Reflections on How Adults Respond to Children’s Contributions in Children–Adult Argumentative Interactions
Abstract
1. Introduction
1.1. Defining Argumentation
1.2. Argumentation in Children
1.3. A Model to Observe Argumentation: The Pragma-Dialectical Theory
1.4. Children’s Subdiscussions as Unexpected Contributions to Children–Adult Dialogic Interactions
1.5. Present Study
2. Methods
2.1. Data Source
2.2. Qualitative Analysis
3. Results
3.1. The Adult Supports the Child’s Contribution
- Example 1: “Good guy or bad guy”
{transcript starts at 00:20:25.787}547 VAL: ma eh (-) natha:nbut ehm, Nathan548 (0.91)549 com’è luca secondo te?how is Luca in your opinion?550 (2.19)551 è bravo?is he good?552 NAT? sì.yes553 (0.65)554 BIA: no.=no555 NAT: =no=no556 BIA: =perché ha robato la pecorella (.) a va[lentina]because he stole Valentina’s little sheep557 NAT: [è medio]=he is medium558 RES: <<smiling> è [medio]>he is medium559 NAT: [ma for]se [per-]but maybe for560 VAL: [è me]diohe is medium561 NAT: ma ti ricordi che io ho detto che (.) forse (.) stacreando un peluche più grande?but do you remember that I said that maybe he is making a bigger softtoy?→ 562 VAL: AH magari vuole fare qualcosa di bello.ah! Perhaps he wants to do something nice563 (0.14)→ 564 magari (-) [nel suo gesto di prendere lapecorel]laperhaps in his gesture of taking the little sheep565 NAT: [forse perché è il compleanno.]maybe because it’s the birthday→ 566 VAL: vuol fare una [cosa bella]he wants to do something nice567 NAT: [forse è il suo com]pleanno (.) de lei.maybe it is her birthday…of her568 (0.47)→ 569 VAL: ah:: e ha preso la sua pecorella magari gli fa unvestito bello.ah! And he took her little sheep perhaps he’ll make a nice dress570 (0.12)571 RES: quindi noi non sappia[mo]so we don’t know572 NAT: [no] gli crea un peluche [(grande per il suocom)]pleanno.no he’s creating a big soft toy for her birthday573 VAL: [ascolta l’elisa]listen to Elisa574 (0.11)→ 575 RES: quindi tu (.) quindi tu stai dicendo che noi nonsappiamo perché l’ha rubato magari non c’era perforza un inten[to negativo]so you… so you are saying that we don’t know why he stole it, perhapsthere wasn’t necessarily a bad purpose{transcript ends at 00:21:08.176}
3.2. The Adult Opposes the Child’s Contribution
- Example 2: “Hot or cold”
{transcript starts at 00:05:56.655}159 VAL: per la notte allora il davide (.) piscina e cope=ehpiscina (-) <<laughing> cuscino> e copertaFor the night so Davide, swimmingpool… pillow and blanket160 DAV: due due (-) due coperte perché fa freddi:ssimo:two two, two blankets because it is very cold161 (0.78)162 RES: quindi sì okaySo yes okay163 (0.28)164 VAL: allora la co=la coperta diciamo UNA ma be:llaca:lda.So the bl-the blanket let’s say ONE but super warm165 NAT: io=io porto:: io=io:: porto il costume (così) miposso fare il bagno nell’i(sola)I-I’ll bring, I-I’ll bring the swimming suit so I can go swimming on theisland166 (0.30)167 DAV: (ma [se fa) freddo.]But it is cold168 VAL: [un bel costu]mino (-) un bel (costume.)A nice small swimming suit, a nice swimming suit169 (0.03)170 GAI: [anch’io:]Me too→ 171 DAV: [fa freddo]It’s cold172 VAL: nella sua TEsta nell’isola si sta [al caldo].In his head on the island it is warm173 GAI: [anch’io] mi porto (---) (xxx) costume da ba:gno:Me too I’m bringing the swimming suit174 VAL: anche tu porti il costume da bagno?You bring the swimming suit too?175 DAV: anch’io allora lo porto.I’ll also bring it then{transcript ends at 00:06:34.905}
3.3. The Adult Opens a Subdiscussion and Puts into Question the Child’s Contribution
- Example 3: “The magic wand”
{transcript starts at 00:17:02.002}468 FED: do:po quando la principessa (2.0) è andata qua e (.)e dopo l’ha messo=l’ha=l’ha fatto con la sua magiacon la bacchetta magica (.) e dopo l’ha trasformatoeh: l’acqua lì (unintelligible, appr 3 sec) e doposi è trasformato in PRIncipeThen when the princess…she went here and, and then she put, she-shedid with her magic with the magic wand, and then she transformed ituhm the water there … and then it turned into a prince.469 VAL: dimmi solo una cosa (-) in questa storia (.) sieted’accordo?Tell me only one thing, in this story… do you agree?470 NAT: no:No471 VAL: perché?Why?472 NAT: perché non (.) non si vede che ha la bacchettamagica.Because you don’t, you don’t see that she has the magic wand.473 VAL: <<nodding> mh_mh>Mh_mh474 FED: ma tanto è invisibileBut anyway it’s invisible→ 475 VAL: è invisibile?It’s invisible?476 NAT: non esiste la bacchetta invisibileIt does not exist the invisible wand477 FED: [si]Yes→ 478 VAL: [l’hai] già vista ?Have you already seen it?479 DAV: si può creare nathan (.) si può anche creAre.It can be created Nathan, it can also be created.480 NAT: si però non (.) ma per me (.) per me (.) per mel’inizio è quando ha bussato la porta la ranocchia.Yes but not, but for me, for me, for me the beginning is when itknocked on the door the frog{transcript ends at 00:17:46.012}
