2.1. Bodily Misperceptions in Mental Disorders
In the run-up to an illness, when one is unsure whether he or she is still healthy or already sick, many people occasionally experience strange body changes. Physical misperceptions caused by fever, including fever hallucinations, are known to occur. Fever is induced when pro-inflammatory cytokines trigger prostaglandin E2 synthesis by binding to receptors on brain endothelial cells [
10]. Fever affects several neurotransmitters, Cox and Lee specifically listed norepinephrine, 5-hydroxytryptamine and acetylcholine, which in turn can severely disrupt the body’s recognition [
11].
Delusions of fever, i.e., a form of hallucination during severe infectious diseases, are often described in the literature in children. According to Eggers [
12], about 30% of all schizophrenic children suffer from bodily hallucinations or coenaesthesia. Eggers described a 7-year-old girl who experienced the sensation of a snake in her stomach. Additionally, a 9-year-old boy reported feeling stones moving back and forth inside his head. Another child said, “It feels like smoke is moving through my body”. An 11-year-old boy said that he felt his head getting longer and bigger in size.
Changes in body perception are most often described in psychoses, primarily schizophrenia. Here is an example: The patient had been raised in a home where her father had sexually abused and impregnated one of her sisters. In her adolescence, she was then raped by a stranger. At 23, she married a man who had sexual intercourse with her up to five times a day. She then developed delusions and hallucinations and had to be hospitalised several times. During the exploration, she also reported seeing her deceased husband: “When I sleep at night, I suddenly wake up because I feel a stitch on my upper arm or leg. Then, when I wake up, there is a man next to my bed”. The patient stated that her husband stood very still about 1 metre away from the bed and said nothing. She always recognised his face exactly. He wore a long coat that reached down to the floor so that she could not see his feet. She had often spoken to him and called his name because he stuck her in the leg or arm with a needle. But he did not give any answer and always disappeared again abruptly [
5].
What is astonishing about schizophrenia is that the sufferers, despite the severity of the symptoms, usually have hardly any insight into the illness. Often, they consider themselves to be completely mentally healthy and are absolutely convinced that all their problems are caused by others.
Another example was written to us by a 17-year-old student: “I see, feel, hear and sometimes smell things that are not there and this is all day, every second and this is not an exaggeration. I know they are hallucinations because they are not real, but I perceive them at any time I am conscious. I can’t stop it. To give an example: I see skeletons walking around, looking at me and even walking through me. I feel that too, I imagine it. It’s like a third eye with which I perceive these hallucinations, I just see it. Or I can feel it very clearly in my body, even now at this moment. I can still distinguish very well what is real and what is not. Nevertheless, these hallucinations, especially when they started and became ingrained in me, have been extremely hard on me. It is difficult for me to concentrate on something, like reading a book, because these hallucinations are always there. When I read a book, they become even stronger and more intense. It takes an enormous amount of strength for me to still manage everyday life with school in this way”. Having the feeling that a skeleton is wandering through one’s own body is not likely to be very pleasant, but there are clearly worse forms of body perception in psychoses.
Another example from the pool of our personal experiences tells of M., a young man who was not averse to drugs, but in whom a drug-induced psychosis was ultimately triggered: M. called the police. When they arrived at his house, they found 15 g of hashish lying in the middle of the table. M. did not care at all that his entire cannabis stash was to be confiscated. He shouted at the police officers that they should first note down the damage caused by the insects. After the police officers confirmed that there were no insects present, they advised him to lie down in his bed for a while and assured him that tomorrow the reality would look different. M. then ran himself a hot bath to relax. The result was that he discovered a large tapeworm crawling out of his anus in the bath. He screamed like mad and ran naked, dripping with water, out of his flat, down the corridor and out of the house onto the open street, where he shouted warningly at people, “The insects are coming!”; a short time later, he found himself in an emergency ambulance, which again took him to the psychiatric ward.
Möller described in detail the fate of a 19-year-old girl who suffered from catatonic schizophrenia [
5]. She herself was a premature baby. Her father died when she was still young, and her mother had since remarried for the third time. As a child, she was weak, unsociable and plagued by severe feelings of inferiority. Later, she worked in the garment industry, but always thought she heard her colleagues making fun of her. She reported having sexual intercourse with her cousin, which later weighed heavily on her. Then she became obsessed with the feeling that she was being blamed by television and radio. One day, according to the file, she suddenly became completely stiff, her head fell to the side and her shoulders and eye area were completely cramped. Afterwards, she believed she was possessed by the devil and thought they wanted to burn her. Epilepsy and other neurological diseases could be ruled out, and she was classified as a catatonia-schizophrenic. In the clinic, she was very sexually uninhibited and asked anyone who came towards her to touch her genitals. She alternately believed that she was the Archangel Gabriel and then again that she had to go to hell. Finally, she was convinced that she was having a child and even felt its movements in her belly. At the same time, she saw angels and devils flying through the air. Once she threw a towel out of the window, convinced it was a snake that had bitten her. The Pope also appeared to her, wearing a long brown robe, a red bishop’s mitre and holding a white book in his hand from which he seemed to be reading. Only his face was somewhat blurred, so that she did not know whether he was alive.
