, correct answers in 23% of cases, and correct pick-ups themselves in 26% of cases. Thus, it is likely that correct declarations are used for the further solution process, either by confirming them through agreement or by directly picking them up leading to transactive interaction. Another central finding is that no significant transitional probability from a correct proposal to an incorrect one could be found.
Concerning the handling of incorrect proposals, the following significant transitional probabilities were found. Comparable to the handling of correct proposals, students confirmed incorrect proposals with a high probability: incorrect new ideas were followed by incorrect agreements in 30% of cases, incorrect disagreements were agreed with in 23% of cases, incorrect answers in 23% of cases, and incorrect agreements themselves in 17% of cases. Furthermore, the high probability of incorrect disagreements following other incorrect proposals is conspicuous (with 35% after incorrect agreements, with 27% after incorrect answers, and with 28% after incorrect disagreements themselves).
Only two direct and one indirect transitional probability from an incorrect proposal leading to a correct one were found. Correct disagreements followed incorrect new ideas as well as incorrect disagreements in 8% of cases. Moreover, incorrect disagreements were followed by questions in 22% of cases, which could constitute an indirect transition to a correct solution approach. However, questions themselves were more often answered incorrectly, in 31% of cases, than correctly, in 23% of cases. These results illustrate that incorrect declarations can also lead to transactive interaction because students try to convert the wrong solution approach into a correct one by rejecting it. Furthermore, correct disagreements—although they are rare—are then likely to be agreed to or picked up and are thus further used transactively in the solution process.

3.2. Results of the Second-Order Markov Chains

In the second step, the transitional probabilities were calculated using second-order Markov chains. The correctness of the former event was also considered in the calculations, as it could influence the transitional probabilities. Two matrices (see Table 5 and Table 6) were generated, including the transitional probabilities—one for correct and one for incorrect former events. Due to the calculation of second-order Markov chains, the number of data that were part of the analysis was reduced. On the one hand, this was due to the necessity of three-event sequences, so incomplete sequences were excluded. On the other hand, the former event was only considered if it was a declaration because its correctness was of interest.
Table 5 shows all transitional probabilities between the different interaction events when the former event was correct. For instance, a correct new idea (given event) was followed in 35% of cases by a correct agreement (target event) if the former event was correct (former event). This transitional probability was significantly higher than expected. Table 6 shows the transitional probabilities between the different interaction events when the former event was incorrect. Here, a correct new idea was followed by 19% by a correct agreement. This transitional probability was also significantly higher than expected.
In Figure 5, the results of the second-order Markov chains with correct and incorrect former events are combined and reduced to those transitional probabilities that were significantly higher than expected. The transitional probabilities for second-order Markov chains with correct former events are given on the left; the transitional probabilities for second-order Markov chains with incorrect former events are given on the right. The percentages can be read as follows: if the former declaration was correct, students disagreed correctly with 20% to incorrect agreements. If the former declaration was incorrect, the transitional probability from an incorrect agreement to a correct disagreement was not significant. If the former declaration was correct, students agreed with 35% to new ideas. If the former declaration was incorrect, students agreed with 19% to new ideas.
The analysis with first-order Markov chains revealed only low transitional probabilities from incorrect proposals to correct ones (Figure 4). When considering the correctness of the former event, the following patterns become apparent: the probabilities of correct disagreements following incorrect proposals were in some cases significantly higher than expected when the former event was correct (20% after incorrect agreements, 14% after incorrect disagreements). However, there were no such significant transitional probabilities from an incorrect declaration to a correct one when the former event was incorrect. This finding might indicate that it is easier for students to engage in transactive interaction leading to a transformation of an incorrect proposal into a correct one by disagreeing correctly when they had already taken a correct solution approach.
Yet it was very likely that the students disagreed incorrectly with incorrect proposals when the former event was incorrect, while these transitional probabilities were only partly significant when the former event was correct (Figure 5). For instance, incorrect agreements were followed in 37% of cases by incorrect disagreements when the former event of this sequence was incorrect. This transitional probability was not significant when the former event was correct. This result, in turn, could indicate that the students recognized the flaws in the interaction process and, therefore, engaged in transactive interaction by disagreeing but were not able to correct them when the former event was incorrect. Comparable with the results of the first-order Markov chains, the results of both second-order Markov chains with correct and incorrect former events again revealed a high rate of incorrect agreements with incorrect proposals. It was very likely that the students confirmed incorrect proposals irrespective of whether the former event was correct or incorrect.
Furthermore, it becomes clear that the high probability of questions after incorrect disagreements (22%; see Figure 4), another way to turn an incorrect proposal into a correct one by initiating transactive interaction, was only significant when the former event was correct (23%). This finding may reveal that students only question incorrect proposals if they already had a correct solution approach. Closely linked with this finding, another noticeable difference was found: using first-order Markov chains, the transitional probability of a correct answer after a question was lower than the probability for an incorrect one (Figure 4). However, when the question was asked following a correct declaration, the probability of a correct answer was with 34% higher than the probability of an incorrect one (14%). In comparison, the likelihood of a correct answer to a question that was asked after an incorrect proposal was with 14% lower than the probability of an incorrect answer (42%).