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
1 | We do not consider the notion of persuasion when we talk about the definition of argumentation, if we look at argumentation in children. |
2 | A more detailed review of the literature can be found in Miserez-Caperos (2017). |
3 | When we use the term device, we refer to the design and the implementation of a pedagogical setting aimed at fostering argumentative discourse in the classroom. |
4 | For further references and examples of argumentation structure, see van Eemeren et al. (2002). |
5 | |
6 | The issue is the question around which a discussion revolves, it is the object of the discussion (cf. Goodwin, 2002; Schär, 2018). |
7 | OPI stands for “Operatore pedagogico per l’integrazione” (transl. Pedagogical operator for integration). This figure is present in the educational system in Ticino and it consists of a specialized supporting teacher, whose main objective is to promote the adoption and application of dispensatory and/or compensatory measures necessary for the implementation of the pedagogical project for students with special educational needs (see https://www4.ti.ch/decs/ds/sps/chi-siamo/servizio-pedagogico-per-lintegrazione (accessed on 23 May 2024). |
8 | The researcher served as both researcher and interlocutor in the kindergarten discussions. As Brasseur claims, “the interaction of the researcher with the fieldwork has an impact on the fieldwork” (Brasseur, 2012, p. 107, our translation). As the researcher was present during the video recordings, it happened that some children would address her and include her in the discussion. Realizing that she was not a neutral observer, the researcher decided to participate in the kindergarten activities with the children as an adult interlocutor. She listened to the teacher’s, the educators’, the children’s comments, and she smiled, nodded, and reacted according to what she heard. She asked questions if something was not clear to her, she answered questions when she was called on. It is important to remark that the researcher was not the designer of the activities, nor did she ever lead a discussion. Hence, the position of the researcher was that of a young adult who was willing to play with the children and who was a potential interlocutor during the discussions. She never took initiatives and with the children she always pointed at the teacher and the educators as the authorities in the classroom. This type of participation was not particularly problematic for the study; the researcher’s turns in the transcripts have been analyzed as adult’s contributions. |
9 | We propose an adapted version of the analytic overview by van Eemeren and Grootendorst, following the one developed by Schär’s (2018) research. This author included the issue in her analytic overview, since her objective was to study how argumentative discussions between children and adults came to life, so she needed to recognize in her data the opening of issues. We add the indication of the sub-issue (cf. Angiolini, 2024) since we are interested in distinguishing the main discussion from the subdiscussion opened by a child. |
10 | Cf. the difference between mixed and non-mixed dispute in van Eemeren and Grootendorst (1984). |
References
- Alexander, R. (2008). Dialectic teaching: Rethinking classroom talk. Education Review, 59(2), 5–13. [Google Scholar]
- Andriessen, J. (2009). Argumentation in higher education: Examples of actual practices with argumentation tools. In N. Muller Mirza, & A.-N. Perret-Clermont (Eds.), Argumentation and education (pp. 195–213). Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Angiolini, E. (2024). An interdisciplinary study of subdiscussions in children-adult argumentative interactions [Ph.D. thesis, Université de Neuchâtel]. [Google Scholar]
- Baucal, A., & Stepanovic, I. (2006). Conservation or conversation: A test of the repeated question hypothesis. Psihologija, 39(3), 257–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berkowitz, M. W., Oser, F., & Althoff, W. (1987). The development of sociomoral discourse. In W. Kurtines, & J. Gewirtz (Eds.), Moral development throught social interaction (pp. 322–352). John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Brasseur, M. (2012). L’interaction du chercheur avec son terrain en recherche-action: Deux cas d’accompagnement individuel des managers. Recherches en Sciences de Gestion, 2, 103–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bubikova-Moan, J., & Sandvik, M. (2023). Argumentation in early childhood: A systematic review. Human Development, 66(6), 397–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eisenberg, A. R., & Garvey, C. (1981). Children’s use of verbal strategies in resolving conflicts. Discourse Processes, 4, 149–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Golder, C. (1993). Savez-vous argumenter à la mode… à la mode des petits? Enfance, 46(4), 359–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Golder, C. (1996). Le développement des discours argumentatifs. Delachaux & Niestlé. [Google Scholar]