Schizophrenia is a typical illness, but false sensations of the body also occur in many other mental illnesses. Anorexia nervosa, too, is ultimately based on a calculation error of the brain, according to today’s assessment. Suchan et al. showed that there are differences in the extrastriate body area between healthy women and those suffering from anorexia nervosa [
13]. Interestingly, this difference decreases measurably after therapy. It is assumed that a specific area of the brain provides false information in this case; no matter how thin a person has become, this area stubbornly reports back that one is still too fat.
People who suffer from gender incongruence, i.e., transgender/transidentity, have a different problem. The appearance, especially of the gender-typical areas of the outer body, does not match the mental gender representation. Psychologically, someone is a woman, but the body is that of a man or vice versa.
Body Integrity Dysphoria or Body Integrity Identity Disorder is another strange disorder from this group of forms. The outer body of these patients is usually completely intact, but they have the urge to want to be disabled. They do feel complete only when they have achieved a specific disability, e.g., the amputation of a leg [
14].
Another disorder from this group of forms, which we will only briefly mention by name here, is Body Dysmorphic Disorder, in which the persons concerned get carried away with the feeling that certain body parts look ugly; often a surgical change is sought, although the respective body part corresponds to the average in every respect.
2.2. Bodily Misperceptions Caused by Drugs
A large number of body hallucinations can be directly caused by drugs. Albert Hofmann, the inventor of LSD, has already been quoted above: “I myself had small, finely formed hands. When I washed them, it happened far away from me, somewhere in the lower right. It was questionable, but completely immaterial, whether they were my hands at all. [...] I once felt like a figure in surrealist paintings whose limbs are not connected to the body, but are just painted next to it.” [
8] (see
Figure 2).
A young man who had used LSD and cannabis experienced something similar: “At this place we then smoked the weed, and not ten minutes later an effect completely untypical for marijuana began. It was unbelievably strong, far too strong to bear with a clear conscience. I told myself that the effect was only from the weed and that it would stop pretty soon. But then I looked at my palm and the individual fingers bent, became longer and shorter; my whole hand was wobbling. Then, unfortunately, I had to admit to myself that the effect came from the LSD after all. I thought, oh no, not here, not now of all times. I panicked, took a booklet on drugs and opened the page with the LSD description. Techno music was playing in the background, sounding almost like a piano. The lines of the book seemed to me like black and white piano keys, pressed low in rhythm with the music to produce the sound I was hearing, and then came back to me. They became completely detached from the book, so that I finally put it away. It was the same with my mouth when I wanted to speak, my ears, my sense of touch, my legs and my sense of taste, which made it difficult for me to use any of these senses consciously because it seemed complicated to reach a certain one through the tangle of sensory functions in order to be able to act with it. I gave my body an attempt at a command from the subconscious and without actual control over what legs would do, they just went off in the direction indicated without giving any feedback as to what exactly they were doing or to what extent I would then have control over it. Each of these organs only functioned individually and uncontrollably. When I said something, it seemed as if my mouth was saying something and I was just a spectator of it. It was the same with my actions. My legs walked all by themselves. I had no real control over it at all anymore, because the sense of moving my legs was attached to one of the prongs, and was so far away that I had a hard time getting to it”.
Under the effect of drugs, one can also become a completely disembodied being. In the book Lucifer’s Garden of Light by Olaf Kraemer [
15], one can read the example of a young man who experimented with a drug that was unknown to him until then. He had placed a few milligrams of Salvia divinorum on an aluminium foil and heated it. He inhaled the rising vapours in a self-experiment, which was at the same time intended to find the dose, and had already come to the conclusion that his experiment would have no effect when he realised that something terrible had obviously happened, something had gone wrong: “In great despair I tried to find my way back to the real world, searching my memories for details about the living room I had just been in. I tried to remember what my body felt like. But the more I searched for a thread that would reconnect me with the world I was familiar with, the more violently the extract tried to show me something else. I found that the reality I wanted to return to did not exist. It was merely an ephemeral dream. I noticed that I had no directed access to a memory of any state other than my present, disembodied one…”.
After inhaling deodorant propellant, a female student wrote the following experience: “A wall full of children’s photos of me, right behind the teacher, I stare at the pictures waiting for someone to laugh at the photos, that’s when they melt into the blackboard and the wall, some still seem to call out to me as they dissipate. Black rags fly from the ceiling, no one seems to see them, graveyard bells ring, I get up, mumble something and leave the room, rather: my body leaves the room. I watch myself do it”.