Comparable with the results of first-order Markov chains (Figure 4), it was very likely that students agreed correctly to correct proposals when using second-order Markov chains. However, there were some differences between second-order Markov chains with correct and incorrect former events. While there were no significant transitional probabilities from a correct answer and a correct disagreement to a correct agreement when the former event was correct, these transitional probabilities were significant with incorrect former events (Figure 5). Thus, students seem to realize that the reinforcement of a correct answer or disagreement is not necessary if they have already followed a correct solution approach, while they reinforce correct answers or disagreements when they occurred after an incorrect solution path. In comparison, the likelihood of correct agreements following correct new ideas (35%) and correct pick-ups (31%) was higher when the former event was correct than when it was incorrect (correct new ideas: 19%, correct pick-ups: 22%). Furthermore, correct pick-ups of correct proposals were again very likely when second-order Markov chains were used. Still, there were some differences depending on whether the former event was correct or incorrect (Figure 5). For instance, correct pick-ups were only then picked up significantly more often than expected (33%) when the former event was correct, while correct disagreements were only picked up more often than expected (21%) when the former event was incorrect.

4. Discussion

The goal of the present study was to investigate exploratorily how students engage in transactive interaction during face-to-face cooperative learning. The analysis with first-order Markov chains revealed that the concrete way students engage in transactive interaction depends on whether they react to a correct or incorrect declaration. While correct declarations do primarily lead to agreements and pick-ups, with the latter representing transactive interaction, incorrect declarations are seldom picked up. Incorrect declarations are instead questioned or disagreed with. However, these questions and disagreements to incorrect proposals only occur and only lead to correct proposals under certain circumstances. The second-order Markov chains revealed that students seemed to only question incorrect proposals if they already had a correct solution approach. Furthermore, these questions were more often answered correctly when they were asked after a correct declaration, while they were more often answered incorrectly when following an incorrect declaration. Disagreements to incorrect proposals, as another transaction with which, in principle, a wrong solution path can be converted into a correct one, only occurred when the former event was correct. This finding might indicate that it is easier for students to transform an incorrect proposal into a correct one by disagreeing correctly when they had already taken a correct solution approach. Moreover, it was very likely that the students disagreed incorrectly with incorrect proposals when the former event was incorrect, while these transitional probabilities were only partly significant when the former event was correct (Figure 5). For instance, incorrect agreements were followed in 37% of cases by incorrect disagreements when the former event of this sequence was incorrect. Although students engaged in transactive interaction by recognizing the flaws regarding the solution approach and disagreeing with it, they were not able to turn it into a correct solution approach when the former event was incorrect. These results illustrate that students seem to be aware that the current solution approach is non-expedient and thus engage in transactive interaction by questioning and disagreeing. However, both transactions seldom lead to a correct solution approach. Moreover, it was very likely that the students confirmed incorrect proposals irrespective of whether the former event was correct or incorrect, leading to a reinforcement of pursuing the wrong solution approach.
In conclusion, students support each other and further develop each other’s ideas when reacting to a correct declaration. Regarding incorrect declarations, transactive interaction takes the form of students vividly disagreeing or questioning each other, although this rarely leads to a correct solution approach. Thus, students seem to adapt their way of transacting with each other regarding the correctness of the solution path followed.

4.1. Limitations

Although this study gives a detailed insight into the interaction processes in cooperative learning by using sequential analysis, a few limitations have to be taken into account. First, our coding manual included only three overarching forms of transactive interaction, i.e., pick-ups, disagreements, and questions. Pick-ups include declarations such as paraphrases, extensions, and integrations of ideas. Furthermore, our questions also included different forms of transactions, namely questions with the goal of clarifying if one has understood the idea of a group member appropriately, questions to gather further information, and critical inquiries. To gain a deeper insight into how students engage in transactive interaction, these transacts should be coded separately in future analyses, e.g., [20,23,57]. It would also be possible to analyze the conditions for engagement in low- and high-transactive interaction separately. Second, we had a relatively small sample of 68 students out of only four classes of one school in our study. Moreover, these students were from different grades. Third, the transitional probabilities were calculated over all 23 groups. However, it can be assumed that those may vary between the different groups depending on the group composition. Relating to that, the interaction behavior of the individual students may also depend on the group composition. The individual engagement in discussions, e.g., in how far students agree or disagree with their group members’ solution approaches or how often they contribute their own solution approach, may be explained by the ambition for conformity in groups. This ambition for conformity can be triggered by an informational and/or normative social influence [58]. Due to the complexity of the task, some students might have been following the solution approaches of their group members—especially those group members with higher social status [59,60,61]. Low-status students, e.g., students with a low grade in mathematics or those who are less popular, may more often confirm proposals. In contrast, high-status students may more often disagree—correctly or incorrectly—with the proposed solutions of their group members. However, these assumptions need to be investigated in further research by analyzing how far the individual behavior in cooperative learning is dependent on the social status and the group composition. In order to analyze individual behavior, a much higher number of data is necessary. Even though we calculated the transitional probabilities over all groups, the absolute frequency of some event sequences was quite low, leading to limited generalizability. This problem of a small number of some event sequences might increase when calculating the transitional probabilities on an individual level. Fourth, in future studies, it should be investigated which kind of (transactive) interaction patterns contribute to or negatively affect student learning. Fifth, the transitional probabilities calculated in this study are limited not only to this specific sample but also to the used mathematical modelling task. Thus, further research is needed to investigate whether the found interaction patterns are transferable to other samples, group tasks, and subjects.