- Golder, C., & Favart, M. (2003). Argumenter c’est difficile…oui, mais pourquoi? Approche psycholinguistique de la production argumentative en situation écrite. Ela. Etudes de Linguistique Appliquée, 2(130), 187–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodwin, J. (2002). Designing issues. In F. H. van Eemeren, & P. Houtlosser (Eds.), Dialectic and rhetoric: The warp and woof of argumentation analysis (pp. 81–96). Kluwer Publications. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greco, S., & Perret-Clermont, A.-N. (2022). Young Children’s Argumentative Contributions. In J. Fahnestock, & R. A. Harris (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of language and persuasion (pp. 457–474). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn, D. (1991). The skills of argument. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn, D., Shaw, V., & Felton, M. (1997). Effects of dyadic interaction on argumentative reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 15(3), 287–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, D., & Udell, W. (2003). The development of argument skills. Child Development, 74(5), 1245–1260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuhn, D., & Udell, W. (2007). Coordinating own and other perspectives in argument. Thinking & Reasoning, 13(2), 90–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mehmeti, T., & Perret-Clermont, A.-N. (2016). Seeking success of migrant students through designed tasks: A case study with Albanian students in Switzerland. In A. Surian (Ed.), Open spaces for interactions and learning diversities (pp. 137–150). Sense Publishers. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/265384385_Seeking_success_of_migrant_pupils_through_designed_tasks_A_case_study_with_Albanian_pupils_in_Switzerland_Presented_at_A-N_Perret-Clermont_T_Mehmeti_chair_Social_cultural_and_psychological_affordances (accessed on 16 May 2025).
- Mercer, N. (2000). Words and minds: How we use language to think together. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Mercer, N., & Littleton, K. (2007). Dialogue and the development of children’s thinking: A socio-cultural approach. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Meyer, J. (1992). The collaborative development of power in children’s arguments. Argumentation and Advocacy, 29(2), 77–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miserez-Caperos, C. (2017). Étude de l’argumentation à visée cognitive dans des interactions entre adulte et enfants: Un regard psychosocial sur le modèle pragma-dialectique [Ph.D. thesis, Université de Neuchâtel]. [Google Scholar]
- Orange, C., Lhoste, Y., & Orange-Ravachol, D. (2008). Argumentation, problématisation et construction de concepts en classe de sciences. In C. Buty, & C. Plantin (Eds.), Argumenter en classe de sciences. Du débat à l’apprentissage. INRP. [Google Scholar]
- Orsolini, M. (1993). Dwarfs do not shoot: An analysis of children’s justifications. Cognition and Instruction, 11, 281–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Orsolini, M., & Pontecorvo, C. (1992). Children’s talk in classroom discussion. Cognition and Instruction, 9(2), 113–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perret-Clermont, A.-N., Arcidiacono, F., Breux, S., Greco, S., & Miserez-Caperos, C. (2015). Knowledge-oriented argumentation in children. Proceedings of the 8th ISSA conference (pp. 135–149). John Benjamins Publishing Compagny. [Google Scholar]
- Perret-Clermont, A.-N., Breux, S., Greco Morasso, S., & Miserez-Caperos, C. (2014). Children and knowledge-oriented argumentation. Some notes for future research. In G. Gobber, & A. Rocci (Eds.), Language, reason and education. Studies in honor of Eddo Rigotti (pp. 259–277). Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
- Perret-Clermont, A.-N., & Carugati, F. (2004). Des psychologues sociaux étudient l’apprentissage. In G. Chatelanat, C. Moro, & M. Saada-Robert (Eds.), Unité et pluralité des sciences de l’éducation (pp. 159–183). Peter Lang Group AG. [Google Scholar]
- Perret-Clermont, A.-N., Schär, R., Greco, S., Convertini, J., Iannaccone, A., & Rocci, A. (2019). Shifting from a monological to a dialogical perspective on children’s argumentation. Lessons learned. In F. H. van Eemeren, & B. Garssen (Eds.), Argumentation in actual practice. Topical studies about argumentative discourse in context (pp. 211–236). John Benjamins Publishing Company. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perret-Clermont, A.-N., Schubauer-Leoni, M. L., & Grossen, M. (1991). Interactions sociales dans le développement cognitif: Nouvelles directions de recherche. Cahiers de Psychologie, 29, 17–39. [Google Scholar]