Another youngster who was keen to experiment once smoked some DMT (dimethyltryptamine) extract by mistake, thinking it was hashish. The hallucinations, he wrote to me, were comparable to LSD, only much stronger: “I lay on the bed and felt completely disembodied, something like the body wasn’t really there anymore. Getting up to walk around the environment was unthinkable. How could I, without a body and especially without legs to carry me? The intoxication only lasted for about an hour. There is not much more to tell of this intoxication than that I was completely incapacitated and absolutely disembodied. I have never heard of a case where a user got up after taking DMT. In all the examples I know of, the users had to lie down and it was only after an hour that they were responsive enough to get up again”.
Cashman reported LSD experiences as early as 1966: “My body melted away in waves, [...] I felt myself flying out into space, without heaviness or restraints, freed to bathe in the blissful glow of heavenly apparition. [...] There was no time, no place, no self. There was only cosmic harmony. [...] For me, the realities of our limited existence were no longer valid. I had seen the ultimate truths, and no others would be able to stand before them. [...] I felt myself flying out into space, without heaviness and without fetters, freed to bathe in the blissful splendour of heavenly appearance.” [
16].
2.3. Bodily Misperceptions Due to Neurological Damage
In Alexander Lurija’s book The Man With a Shattered World, the brain-injured soldier Zasetzki also reported a wealth of changes in his own body schema: “Sometimes I sit there and suddenly feel that my head is as big as a table, at least as big. But arms and legs and torso have become tiny. It seems strange and ridiculous to myself when I suddenly remember it! I call these phenomena peculiarities of the body! And when I close my eyes I don’t even know where my right leg is, and for some reason it has always seemed (and been felt by me) as if it were somewhere above the shoulders and even above the head.” [
17].
Ultimately, all misperceptions have a neurological basis. Logically, processing errors in the sensory cortex come into question first, because this is where the perception and initial processing of all stimuli from the body occur. However, this is by far not the only brain area, more it is a matter of a disturbance in a network that consists of very different parts.
Using fMRI techniques, in 2001, Downing et al. found an area in the right lateral cortex which gave a stronger response when subjects viewed images of human bodies [
18]. Downing named this field “extrastriate body area” Four years later, Peelen and Downing found a second body-selective area in the middle fusiform gyrus [
19]. This fusiform body area responds selectively to images of human bodies. The extrastriate and the fusiform body area seem to be sensitive to bodily actions expressing emotions, such as anger, disgust, happiness and fear.
In 2010, Berlucci and Aglioti [
3] pointed out that “In the nineteenth century, neurological thinking about the means by which the body communicates with the brain emphasized the importance of the concept of coenaesthesia, a mainly unconscious sense of the normal functioning of the body and its organs which emerges to full consciousness only when one is unwell”. Today, this concept is renamed as interoception, which refers to the inner perception of one’s own bodily processes (e.g., hunger, muscular sensations, pain, temperature (fever), thirst and visceral sensations in the guts). It works together with exteroception and proprioception: Exteroception refers to the perception of the environment through sense organs, while proprioception is the unconscious perception of one’s own movement, position, tension, posture and position in space.
In 2009, Craig pointed out that interoception, proprioception and exteroception feed the brain with information about the condition of the body [
20]. The cortical representation is mainly settled in the insula. According to Craig, the insular cortex has sensory inputs (e.g., gustatory, somatosensory, vestibular and visceral) that are integrated across modalities and are closely connected to the anterior cingulate cortex. They form an emotional network with which sensory reception is linked to conscious feelings and motivations [
20]. Self-recognition is also attributed to this network in conjunction with the default network system. Craig wrote in 2009 that the anterior insular cortex is responsible for the integration of all bodily feelings and, when disturbed, can result in errors of body belonging, such as hemiplegia with anosognosia, neglect, body integrity dysphoria, autoscopy or out-of-body experiences and “astral travel” [
20].
Autoscopy and heautoscopic hallucinations are not necessarily physically noticeable changes, but one sees oneself from the outside. According to Goldenberg, autoscopic phenomena are associated with temporo-occipital rather than parietal lesions. Usually, they are short-lived and often associated with epileptic seizures originating in the temporal lobes [
21].
Phantom feelings are another example of how the interaction between the brain and body no longer works. Ramachandran [
22] described a motorcyclist who had lost an arm in an accident. He could extend his phantom arm, wave it in the air, touch things and even has the feeling he could grasp objects with it. Phantom sensations do not only occur for lost extremities; Ramachandran described a female patient who after mastectomy felt phantom breasts, and another man who experienced phantom erections following the removal of his penis.