4.2. Practical Implications

In summary, our analyses revealed that students further develop correct proposals by their group members or confirm them during transactive interaction. Concerning wrong solution approaches, they frequently discuss or question. However, these discussions rarely led to a correct solution approach in our sample. Still, this might indicate that the students recognized flaws in reasoning and experienced a socio-cognitive conflict between their knowledge and their group members’ solution approaches but were not able to transfer the incorrect solution approaches into correct ones.
These findings point to the relevance of teachers for a successful implementation of cooperative learning: Teachers should monitor students’ collaborative, cognitive, and metacognitive activities and support them if necessary [12]. Moreover, diagnosing errors and misconceptions seems to be necessary as well. If students do not correct errors by themselves and are not able to transfer an incorrect solution approach into a correct one, teachers should provide domain-specific support, including, for example, feedback, or hints [12]. Such teacher support should be adaptive, i.e., it should match the specific difficulties of the respective group [43]. However, research shows that providing adaptive support during cooperative learning is a challenging endeavor for teachers. Meloth and Deering [62] have shown that teachers’ support is rarely adapted to the specific difficulties of learners. This is particularly problematic as teachers’ intervention that is not adapted to the group can be accompanied by a deterioration of the quality of student interaction [63]. An additional and promising approach to support student learning can be derived from research on productive failure, which shows that problem-solving prior to instruction can be superior to a sequence of instruction followed by problem-solving [64,65]. Productive failure instructions consist of two phases: in the first phase, students in small groups try to solve a problem, while incorrect solution approaches will (at least in some groups) probably be pursued. In the second phase, the teacher presents the correct solution, contrasting it to typical (incorrect) student solutions. As shown in our study, the students vividly discussed their different solution approaches—even though these discussions rarely led to correct solution approaches. These insufficient solutions would provide a good starting point for a subsequent in-class discussion of the canonical solution, because the students already might have detected knowledge gaps. Still, implementing such an in-class discussion about typical mistakes of the groups while solving the task is challenging for teachers: teachers have to monitor and diagnose students’ solution paths to be able to discuss and contrast them afterward.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.N.; Methodology, L.N. and T.B.; Software, T.B.; Formal Analysis, L.N.; Investigation: A.H. and A.-K.D., Data Curation, L.N. and T.B.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, L.N.; Writing—Review and Editing, L.N., T.B., A.H., A.-K.D. and F.L.; Visualization, L.N. and T.B.; Supervision, F.L., Project Administration, L.N., A.H., A.-K.D. and F.L., Funding Acquisition, F.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project is part of the “Qualitätsoffensive Lehrerbildung”, a joint initiative of the Federal Government and the Federal States that aims to improve the quality of teacher training. The program is funded by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (award number: 01JA1505). The authors are responsible for the content of this publication.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all parents of all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy restrictions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Johnson, D.W.; Johnson, R.T. An educational psychology success story: Social interdependence theory and cooperative learning. Educ. Res. 2009, 38, 365–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Ginsburg-Block, M.; Rohrbeck, C.A.; Fantuzzo, J.W. A meta-analytic review of social, self-concept, and behavioral outcomes of peer-assisted learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 98, 732–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Hattie, J. Visible Learning for Teachers: Maximizing Impact on Learning; Routledge: London, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  4. Kyndt, E.; Raes, E.; Lismont, B.; Timmers, F.; Cascallar, E.; Dochy, F. A meta-analysis of the effects of face-to-face cooperative learning. Do recent studies falsify or verify earlier findings? Educ. Res. Rev. 2013, 10, 133–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Rohrbeck, C.A.; Ginsurg-Block, M.; Fantuzzo, J.W.; Miller, T.R. Peer-assisted learning interventions with elementary school students: A meta-analytic review. J. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 95, 240–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bleck, V.; Lipowsky, F. Kooperatives Lernen—Theoretische Perspektiven, empirische Befunde und Konsequenzen für die Implementierung. [Cooperative learning—Theoretical perspectives, empirical results, and consequences for its implementation]. In Handbuch Schulforschung; Hascher, T., Idel, T.-S., Helsper, W., Eds.; Springer: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Jürgen-Lohmann, J.; Borsch, F.; Giesen, H. Kooperativer Unterricht in unterschiedlichen schulischen Lernumgebungen. [Cooperative instruction in different school environments]. Unterrichtswissenschaft 2002, 30, 367–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lipowsky, F. Unterricht. [Instruction]. In Pädagogische Psychologie; Wild, E., Möller, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Helmke, A. (Ed.) Unterrichtsqualität und Lehrerprofessionalität: Diagnose, Evaluation und Verbesserung des Unterrichts; [Instructional Quality and Teachers‘ Professionalism. Diagnosis, Evaluation, and Improvement of Instruction], 2nd ed.; Klett: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  10. Troll, B.; Heil, D.; Pietsch, M.; Besser, M. Netzwerkbasierte Betrachtung von ko-konstruktiven Interaktionsprozessen im Unterricht—Ein Ansatz zur Beschreibung und Analyse von Angebot und Nutzung. [A network-based view of co-constructive interaction processes in classrooms—An approach to the description and analysis of opportunity and use]. Unterrichtswissenschaft 2022, 50, 237–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Vieluf, S.; Praetorius, A.