- Pontecorvo, C., & Arcidiacono, F. (2010). Development of reasoning through arguing in young children. Cultural-Historical Psychology, 6(4), 19–29. [Google Scholar]
- Pramling, N., & Säljö, R. (2014). Reasoning about evolution: Metaphors in teacher students’ rendering of darwinian ideas. In T. Zittoun, & A. Iannaccone (Eds.), Activities of thinking in social spaces (pp. 99–120). Nova Science. [Google Scholar]
- Rapanta, C., Garcia-Mila, M., & Gilabert, S. (2013). What is meant by argumentative competence? An integrative review of methods of analysis and assessment in education. Review of Educational Research, 83(4), 483–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rigotti, E., & Greco Morasso, S. (2009). Argumentation as an object of interest and as a social and cultural resource. In N. Muller Mirza, & A.-N. Perret-Clermont (Eds.), Argumentation and education (pp. 9–66). Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Schär, R. G. (2018). An argumentative analysis of the emergence of issues in adult-children discussions [Ph.D. dissertation, Università della Svizzera Italiana]. [Google Scholar]
- Schär, R. G. (2021). An argumentative analysis of the emergence of issues in adult-children discussions. John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Schär, R., & Greco, S. (2018). The emergence of issues in everyday discussions between adults and children. International Journal of Semiotics and Visual Rhetoric, 2(1), 29–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwarz, B. B. (2009). Argumentation and learning. In N. Muller Mirza, & A.-N. Perret-Clermont (Eds.), Argumentation and education (pp. 91–126). Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz, B. B., & Baker, M. (2017). Dialogue, argumentation and education: History, theory and practice. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Selting, M., Auer, P., & Barth-Weingarten, D. (2011). A system for transcribing talk-in-interaction: GAT 2. Gesprächsforschung: Online-Zeitschrift zur verbalen Interaktion, 12(2), 1–51. [Google Scholar]
- Stein, N. L., & Albro, E. R. (2001). The origins and nature of arguments: Studies in conflict understanding, emotion, and negociation. Discourse Processes, 32(2–3), 113–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stein, N. L., & Miller, C. A. (1993). The development of memory and reasoning skill in argumentative contexts: Evaluating, explaining, and generating evidence. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (pp. 285–335). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- van Eemeren, F. H. (2009). Examining argumentation in context: Fifteen studies on strategic maneuvering. John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- van Eemeren, F. H. (2018). Argumentation theory: A pragma-dialectical perspective. Springer. [Google Scholar]
- van Eemeren, F. H., & Garssen, B. (2012). Exploring argumentative contexts. John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- van Eemeren, F. H., & Grootendorst, R. (1984). Speech acts in argumentative discussions. Foris. [Google Scholar]
- van Eemeren, F. H., & Grootendorst, R. (2004). A Systematic Theory of Argumentation: The pragma-dialectical approach. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- van Eemeren, F., Grootendorst, R., & Snoeck Henkemans, F. (2002). Argumentation. Analysis, evaluation, presentation. L. Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Völzing, P.-L. (1981). Kinder argumentieren. Die ontogenese argumentativer fähigkeiten. Schöningh. [Google Scholar]
- Wells, G. (1999). Dialogic inquiry: Toward a sociocultural practice and theory of education. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Zadunaisky Ehrlich, S., & Blum-Kulka, S. (2010). Peer talk as a “double opportunity space”: The case of argumentative discourse. Discourse & Society, 21(2), 211–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Angiolini, E.; Miserez-Caperos, C. Reflections on How Adults Respond to Children’s Contributions in Children–Adult Argumentative Interactions. Educ. Sci. 2025, 15, 1069. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15081069
Angiolini E, Miserez-Caperos C. Reflections on How Adults Respond to Children’s Contributions in Children–Adult Argumentative Interactions. Education Sciences. 2025; 15(8):1069. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15081069
Chicago/Turabian StyleAngiolini, Elisa, and Céline Miserez-Caperos. 2025. "Reflections on How Adults Respond to Children’s Contributions in Children–Adult Argumentative Interactions" Education Sciences 15, no. 8: 1069. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15081069
APA StyleAngiolini, E., & Miserez-Caperos, C. (2025). Reflections on How Adults Respond to Children’s Contributions in Children–Adult Argumentative Interactions. Education Sciences, 15(8), 1069. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci15081069