Most amputees have feelings of a phantom limb, i.e., the missing limb is still there. However, it often feels shorter than the original healthy part of the body or feels like it is in a distorted or even painful position. For example, amputees may feel itching or a twitch in the non-existent body part. Some try to stand up with a leg that is no longer there, and others try to grab things with the amputated arm. This is due to the fact that the areas of the brain that were used for creating the sensation of the missing body part remain intact even after an amputation. In the 1980s, Melzack postulated that the experience of the body arises from a network of interconnected neuronal structures which he called the “neuromatrix” [
17]. In addition to the primary sensory cortex in the parietal lobe, an influence of the thalamus is discussed here in particular. After an amputation, however, there is a restructuring, since neurons that no longer receive input search for new tasks. Some regions of the thalamus that originally represented the missing limb remain functional, while other thalamic neurons begin to respond to stimulation in other regions of the body [
23] In addition, Melzack recognized that many people who were born without definite limbs also had phantom limbs [
24].
In 2000, Brugger and co-authors described a woman born without forearms and legs who described vivid phantom sensations [
25]. An fMRI study showed that “movements” of the non-existent body parts did not activate primary sensorimotor areas but rather the premotor and parietal cortex. Such findings show that parts of the body that never existed in the child’s development can nevertheless be anchored in the brain. Possibly, the observation of the movements of other people is added, so that these brain areas do not turn to other tasks.
The problem for those affected is that the corresponding parts of the brain claim that the amputated body part is still there, but the eyes show the opposite. The idea of being able to grasp an object with a phantom hand also does not mean that this object is now being felt. Sooner or later, this brings those affected to a realistic perception of their phantom feelings.
Another example of strange changes is Alice in Wonderland syndrome. Here, Bittmann and co-authors [
26] describe the statements of a patient as follows: “The people who talked to me sounded like they were talking very fast. I had the feeling of being upside down. I was with my grandmother and on her red sofa. Later I found out that I was not there, but in my own house. (...) In the evening, lying down in front of the television: Visual perception was like looking through the wrong end of binoculars. Everything was pushed far away”.
Typical of the syndrome is a change in proportions; what is close appears distant, whereas what is far away seems close enough to touch. This also applies to the body, e.g., the head, arms or legs are perceived as disproportionately huge, and often the ground under the feet feels soft. In addition, there is often a loss of orientation, as well as fear and the feeling of “going crazy”. Usually, the sense of time also changes. Causes include migraine attacks, brain damage, drugs or febrile illnesses.
Alien hand syndrome, also called alien limb syndrome (as it can affect all limbs), is usually due to the right and left hemispheres of the brain, which normally exchange information via the corpus callosum, having communication problems, resulting in the left hemisphere of the brain no longer knowing what the right is doing. This leads to one hand making independent movements that are often untargeted, interfere with the activity of the other hand or may even strike the owner of that hand.
In the bestseller “The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat”, Oliver Sacks vividly described the behaviour of a sufferer: “When I asked him what happened at night, he told me straightforwardly that he always found a dead, cold, hairy leg in his bed when he woke up at night. He could not explain where the dead leg came from and would therefore try to push it out of bed with his healthy arm. But it would somehow stick to his body and he could not get it off. Every time he managed to push the leg out of the bed, he would fall behind. In his view, it would be a bad joke by the hospital staff, who would put an amputated human leg in his bed night after night.” [
27].
Somatoparaphrenia is a disorder, most often neurological, in which patients actively deny that a particular limb is part of their own body. If you bring evidence, then there are pseudo-justifications as to why it cannot be your own body part. Sometimes these symptoms take on delusional proportions, and the arm or leg can be treated like a strange being [
28,
29]. Somatoparaphenia differs from asomatognosia, in which there is a passive loss of recognition of one’s own body parts. It is usually caused by a lesion in a network that appears to include the temporo-parietal junction, posterior insula, basal ganglia and thalamo-cortical connections, among others. There may be correlations with Capgras syndrome, in which a previously familiar person suddenly appears strange after a temporal lesion. In the case of somatoparaphrenia, a previously familiar part of the body also appears strange and does not belong to oneself. Parallels have also been made to Body Integrity Dysphoria (Body Identity Integrity Disorder, Amputee Identity Disorder); in this condition, those affected feel the need to amputate a part of the body that is also perceived as not belonging to their own body. However, sufferers neither show a serious neurological brain lesion nor do they deny that the affected body part is their own. Likewise, delusional justifications are not usually presented in the case of BID sufferers, but they are rationally aware of the pros and cons of an amputation [
14].
Cotard’s syndrome is even more drastic; in this condition, the patient claims to be dead, smelling his decaying flesh and feeling maggots gnawing and crawling all over him. The sufferers prefer to stay in cemeteries, since they are actually already dead; many refuse to eat. Cotard’s syndrome is a rare condition that has not shown clear neurological damage thus far, and it often occurs in cases of severe depression.