-K.; Rakoczy, K.; Kleinknecht, M.; Pietsch, M. Angebots-Nutzungsmodelle der Wirkweise des Unterrichts: Eine kritische Auseinandersetzung mit ihrer theoretischen Konzeption. [Offer-and-use-models of instruction: A critical examniation of their theoretical conception]. Z. Für Pädagogik 2020, 66, 63–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kaendler, C.; Wiedmann, M.; Rummel, N.; Spada, H. Teacher competencies for the implementation of collaborative learning in the classroom: A framework and research review. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2015, 27, 505–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. King, A. Guiding knowledge construction in the classroom: Effects of teaching children how to question and how to explain. Am. Educ. Res. J. 1994, 31, 338–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Webb, N.M. Peer interaction and learning in small groups. Int. J. Educ. Res. 1989, 13, 21–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Webb, N.M.; Palincsar, A.S. Group processes in the classroom. In Handbook of Educational Psychology; Berliner, D.C., Calfee, R.C., Eds.; Prentice Hall International: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1996; pp. 841–873. [Google Scholar]
  16. Baker, M.; Andriessen, J.; Lund, K.; van Amelsvoort, M.; Quignard, M. Rainbow: A framework for analyzing computer-mediated pedagogical debates. Int. J. Comput.-Support. Collab. Learn. 2007, 2, 247–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gätje, O.; Jurkowski, S. When students interlink ideas in peer learning: Linguistic characteristics of transactivity in argumentative discourse. Int. J. Educ. Res. Open 2021, 2, 100065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Webb, N.M. Peer learning in the classroom. In International Encyclopedia of Education; Peterson, P., Baker, E., McGaw, B., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2010; pp. 636–642. [Google Scholar]
  19. Van Boxtel, C.; Roelofs, E. Investigating the quality of student discourse: What constitutes a productive student discourse. J. Classr. Interact. 2001, 36, 55–62. [Google Scholar]
  20. Berkowitz, M.W.; Gibbs, J.C. Measuring the developmental features of moral discussion. Merrill-Palmer Q. 1983, 29, 399–410. [Google Scholar]
  21. Chi MT, H.; Wylie, R. The ICAP framework: Linking cognitive engagement to active learning outcomes. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 49, 219–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hänze, M.; Jurkowski, S. Das Potenzial kooperativen Lernens ausschöpfen: Die Bedeutung der transaktiven Kommunikation für eine lernwirksame Zusammenarbeit. [Maximizing the potential of cooperative learning: The relevance of transactive communication for successfull collaboration]. Z. Für Pädagogische Psychol. 2022, 36, 141–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Azmitia, M.; Montgomery, R. Friendship, transactive dialogues, and the development of scientific reasoning. Soc. Dev. 1993, 2, 202–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Kruger, A.C.; Tomasello, M. Transactive discussions with peers and adults. Dev. Psychol. 1986, 22, 681–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Nemeth, L.; Denn, A.-K.; Hirstein, A.; Lipowsky, F. Interaktionen von Schülerinnen und Schülern in kooperativen Lernsituationen [Student-student interaction during cooperative learning]. In Interaktion im Klassenzimmer. Forschungsgeleitete Einblicke in das Geschehen im Unterricht; Schäfer, L., Verriere, K., Eds.; Springer VS: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2019; pp. 51–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Vogel, F.; Kollar, I.; Ufer, S.; Reichersdorfer, E.; Reiss, K.; Fischer, F. Developing argumentation skills in mathematics through computer-supported collaborative learning: The role of transactivity. Instr. Sci. 2016, 44, 477–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Barron, B. When smart groups fail. J. Learn. Sci. 2003, 94, 307–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. De Lisi, R.; Golbeck, S.L. Implications of Piagetian theory for peer learning. In Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning; O’Donnell, A.M., King, A., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1999; pp. 3–37. [Google Scholar]
  29. Jurkowski, S.; Hänze, M. Soziale Kompetenzen, transaktives Interaktionsverhalten und Lernerfolg. Experimenteller Vergleich zweier unterschiedlich gestalteter Gruppenunterrichtsbedingungen und Evaluation eines transaktivitätsbezogenen Kooperationsskripts. [Social competencies, transactive interaction, and achievement. Comparison of two differently designed cooperative learning environments and evaluation of scripting transactive interaction]. Z. Für Pädagogische Psychol. 2010, 24, 241–257. [Google Scholar]
  30. Jurkowski, S.; Hänze, M. How to increase the benefits of cooperation: Effects of training in transactive communication on cooperative learning. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 85, 357–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Teasley, S. Talking about reasoning: How important is the peer in peer collaboration? In Discourse, Tools and Reasoning: Essays on Situated Cognition; Resnick, L.B., Säljö, R., Pontecorvo, C., Burge, B., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1997; pp. 361–384. [Google Scholar]
  32. Barnes, M. The Use of Positioning Theory in Studying Student Participation in Collaborative Learning Activities. In Proceedings of the Paper Presented as Part of the Symposium “Social Positioning Theory as an Analytical Tool” at the Annual Meeting of the Australian Association for Research in Education, Melbourne, Australia, 28 November–2 December 2004; Available online: https://www.aare.edu.au/data/publications/2004/bar04684.pdf (accessed on 6 February 2023).
  33. Cress, U.; Hesse, F.W. Quantitative methods for studying small groups. In The International Handbook of Collaborative Learning; Hmelo-Silver, C.E., Chinn, C.A., Chan, C.K.K., O’Donnell, A.M., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2013; pp. 93–111. [Google Scholar]
  34. De Laat, M.; Lally, V.; Lipponen, L.; Simons, R.-J. Investigating patterns of interaction in networked learning and computer-supported collaborative learning: A role for Social Network Analysis. Int. J. Comput.-Support. Collab. Learn. 2007, 2, 87–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Bakeman, R.; Gottman, J. Observing Interaction. An Introduction to Sequential Analysis; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  36. Erkens, G.; Prangsma, M.E.; Jaspers JG, M.; Kanselaar, G. Computer Support for Collaborative and Argumentative Writing; Utrecht University: Utrecht, The Netherlands, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hou, H.-T.; Chang, K.-E.; Sung, Y.-T. Analysis of problem-solving-based online asynchronous discussion pattern. Educ. Technol. Soc. 2008, 11, 17–28. [Google Scholar]
  38. Jeong, A.C. The sequential analysis of group interaction and critical thinking in online threaded discussions. Am. J. Distance Educ. 2003, 17, 25–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Jeong, A.C.; Li, H.; Pan, A.J. A sequential analysis of responses in online debates to postings of students exhibiting high versus low grammar and spelling errors. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 2017, 65, 1175–1194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Jeong, A.C. A guide to analyzing message-response sequences and group interaction patterns in computer-mediated communication. Distance Educ. 2005, 26, 367–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hirstein, A.; Denn, A.-K.; Jurkowski, S.; Lipowsky, F. Entwicklung der professionellen Wahrnehmungs- und Beurteilungsfähigkeit von Lehramtsstudierenden durch das Lernen mit kontrastierenden Videofällen – Anlage und erste Ergebnisse des Projekts KONTRAST [Developing professional vision and assessment by analyzing contrasting video cases – Design and first results of the KONTRAST project]. Beiträge zur Lehrerinnnen-und Lehrerbildung. 2017, 35, 472–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lyman, F. The responsive classroom discussion. In Mainstreaming Digest; Anderson, A.S., Ed.; University of Maryland: College Park, MD, USA, 1981; pp. 109–113. [Google Scholar]
  43. Pauli, C.; Reusser, K. Zur Rolle der Lehrperson beim kooperativen Lernen. [Teachers‘ role during cooperative learning]. Schweiz. Z. Für Bild. 2000, 22, 421–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Ding, M.; Li, X.; Piccolo, D.; Kulm, G. Teacher interventions in cooperative-learning mathematics classes. J. Educ. Res. 2007, 100, 162–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Herget, W. Typen von Aufgaben. [Types of tasks]. In Bildungsstandards Mathematik: Konkret. Sekundarstufe I: Aufgabenbeispiele, Unterrichtsanregungen, Fortbildungsideen, 6th ed.; Blum, W., Drüske-Noe, C., Hartung, R., Köller, O., Eds.; Cornelsen: Berlin, Germany, 2012; pp. 178–193. [Google Scholar]
  46. Archiv Büro André Heller. Picture of the Football Globe (Picture 31 of 46). Available online: http://archiv.artevent.at/fussballglobus_presse/ (accessed on 24 April 2019).
  47. Eckert-Kraft, B. “Uns” Uwe Seeler Wird 80. Uwe Seeler Turns 80. 2016. Available online: http://www.hamburg-city-webguide.de/2016/11/05/uns-uwe-seeler-wird-80/ (accessed on 24 April 2019).
  48. Rimmele, R. Videograph—Multimedia-Player zur Kodierung von Videos; [Videograph—Multimedia-Player for Video Coding]; Leibniz-Institut für die Pädagogik der Naturwissenschaften: Kiel, Germany, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  49. Pauli, C. Klassengespräch. [Classroom discussion]. In Dokumentation der Erhebungs- Und Auswertungsinstrumente Zur Schweizerisch-Deutschen Videostudie “Unterrichtsqualität, Lernverhalten und Mathematisches Verständnis”; Klieme, E., Pauli, C., Reusser, K., Eds.; Teil 3: Hugener, I., Pauli, C., Reusser, K.: Videoanalyse (Materialien zur Bildungsforschung, Band 15, S. 124–147); DIPF & GFPF: Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  50. Ackermann, S.; Gabriel, K.; Lipowsky, F. Die Kodierung von Redebeiträgen im Mathematikunterricht. [The coding of turns in mathematics classrooms]. In Dokumentation der Erhebungsinstrumente des Projekts “Persönlichkeits- und Lernentwicklung von Grundschulkindern” (PERLE); Lipowsky, F., Faust, G., Eds.; Teil 3: Lotz, M., Lipowsky, F., Faust, G.: Technischer Bericht zu den PERLE-Videostudien (Material zur Bildungsforschung, Band 23/3, S. 379–390); DIPF & GFPF: Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  51. Helmke, A.; Helmke, T.; Schrader, F.-W.; Wagner, W. Die Basiskodierung der DESI-Videostudie; [The Base Coding of the DESI-Video Study]; Universität Koblenz-Landau: Mainz, Germany, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  52. Hugener, I.; Drollinger-Vetter, B. Inhaltsbezogene Aktivitäten. [Content-related activities]. In Dokumentation der Erhebungs- und Auswertungsinstrumente zur Schweizerisch-Deutschen Videostudie “Unterrichtsqualität, Lernverhalten und Mathematisches Verständnis”; Klieme, E., Pauli, C., Reusser, K., Eds.; Teil 3: Hugener, I., Pauli, C., Reusser, K.: Videoanalyse (Materialien zur Bildungsforschung, Band 15, S. 62–88); DIPF & GFPF: Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  53. Hänze, M.; Berger, R. Kooperatives Lernen im Gruppenpuzzle und im Lernzirkel. [Cooperative learning within the Jigsaw Classroom and the Station-to-Station Method]. Unterrichtswissenschaft 2007, 35, 227–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Graesser, A.C.; Person, N.K. Question asking during tutoring. Am. Educ. Res. J. 1994, 31, 104–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kronenberger, J. Kooperatives Lernen im Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftlichen Unterricht der Primarstufe. Lernerfolg, Lernfreude und Elaborationsniveau im Gruppenpuzzle; [Cooperative Learning in Elementary School Mathematics and Science. Learning Success, Motivation, and Level of Elaboration Using the Jigsaw Method]; Dr. Kovac: Hamburg, Germany, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  56. Webb, N.M.; Farivar, S. Promoting helping behavior in cooperative small groups in middle school mathematics. Am. Educ. Res. J. 1994, 31, 369–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Jurkowski, S.; Mundelsee, L.; Jüngst, C.; Hänze, M. Messung gemeinsamer Wissenskonstruktion: Ein Vergleich von hoch-inferenter Beobachtung niedrig-inferenter Codierung und Selbsteinschätzung der transaktiven Kommunikation. [Measuring knowledge co-construction: A comparison of high-inference observation, low-inference coding and self-report of transactive communication]. Z. Für Erzieh. 2022, 25, 1505–1527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Aronson, E.; Wilson, T.D.; Akert, R.M. Social Psychology, 8th ed.; Pearson Education Limited: London, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  59. Cohen, E.G.; Lotan, R.A. Producing equal-status interaction in the heterogeneous classroom. Am. Educ. Res. J. 1995, 32, 99–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Judge, T.A.; Bono, J.E.; Ilies, R.; Gerhardt, M.W. Personality and leadership. A qualitative and quantitative review. J. Appl. Psychol. 2002, 87, 765–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Smith, J.A.; Foti, R.J. A pattern approach to study leader emergence. Leadersh. Q. 1998, 9, 147–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Meloth, M.S.; Deering, P.D. The role of teacher in promoting cognitive processing during collaborative learning. In Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning; O’Donnell, A.M., King, A., Eds.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1999; pp. 235–255. [Google Scholar]
  63. Fürst, C. Die Rolle der Lehrkraft im Gruppenunterricht. [Teachers‘ role during group work]. In Gruppenunterricht im Schulalltag. Realität und Chancen; Dann, H.-D., Diegritz, T., Rosenbusch, H.S., Eds.; Universitätsbibliothek Erlangen: Erlangen, Germany, 1999; pp. 107–150. [Google Scholar]
  64. Kapur, M. Productive failure in learning math. Cogn. Sci. 1999 2014, 38, 1008–1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Loibl, K.; Roll, I.; Rummel, N. Towards a theory of when and how problem solving followed by instruction supports learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 29, 693–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Example of second-order Markov chains.
Figure 1. Example of second-order Markov chains.
Education 13 00790 g001
Figure 2. The football globe task [45]. Picture of the football globe was reprinted with permission from Archiv Büro André Heller [46]. 2006, André Heller. Picture of the bronze foot was reprinted with permission from Eckert-Kraft [47]. 2016, Beate Eckert-Kraft.
Figure 2. The football globe task [45]. Picture of the football globe was reprinted with permission from Archiv Büro André Heller [46]. 2006, André Heller. Picture of the bronze foot was reprinted with permission from Eckert-Kraft [47]. 2016, Beate Eckert-Kraft.
Education 13 00790 g002
Figure 3. Overview of the categories to analyze students’ interaction behavior.
Figure 3. Overview of the categories to analyze students’ interaction behavior.
Education 13 00790 g003
Figure 4. Significant transitional probabilities using first-order Markov chains. Rectangles with continuous outlines represent correct, and rectangles with dotted outlines represent incorrect declarations.
Figure 4. Significant transitional probabilities using first-order Markov chains. Rectangles with continuous outlines represent correct, and rectangles with dotted outlines represent incorrect declarations.
Education 13 00790 g004
Figure 5. Significant transitional probabilities using second-order Markov chains. The probabilities for a transition between two interaction events with a correct declaration as the former event are given on the left; the probabilities for a transition between two interaction events with an incorrect declaration as the former event are given on the right. Rectangles with continuous outlines represent correct declarations, and rectangles with dotted outlines incorrect declarations. n.s. = non-significant.
Figure 5. Significant transitional probabilities using second-order Markov chains. The probabilities for a transition between two interaction events with a correct declaration as the former event are given on the left; the probabilities for a transition between two interaction events with an incorrect declaration as the former event are given on the right. Rectangles with continuous outlines represent correct declarations, and rectangles with dotted outlines incorrect declarations. n.s. = non-significant.
Education 13 00790 g005
Table 1. Examples and references of the categories.
Table 1. Examples and references of the categories.
Coding SystemSubcategoriesExampleLiterature
turn/“A football player is about 1.80 m tall.”
“What time is it?”
[49]
task relationtask-related“A football player is about 1.80 m tall.”[50,51,52]
non-task-related“What time is it?”
functionorganization“I’ll go and get my ruler.”[53]
task processing“The door fits seven times.”
reference (no self-references)no reference“I have calculated how often a human person would fit into the football field.”
(new idea)
[20]
referenceS1:“The door fits 8 times.”
S2:“No, 7 times.”
directness of referencedirectS1:“A foot length fits ten times into a human person.”own development
S2:“No, only 6 times.”
delayedS1:“How do you calculate the height of a ball?”
S2:“We should find out how often the door fits into the ball.”
S3:“We need to find out the diameter to calculate the height.”
syntactic classificationinterrogative“How did you calculate this?”[54]
declarative“Firstly, we should find out which additional information we need.”
type of questionread aloudreading/paraphrasing the task[54,55,56]
inquiry“Do you understand?”
non-understanding“I just don’t get it!”
procedure“How should we go on?”
content-specific“Are you sure?”
type of declarationstatement“A door is about 2 m high.”own development
answerS1:“How tall is a normal football player?”
S2:“About 1.90 m.”
agreementS2:“About 1.90 m.”
S3:“Yes, I think so too.”
disagreementS2:“About 1.90 m.”
S4:“No, much smaller! About 1.70 m.”
correctnesscorrect“A football player is about 1.80 m tall.”own development
incorrect“If we take half of the circumference of the ball, we have its height.”
Table 2. Intercoder reliabilities of the categories between the master coder and the two coders.
Table 2. Intercoder reliabilities of the categories between the master coder and the two coders.
Coding SystemPercentage of AgreementCohen’s κ
pair-phase (begin)≥91%/
pair-phase (end)≥86%/
turn≥86%/
task relation≥98%≥0.91
function=100%≥0.86
reference≥93%≥0.72
directness of reference≥95%≥0.72
syntactic classification≥98%≥0.93
type of question≥94%≥0.83
type of declaration≥88%≥0.78
correctness≥89%≥0.88
Table 3. Category combinations used in the analyses.
Table 3. Category combinations used in the analyses.
Correct-NessCodingCategoryAbsolute FrequenciesRelative Frequencies
1correctstatement without referencenew idea2024%
2statement with direct referencepick-up73716%
3answer with direct referenceanswer2124%
4agreement with direct referenceagreement4219%
5disagreement with direct referencedisagreement1884%
total correct176044%
6incorrectstatement without referencenew idea47610%
7statement with direct referencepick-up00%
8answer with direct referenceanswer2756%
9agreement with direct referenceagreement4429%
10disagreement with direct referencedisagreement101721%
total incorrect221056%
total declarations397084%
11 content-specific questionquestion77216%
total4742100%
Table 4. Transitional probabilities of the first-order Markov chains.
Table 4. Transitional probabilities of the first-order Markov chains.
Target Event *
CorrectIncorrect
IdeaPick-UpAnswerAgreeDisagreeIdeaAnswerAgreeDisagreeQuestTotal
Given eventCorrectIdea0.020.230.010.320.010.070.000.030.150.17117
Pick-up0.030.260.000.290.020.030.000.020.170.18522
Answer0.010.230.120.260.050.030.010.010.140.15155
Agree0.060.350.000.230.010.060.000.010.170.11268
Disagree0.020.220.000.260.120.010.000.020.220.13143
IncorrectIdea0.030.070.010.040.080.080.010.300.200.20291
Answer0.010.060.030.010.060.070.090.230.270.18196
Agree0.030.070.000.010.060.090.010.170.350.20269
Disagree0.020.080.000.010.080.060.000.230.280.22715
Quest0.010.110.230.030.020.040.310.040.120.11538
Total724921533661551741943856675463214
* Bold numbers represent transitional probabilities that were significantly higher than expected. Underlined numbers represent transitional probabilities that were significantly lower than expected. Idea = new idea, agree = agreement, disagree = disagreement, quest = question.
Table 5. Transitional probabilities of the second-order Markov chains with correct declarations as the former event.
Table 5. Transitional probabilities of the second-order Markov chains with correct declarations as the former event.
Target Event *
CorrectIncorrect
IdeaPick-UpAnswerAgreeDisagreeIdeaAnswerAgreeDisagreeQuestTotal
Given eventCorrectIdea0.040.300.000.350.040.000.000.000.220.0423
Pick-up0.050.330.000.310.020.030.000.010.110.13236
Answer0.000.230.000.310.080.080.000.000.230.0813
Agree0.060.390.000.210.010.060.000.000.150.12210
Disagree0.000.250.000.210.180.040.000.000.210.1128
IncorrectIdea0.000.130.000.060.130.030.000.350.100.1931
Answer0.000.000.000.000.000.000.000.001.00 10.001
Agree0.000.100.000.000.200.300.000.200.000.2010
Disagree0.020.150.010.020.140.030.010.230.160.23154
Quest0.000.210.340.080.020.010.140.020.090.10133
Total282344615145302054114117839
* Bold numbers represent transitional probabilities that were significantly higher than expected. Underlined numbers represent transitional probabilities that were significantly lower than expected. Idea = new idea, agree = agreement, disagree = disagreement, quest = question. 1 Although incorrect answers were followed in 100% of cases by an incorrect disagreement significantly higher than expected, this result has to be viewed with caution: in total, there was only one event following an incorrect answer after a correct declaration, and this single event was an incorrect disagreement. Therefore, this event sequence is not considered in Figure 5.
Table 6. Transitional probabilities of the second-order Markov chains with incorrect declarations as the former event.
Table 6. Transitional probabilities of the second-order Markov chains with incorrect declarations as the former event.
Target Event *
CorrectIncorrect
IdeaPick-UpAnswerAgreeDisagreeIdeaAnswerAgreeDisagreeQuestTotal
Given eventCorrectIdea0.000.240.000.190.000.140.000.050.240.1421
Pick-up0.020.150.010.220.010.040.000.020.330.1981
Answer0.000.250.250.250.000.000.000.000.130.138
Agree0.070.200.000.270.000.130.000.070.270.0015
Disagree0.020.210.000.250.120.010.000.040.240.1285
IncorrectIdea0.050.080.000.030.080.070.000.300.180.2060
Answer0.000.060.060.000.060.060.000.060.350.3517
Agree0.030.090.000.000.050.100.010.160.370.19208
Disagree0.020.070.000.000.050.090.000.250.330.17297
Quest0.010.090.140.010.010.060.420.040.140.09227
Total231063654447698132811581019
* Bold numbers represent transitional probabilities that were significantly higher than expected. Underlined numbers represent transitional probabilities that were significantly lower than expected. Idea = new idea, agree = agreement, disagree = disagreement, quest = question.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Nemeth, L.; Blumenfeld, T.; Denn, A.-K.; Hirstein, A.; Lipowsky, F. An Exploratory Analysis of Transactive Interaction Patterns in Cooperative Learning Using Sequential Analysis. Educ. Sci. 2023, 13, 790. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci13080790

AMA Style

Nemeth L, Blumenfeld T, Denn A-K, Hirstein A, Lipowsky F. An Exploratory Analysis of Transactive Interaction Patterns in Cooperative Learning Using Sequential Analysis. Education Sciences. 2023; 13(8):790. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci13080790

Chicago/Turabian Style

Nemeth, Lea, Tim Blumenfeld, Ann-Katrin Denn, Anastasia Hirstein, and Frank Lipowsky. 2023. "An Exploratory Analysis of Transactive Interaction Patterns in Cooperative Learning Using Sequential Analysis" Education Sciences 13, no. 8: 790. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci13080790

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop