Next Article in Journal
Influence of Physical Activity, Physical Fitness, Age, Biological Maturity and Anthropometric Variables on the Probability of Suffering Lumbar, Neck and Shoulder Pain in Spanish Adolescents from the Region of Murcia
Previous Article in Journal
Effect of Insulin Pen Training Using the Teach-Back Method on Diabetes Self-Management, Quality of Life, and HbA1c Levels in Older Patients with Type 2 Diabetes: A Quasi-Experimental Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Case Report

Strategies to Improve Bladder Control: A Preliminary Case Study

1
Department of Philosophy, Sociology, Education and Applied Psychology, University of Padova, 35131 Padova, Italy
2
Centre for Mechanics of Biological Materials (CMBM), University of Padova, 35131 Padova, Italy
3
Urology Unit, Spinal Unit ORAS, Motta di Livenza, 31045 Treviso, Italy
4
Smell & Taste Clinic, Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University of Dresden, 01307 Dresden, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Healthcare 2024, 12(18), 1855; https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12181855
Submission received: 22 August 2024 / Revised: 13 September 2024 / Accepted: 13 September 2024 / Published: 15 September 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Health Assessments)

Abstract

:
Background: Lower urinary tract symptoms (LUTSs) are a common complaint in adult and elderly men with bladder outlet obstruction, and have a considerable impact on their quality of life. Symptoms affect storage, voiding and post micturition stages. Among the latter, a feeling of incomplete emptying is one of the most bothersome for the patients; a condition that in turn contributes to affect urinary urgency, nocturia and frequency. Common recommendations include self-management practices (e.g., control of fluid intake, double-voiding and distraction techniques) to relieve patients’ symptoms, whose effectiveness, however, is under debate. Methods: In this report we describe two pioneering procedures to favor bladder residual content voiding in people complaining of LUTS disorders. The first is based on motor imagery and the second on the use of odors. The beneficial effects of Mental imagery techniques on various tasks (e.g., in the treatment of several pathological conditions or as valid mnemonics aids have a long tradition and have received consistently experimental support. Thus, a patient (a 68-year-old Caucasian man) complaining of LUTS was trained to use a motor imagery technique (building up a visual image comprising the bladder, the detrusor muscle and the urethra, and to imagine the detrusor muscle contracting and the flow of urine expelled) for 90 days and two odors (coffee and a lavender scented cleanser) for 10 days, as a trigger for micturition. He was asked to record—immediately after the first morning micturition—the time interval between the first (free) and the second (cued) micturition. Results: Reported data suggest the efficacy of motor imagery in favoring the bladder residual urine voiding in a few minutes (M = 4.75 min.) compared to the control condition, i.e., the baseline of the patient (M = 79.5 min.), while no differences between the odor-based procedures (M 1st odorant = 70.6 min.; M 2nd odorant = 71.1 min) and the latter were observed. Conclusions: A procedure based on an imagery technique may, therefore, be of general value—as a suggested protocol—and accordingly can be applicable to clinical settings. An olfactory bladder control hypothesis cannot, however, be ruled out and is discussed as a promising future line of research.

1. General Introduction

According to the European urology association (EAU) “lower urinary tract symptoms (LUTS)” are a common complaint in adult men with a major impact on quality of life and have a substantial economic burden” [1]. LUTSs are symptoms in the storage (e.g., increased frequency, urgency, nocturia) and voiding (e.g., slow stream, straining, intermittency) phase, as well as in post-micturition (e.g., feeling of incomplete emptying, dribbling) and can be related to obstruction during voiding or to bladder dysfunctions such as bladder over- or under-activity. Therefore, the management of male LUTSs is tailored according to the type of LUTS, considering underlying mechanisms and individual patient preference.
The therapeutic options range from herbal remedies to drugs (i.e., alpha blockers to relax the smooth muscle of the prostate, 5-alpha-reductase inhibitors to shrink the prostate, phosphodiesterase-5 inhibitors and antimuscarinics or beta-3 adrenoceptors to relax the bladder) and even surgical interventions.
However, in men with uncomplicated LUTSs, clinicians routinely suggest self-management. This includes lifestyle advice, such as control of fluid intake, moderation of intake of caffeine or alcohol, use of relaxed and double-voiding techniques, scheduled voiding, distraction techniques—such as mental tricks to take the mind off the bladder—and toilet and bladder retraining. Individualized intervention focusing on managing patients’ most bothersome symptoms are suitable, especially in storage LUTSs which have a greater impact on quality of life (e.g., on social functioning and sleep disturbance from nocturia). According to data collected through self-administered questionnaires, self-management guidelines show some evidence both in relieving patients’ symptoms and at slowing down progression of LUTSs [2,3,4,5].
Muscle relaxation and a double-voiding technique can also be recommended to decrease post-void residual, and Kegel exercises [6]—for pelvic floor muscle strengthening—can help to check unpredictable urinary urgency, time between voids and episodes of nocturia. Unfortunately, the effectiveness of these techniques is debatable and has not found experimental support [5].
At variance from the limits of self-management advice, the use of strategies based on mental imagery may represent a different viable and fruitful approach to help people complaining of LUTS disorders. The beneficial effects of mental imagery on various tasks have a long tradition and have received consistently experimental support. Such a strategy is the core of many mnemonic aids [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14] as it has been shown to be valuable in the treatment of several pathological conditions. Indeed, the extant literature shows that psychiatric disorders (e.g., bipolar, post-traumatic stress disorders, anxiety disturbances, depression, phobias, obsessive and other personality disorders [8,15,16,17]) as well as neurologic diseases (e.g., Parkinson disease, motor impairments, strokes, unilateral neglect, neuropathic pains, traumatic brain injures [8,18,19,20,21,22]) greatly benefit from the use of mental imagery-based techniques, which favor rehabilitation and recovery. The same rationale applies in the treatment of physical disorders [23] and as a coadjuvant physical therapy [24]. The latter especially concerns sport players, easily at risk of injuries [25,26,27,28,29,30], whereas mental training practice is a fruitful tool to recover, improve physical skills and ameliorate performance.
Mental imagery techniques can be experienced both from an internal and an external perspective. The first requires a person to focus on what is happening inside their body, namely that they are executing a mentally generated motor movement, accompanied by a proprioceptive/kinesthetic sensation in which their muscles are involved (i.e., are contracting or relaxing), thus mimicking what happens in an overt action. From an external perspective, a person is required to imagine seeing themselves while performing a motor act, as though they are an observer [31,32,33,34].
As visual mental imagery has been conceptualized as a depictive representation, analogous to a real perception [8], several studies have focused on its possible common neural substrate, as well as on the relationship between imagined and executed actions. Most available data—coming from neurophysiological records and structural and functional brain imaging studies—are consistent with this notion [8,31,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42], showing activation in the same central brain regions; namely, the primary visual cortex and the pre-motor and parietal areas.
Based on the above well-documented literature on the efficacy of visual mental imagery and motor imagery in several domains, our main hypothesis aimed to verify whether this beneficial effect could be extended to urologic complaints. As outlined above, among post-micturition LUTSs, the sensation of incomplete emptying is a condition that in turn affects urinary urgency, nocturia and frequency; namely, the most bothersome LUTS for patients. Thus, the use of mental imagery strategies could be effective in favoring voiding the urinary bladder of its residual content, providing relief to the patients and the improvement of storage symptoms.
A second pioneering hypothesis was to investigate whether the use of odors of beverages and foods expected either to favor diuresis or micturition (like, e.g., onion, coffee, artichoke, asparagus) or typically associated with the bathroom environment (like, e.g., a scented cleanser) could—if sniffed along with micturition—trigger the expulsion of residual urine content. The rationale of the hypothesis relies on the well-demonstrated role that odors play in several aspects of daily life, both in health (e.g., fostering the acquisition of early preferences through associative learning, governing the appreciation and aversion to foods, triggering old and vivid memories bearing emotional content [43,44,45,46,47]) and disease (e.g., acting as a biomarker—the sebum odor—revealing Parkinson’s disease or cancer biomarkers, as the presence of volatile organic compounds in urine, and furthermore as early markers of pathological conditions, signs of metabolic and infectious diseases or even indicators of fear [48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57]).
In this report we describe the case of a patient complaining of LUTSs, who was trained to use both motor imagery for a 90-day period, and two odorants (coffee and a lavender scented cleanser) for 10 days at a time, to investigate their effectiveness in promoting the decreasing of the post-void residual urine. We are not aware of other studies on this topic.

2. Methods

2.1. The Patient

G.A. was a 68-year-old man who complained of being afflicted with mixed LUTSs in the last 4 years. He reported having very bothersome symptoms such as urgency, frequency and nocturia 3–4 times per night and sensations of incomplete voiding. He filled out the International Prostate Symptom Score (IPSS, [58]), which evaluates the occurrence and severity of LUTSs; the questionnaire consists of 7 questions, 3 about storage symptoms, 4 about voiding symptoms and a question relating to quality of life (QoL). Each symptom is scored on a scale of 0–5 except the QoL question, which is scored from 0 to 6. Thus, the score can range from 0 to 35. A symptom score of 0–7 is considered mild, 8–19 is considered moderate and 20–35 is considered severe. G.A. reported a score of 13—moderately symptomatic—but expressed a great impact of storage LUTSs on his QoL when he answered “unhappy” to the question “If you were to spend the rest of your life with your urinary condition just the way it is now, how would you feel about that?”. The “8-Item Overactive Bladder questionnaire score” was also administered obtaining an abnormal score of 18 [59]. This questionnaire includes eight questions with answers ranging from zero (no symptoms or impact) to three (extreme symptoms or impact). The total score is the sum of all questions (minimum = 0, maximum = 24). An average micturition urgency of grade 2 according to the “Patient’s Perception of Intensity of Urgency Scale” (PPIUS) was detected. PPIUS [60] describes the degree of micturition urgency on a scale from 0 (no urgency) to 4 (urge urinary incontinence).
Urine analysis and urine culture were negative, serum prostate-specific antigen was 9.8 ng/mL, physical examination and renal-bladder ultrasound results were unremarkable, with post-void residual urine of about 90 cc. Uroflowmetry revealed a mild obstructive flow pattern and multiparametric magnetic resonance imaging showed a large prostate (115 cc) without suspicious areas.
G.A. was treated with Serenoa repens with no benefit and then with tamsulosin with retrograde ejaculation disturbance and without efficacy on the LUTSs.
The test sessions occurred at the home of the patient. Informed written consent was obtained; the study was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the Good Clinical Practice of the European Community in the treatment of patients; the study was approved by the Bioethics Committee at the University of Padova (code number 740-a; 24 July 2024).
At the time of testing G.A. was in good health, fully alert and cooperative.

2.2. Materials and Procedures

The whole test procedure lasted 120 days, from September 2023 to December 2023, according to the following steps.
Step 1 (motor imagery strategy acquisition): Firstly, an expert urologist provided the patient in simple words with information on the physiology of the urinary tract, focusing mainly on how the information run from the brain to the urinary tract when there is a need to empty the urinary bladder. Then, the patient was trained to build up a visual image comprising the bladder, the detrusor muscle and the urethra (see examples depicted in Figure 1). He was instructed to imagine a signal coming from the brain, running along the spinal cord and reaching the bladder. Then, to imagine the detrusor muscle contracting, the flow of urine coming out from the bladder, through the urethra and being expelled. He was asked to concentrate mainly on the act: contracting—expelling (and to rehearse mentally the motor task, which required about three days, according to his self-report), without any overt physical movement). When the patient was familiarized with the task, the first experimental session took place (Step 2).
Step 2 (motor imagery strategy and bladder control): G.A. use the mental imagery strategy for three months according to the following rules. Every morning, immediately after breakfast, when in need of urination, the patient collected in a glass and weighted the expelled urine; immediately after that, he come back again to the bathroom and used the learned mental imagery strategy, waiting until the residual content of urine was expelled. This was again collected and weighted. The interval between the first (free) and the second (cued) micturition was recorded by means of a timer. During the task G.A. was suggested to close his eyes to improve concentration. The procedure was run meticulously by the patient, with the task performed between half past seven and half past eight in the morning of every day; the breakfast diet of G.A. for the whole period of testing did not vary (i.e., coffee, apple, and muesli with milk).
Step 3 (odor 1 strategy and bladder control): According to the secondary hypothesis, G.A. was asked to associate to micturition an odorant expected to favor diuresis or micturition (since, as outlined in the introduction, some beverages and foods known to elicit diuresis have an odor). The selected odorant was “coffee”. The general procedure was almost the same as for the motor imagery experiment, with the difference that, during the second round (after the collection of urine from the first micturition) G.A. came back to the bathroom, he was requested to smell the odor of fresh coffee from a glass and to wait for a possible second micturition. According to the extant procedures, it was suggested to G.A. to sniff the odorant for about 4 s, with the stimulus kept approximately 2 cm in front of both nostrils. Intervals of a few seconds between sniffs were also suggested to avoid carry-over adaption effects. The waiting time between the first and the possible second micturition was fixed at about seven minutes. The odor 1 procedure was followed by G.A. for ten days.
Step 4 (odor 2 strategy and bladder control): The general procedure here was the same as above (Step 3), with the difference that G.A. was asked to associate with micturition an odorant expected to be associated with a cleaned bathroom. We selected a natural and biological household cleanser with the odor of lavender (Gallo, Bio farm, Villanova di Camposampiero, Italy). The odor 2 procedure was followed by G.A. for ten days.
Step 5 (control condition, no intervention): The general procedure here was the same as above, with the difference that GA was asked to not use any strategy while waiting for a possible second round micturition. The control procedure was followed for ten days.
To summarize, the whole procedure followed by the patient was: 30 days motor imagery strategy; 10 days odor 1 strategy; 30 days motor imagery strategy; 10 days odor 2 strategy; 30 days motor imagery strategy; 10 days control condition.

3. Results

Descriptive statistics are shown in Figure 2, Figure 3 and Figure 4. The patient’s mean scores (M) and standard deviations (SD) in the function of the four conditions were as follows:
Motor imagery strategy:
M expelled urine first round, cc 132. 7 ± 20.8; range: cc 80–197.
M expelled urine second round, cc 71.3 ± 17.4; range: cc 31–133
M time interval between 1st and 2nd micturition, 4.75 ± 1.36 min.
Range: 2–7 min
Odor 1 strategy: M time interval between 1st and 2nd micturition, 70.6 ± 10.5 min.
Range: 53–85 min
Odor 2 strategy: M time interval between 1st and 2nd micturition, 71.1 ± 11.6 (min).
Range: 55–86 min
Control condition: M time interval between 1st and 2nd micturition, 79.5 ± 10.7 (min).
Range: 55–86 min
Post-training questionnaires administration showed as follows:
-
IPSS decreased from 13 to 9, with improvement of voiding LUTSs, reduction in nocturia from 3 to 4 to 2 times per night and a 1-point improvement in QoL assessment.
-
The “8-Item Overactive Bladder questionnaire score” decreased from 13 to 9.
-
PPIUS remained unchanged at 2 (moderate urinary urgency).
These results suggest that the mental imagery strategy was successful in reducing bladder residual content voiding in a few minutes amount of time (see, Figure 2) at variance from the other conditions (see, Figure 3) whose scores are almost the same.
Figure 4 provide a picture of the quantity (cc) of expelled urine in the few minutes between the first- and second-round micturition in the function of the motor imagery condition.

4. Discussion and Future Directions

In this study, we described a procedure to relieve the feeling of incomplete bladder emptying and to reduce the bladder post-void residual urine in a patient exhibiting storage LUTSs; a condition which is one of the most bothersome at post-micturition stage and that has a negative impact on urinary urgency, nocturia and frequency [3,4].
We proposed two training approaches, the first based on motor imagery and the second on the use of odors, based on the rationales of the effectiveness of mental imagery for various tasks [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,38] and the role that odors play in several aspects of daily life [43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57].
The results of the descriptive statistics unequivocally show that the short average time interval (M = 4.75 min) between the first and second micturition was due to the motor imagery strategy, at variance with the other conditions (M = 70.6 min for the odor 1 strategy; 71.1 min for the odor 2 strategy; and 79.5 min for the control, respectively) whose scores are almost the same and match the patient’ self-report about early-morning micturition.
It is also worth stressing here that the level of satisfaction of the patient is high, and that he is currently successfully adhering to the strategy, both every morning when waking up and before going to sleep.
The motor imagery procedure shows, moreover, a further positive impact on storage LUTS disorders—i.e., urinary urgency, nocturia and frequency—since the possibility of patients voiding the bladder of all or most of its content plausibly lessens the frequency of micturition and also reduces the numbers of times patients wake up at night (when the motor imagery procedure is adopted before going to sleep; see the post-training questionnaire scores). This approach also limits the likelihood of bacterial bladder infections due to urinary residue [61]. Due to its feasibility, the strategy can be performed several times during the day and, in addition—if necessary—can be integrated with other powerful imagery cues; namely, imaging that the day is particularly cold, that the urgency is unstoppable and that it is necessary to quickly reach a place safe for micturition.
What about the use of odorants to manage LUTSs? Can the olfactory bladder control hypothesis be ruled out? We think not. Although not successful here, it could be a promising future line of research. Firstly, it is still plausible that an odor could trigger micturition if it has diuretic properties. Therefore, looking for one or more suitable odors is a challenge.
Secondly, it is known that the evaluative conditioning paradigm (a form of associative learning) is a well-established and experimentally verified way to induce behaviors and emotions [62,63] and can represent here a good candidate for our purposes. In the present study, we assumed that the odor of a lavender-scented cleanser could have an associative relationship with the bathroom environment [64]; probably, however, this odorant was not so strictly linked this as to trigger micturition. Thus, according to the steps of the evaluative conditioning paradigm, a patient could be trained to learn an association between an odorant and micturition. Currently, we have a study underway to test this hypothesis, with the odor of lavender as the neutral stimulus (NS), to be sniffed before and during the proprioceptive sensation (i.e., the unconditioned stimulus, US) that accompanies micturition, and micturition as an unconditioned response (UR). According to the paradigm, following repeated association, the sole presentation of the odor (now, the conditioned stimulus, CS) should elicit micturition (now, the conditioned response, CR [63]).
Therefore, a suitable odor as a diuretic stimulus and bladder conditioning by means of the evaluative conditioning paradigm, can be successful strategies to help people to manage low urinary tract disorders.
To conclude, the present study has demonstrated the usefulness of the motor imagery strategy in promoting the emptying of the bladder from the residual content of urine in a man suffering moderate storage LUTSs. We, therefore, recommend this procedure as a novel suggested protocol for clinical and experimental purposes.
We acknowledge, however, that our study, has the limitation of being preliminary in nature and based on a case report. Accordingly, although single-subject designs are per se well-known and effective experimental procedures [65], further research, including large observational and randomized controlled trials, is needed.

Author Contributions

G.M.Z. conceptualized, devised and wrote the initial draft, which was checked and edited by T.H. Questionnaires were administered and commented by E.A. Methodology and data curation, G.M.Z. and T.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Bioethics Committee at the University of Padova (no. code 740-a; 24 July 2024).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

No data are available on the web due to privacy concerns.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to emeritus Remo Job for comments on and criticisms of an early draft, to Patrizio Tressoldi for statistical and technical support and to Emanuele Luigi Carniel for stimulating insights.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Brown, C.T.; Yap, T.; Cromwell, D.A.; Rixon, L.; Steed, L.; Mulligan, K.; Mundy, A.; Newman, S.P.; van der Meulen, J.; Emberton, M. Self-management for men with lower urinary tract symptoms: Randomized controlled trial. BMJ 2007, 334, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Yap, T.L.; Brown, C.; Cromwell, D.A.; van der Meulen, J.; Emberton, M. The impact of self-management of lower urinary tract symptoms on frequency- volume chart measures. BJU Int. 2009, 104, 1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Albarqouni, L.; Sanders, S.; Clark, J.; Tikkinen, K.A.O.; Glasziou, P. Self-Management for Men with Lower Urinary Tract Symptoms: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Ann. Fam. Med. 2021, 19, 157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Lee, S.W.; Doo, S.W.; Yang, W.J.; Song, Y.S. Importance of Relieving the Most Bothersome Symptom for Improving Quality of Life in Male Patients with Lower Urinary Tract Symptoms. Urology 2012, 80, 684–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Blanker, M.H.; Brandenbarg, P.; Slijkhuis, B.G.; Steffens, M.G.; van Balken, M.R.; Jellema, P. Development of an online personalized self-management intervention for men with uncomplicated LUTS. Neurourol. Urodyn. 2019, 38, 1685–1691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Kegel, A.H. Progressive resistance exercise in the functional restoration of the perineal muscles. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 1948, 56, 238–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Paivio, A. Mental Representations. A Dual Coding Approach; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  8. Pearson, J.; Naselaris, T.; Holmes, E.A.; Kosslyn, S.M. Mental Imagery: Functional Mechanisms and Clinical Applications. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 590–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Richardson, J.T.E. Mental Imagery and Human Memory; Red Globe Press: London, UK, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  10. McDaniel, M.A.; Pressley, M. Imagery and Related Mnemonic Processes: Theories, Individual Differences, and Applications; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  11. Higbee, K.L. Your Memory, How It Works and How to Improve It; Prentice Hall Press: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cioca, L.-I.; Nerișanu, R.A. Enhancing Creativity: Using Visual Mnemonic Devices in the Teaching Process in Order to Develop Creativity in Students. Sustainability 2020, 12, 1985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Hirst, R.K. Using Visual Mnemonics to Make Instructions Easier to Remember. J. Tech. Writ. Commun. 1990, 20, 411–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Zucco, G.M.; Coselli, L. L’immaginazione motoria: Strumenti per la valutazione ed effetti dei training sulla prestazione sportiva (Translation: Motor Imagery). In L’arte del Movimento tra ieri e Oggi (Translation: The Art of Movement); Gundlach, H., Perez Cordoba, E., Sinatra, M., Tanucci, G., Eds.; Pensa Multimedia: Lecce, Italy, 2012; pp. 274–285. [Google Scholar]
  15. Brockman, R.N.; Calvert, F.L. Imagery Rescripting for PTSD and Personality Disorders: Theory and Application. J. Contemp. Psychother. 2017, 47, 23–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Holmes, E.A.; Mathews, A. Mental imagery in emotion and emotional disorders. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 30, 349–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Brewin, C.R.; Wheatley, J.; Patel, T.; Fearon, P.; Hackmann, A.; Wells, A.; Myers, S. Imagery rescripting as a brief stand-alone treatment for depressed patients with intrusive memories. Behav. Res. Ther. 2009, 47, 569–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Abraham, A.; Duncan, R.P.; Earhart, G.M. The Role of Mental Imagery in Parkinson’s Disease Rehabilitation. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Page, S.J. Imagery Improves Upper Extremity Motor Function in Chronic Stroke Patients: A Pilot Study. Occup. Ther. J. Res. 2000, 20, 200–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Bisiach, E.; Luzzatti, C. Unilateral Neglect of Representational Space. Cortex 1978, 14, 129–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Jackson, P.L.; Lafleur, M.F.; Malouin, F.; Richards, C.; Doyon, J. Potential role of mental practice using motor imagery in neurologic rehabilitation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2001, 82, 1133–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Potvin, M.-J.; Rouleau, I.; Sénéchal, G.; Giguère, J.-F. Prospective memory rehabilitation based on visual imagery techniques. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2011, 21, 899–924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Rushal, B.; Lippman, L. The role of imagery in physical performance. Int. J. Sport Psych. 1988, 29, 57–72. [Google Scholar]
  24. Warner, L.; McNeill, E. Mental Imagery and Its Potential for Physical Therapy. Phys. Ther. 1988, 68, 516–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Jones, L.; Stuht, G. The uses of mental imagery in athletics: An overview. Appl. Prev. Psych. 1997, 6, 2101–2115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Evans, L.; Hare, R.; Mullen, R. Imagery Use during Rehabilitation from Injury. J. Imag. Res. Sport Phys. Act. 2006, 1, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Driediger, M.; Hall, C.; Callow, N. Imagery use by injured athletes: A qualitative analysis. J. Sports Sci. 2006, 24, 261–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Gabbard, C.; Fox, A. Using motor imagery therapy to improve movement efficiency and reduce fall injury risk. J. Nov. Physiother. 2013, 3, 186. [Google Scholar]
  29. Clark, B.C.; Mahato, N.K.; Nakazawa, M.; Law, T.D.; Thomas, J.S. The power of the mind: The cortex as a critical determinant of muscle strength/weakness. J. Neurophysiol. 2014, 112, 3219–3226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Slimani, M.; Tod, D.; Chaabene, H.; Miarka, B.; Chamari, K. Effects of Mental Imagery on Muscular Strength in Healthy and Patient Participants: A Systematic Review. J. Sports Sci. Med. 2016, 15, 434–450. [Google Scholar]
  31. Jeannerod, M. Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition. NeuroImage 2001, 14, S103–S109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Wang, Y.; Morgan, W.P. The effect of imagery perspectives on the psychophysiological responses to imagined exercise. Behav. Brain Res. 1992, 52, 167–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Yu, Q.H.; Fu, A.S.; Kho, A.; Li, J.; Sun, X.H.; Chan, C.C. Imagery perspective among young athletes: Differentiation between external and internal visual imagery. J. Sport Health Sci. 2016, 5, 211–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Tsakiris, M.; De Preester, H. The Interoceptive Mind: From Homeostasis to Awareness; Oxford University Press: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  35. Decety, J. Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate? Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 1996, 3, 87–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hardwick, R.M.; Caspers, S.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Swinnen, S.P. Neural correlates of action: Comparing meta-analyses of imagery, observation, and execution. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 94, 31–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Guillot, A.; Collet, C. Neurophysiological substrates of motor imagery ability. In The Neurophysio-Logical Foundations of Mental and Motor Imagery; Guillot, A., Collet, C., Eds.; Oxford U.P: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 109–123. [Google Scholar]
  38. Kosslyn, S.M.; Ganis, G.; Thompson, W.L. Neural foundations of imagery. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 21, 635–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. O’Craven, K.M.; Kanwisher, N. Mental Imagery of Faces and Places Activates Corresponding Stimulus-Specific Brain Regions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000, 12, 1013–1023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ganis, G.; Thompson, W.L.; Kosslyn, S.M. Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: An fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 2004, 20, 226–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kosslyn, S.M.; Thompson, W.L. When is early visual cortex activated during visual mental imagery? Psychol. Bull. 2003, 129, 723–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Porro, C.A.; Francescato, M.P.; Cettolo, V.; Diamond, M.E.; Baraldi, P.; Zuiani, C.; Bazzocchi, M.; di Prampero, P.E. Primary Motor and Sensory Cortex Activation during Motor Performance and Motor Imagery: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. J. Neurosci. 1996, 16, 7688–7698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Zucco, G.M.; Herz, R.S.; Schaal, B. (Eds.) Olfactory Cognition: From Perception and Memory to Environmental Odours and Neuroscience; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  44. White, T.L.; Thomas-Danguin, T.; Olofsson, J.K.; Zucco, G.M.; Prescott, J. Thought for food: Cognitive influences on chemosensory perceptions and preferences. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 79, 103776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Hummel, T.; Welge-Luessen, A. Taste and Smell: An Update; Karger Press: Basel, Switzerland, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  46. Doty, R.H. (Ed.) Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation; Wiley Blackwell Publisher: Haboken, NJ, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  47. Schaal, B.; Ferdenzi, C.; Wathelet, O. Odeurs et Emotions (Odor and Emotion); Editions Universitaires de Dijon: Dijon, France, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  48. Trivedi, D.K.; Sinclair, E.; Xu, Y.; Sarkar, D.; Walton-Doyle, C.; Liscio, C.; Banks, P.; Milne, J.; Silverdale, M.; Kunath, T.; et al. Discovery of Volatile Biomarkers of Parkinson’s Disease from Sebum. ACS Central Sci. 2019, 5, 599–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cornu, J.-N.; Cancel-Tassin, G.; Ondet, V.; Girardet, C.; Cussenot, O. Olfactory Detection of Prostate Cancer by Dogs Sniffing Urine: A Step Forward in Early Diagnosis. Eur. Urol. 2011, 59, 197–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Mika, S.; Kazushige, T. The scent of disease: Volatile organic compounds of the human body related to disease and disorder. J. Biochem. 2011, 150, 257–266. [Google Scholar]
  51. Ackerl, K.; Atzmueller, M.; Grammer, K. The scent of fear. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 2002, 3, 79–84. [Google Scholar]
  52. Kovács, T. Mechanisms of olfactory dysfunction in aging and neurodegenerative disorders. Ageing Res. Rev. 2004, 3, 215–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Hummel, T.; Liu, D.; Mueller, C.; Stuck, B.; Welhge-Luessen, A.; Haehner, A. Olfactory Dysfunction: Etiology, Diagnosis, and Treatment. Dtsch. Arztebl. Int. 2023, 120, 146–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Pieniak, M.; Oleszkiewicz, A.; Avaro, V.; Calegari, F.; Hummel, T. Olfactory training—Thirteen years of research reviewed. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2022, 141, 104853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Doty, R.L.; Hawkes, C.; Good, K.; Duda, J. Odor perception and neuropathology in neurodegenerative diseases and schizophrenia. In Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation; Doty, R.H., Ed.; Wiley Blackwell Publisher: Haboken, NJ, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  56. Zucco, G.M.; Doty, R.L. Multiple Chemical Sensitivity. Brain Sci. 2021, 12, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Zucco, G.M.; Bollini, F. Odour recognition memory and odour identification in patients with mild and severe major depressive disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2011, 190, 217–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Barry, M.J.; Fowler, F.J., Jr.; O’Leary, M.P.; Bruskewitz, R.C.; Holtgrewe, H.L.; Mebust, W.K. The American Urological Association Symptom Index for Benign Prostatic Hyperplasia. J. Urol. 1992, 148 Pt 1, 1549–1557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Basra, R.; Artibani, W.; Cardozo, L.; Castro-Diaz, D.; Chapple, C.; Cortes, E.; De Ridder, D.; Pons, M.E.; Haab, F.; Hohenfellner, M.; et al. Design and Validation of a New Screening Instrument for Lower Urinary Tract Dysfunction: The Bladder Control Self-Assessment Questionnaire (B-SAQ). Eur. Urol. 2007, 52, 230–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. EMA (European Medicines Agency); Committee for Proprietary Medicinal Products. Note for Guidance on the Clinical Investigation of Medicinal Products for the Treatment of Urinary Incontinence; CPMP/EWP/18/01; EMA: London, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  61. Browne, R.F.J.; Zwirewich, C.; Torreggiani, W.C. Imaging of urinary tract infection in the adult. Eur. Radiol. Suppl. 2004, 14, E168–E183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. De Houwer, J.; Thomas, S.; Baeyens, F. Associative learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychol. Bul. 2001, 127, 853–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Zucco, G.M. The acquisition of odour preferences via evaluative olfactory conditioning: Historical background and state of the art. In Olfactory Cognition: From Perception and Memory to Environmental Odours and Neuroscience; Zucco, G.M., Herz, R.S., Schaal, B., Eds.; John Benjamin’s Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  64. Baeyens, F.; Wrzesniewski, A.; De Houwer, J.; Eelen, P. Toilet rooms, body massages, and smells: Two field studies on human evaluative odor conditioning. Curr. Psychol. 1996, 15, 77–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Kazdin, A.E. Single-Case Research Design: Methods for Clinical and Applied Settings, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Examples of images used to train the patient in the use of motor imagery (picture on the left available from www.google.com/search?q=urinary+system, (accessed on 1 November 2023). National Cancer Institute (NCI); picture on the right, from the authors’ archive).
Figure 1. Examples of images used to train the patient in the use of motor imagery (picture on the left available from www.google.com/search?q=urinary+system, (accessed on 1 November 2023). National Cancer Institute (NCI); picture on the right, from the authors’ archive).
Healthcare 12 01855 g001
Figure 2. Time interval (minutes) between 1st and 2nd micturition in function of motor imagery strategy. (M = 4.75 ± 1.36 min), for a ninety-day period.
Figure 2. Time interval (minutes) between 1st and 2nd micturition in function of motor imagery strategy. (M = 4.75 ± 1.36 min), for a ninety-day period.
Healthcare 12 01855 g002
Figure 3. Time interval (minutes) between 1st and 2nd micturition in function of control (M = 79.5 ± 10.7). Odor 1 strategy (M = 70.6 ± 10.5) and odor 2 strategy (M = 71.1 ± 11.6) conditions for a ten-day period.
Figure 3. Time interval (minutes) between 1st and 2nd micturition in function of control (M = 79.5 ± 10.7). Odor 1 strategy (M = 70.6 ± 10.5) and odor 2 strategy (M = 71.1 ± 11.6) conditions for a ten-day period.
Healthcare 12 01855 g003
Figure 4. Expelled urine in first and second round (cc) in function of motor imagery strategy for a ninety-day period. Legend: M1cc: urine expelled at first micturition; M2ccMI: urine expelled at second micturition, post motor imagery.
Figure 4. Expelled urine in first and second round (cc) in function of motor imagery strategy for a ninety-day period. Legend: M1cc: urine expelled at first micturition; M2ccMI: urine expelled at second micturition, post motor imagery.
Healthcare 12 01855 g004
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zucco, G.M.; Andretta, E.; Hummel, T. Strategies to Improve Bladder Control: A Preliminary Case Study. Healthcare 2024, 12, 1855. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12181855

AMA Style

Zucco GM, Andretta E, Hummel T. Strategies to Improve Bladder Control: A Preliminary Case Study. Healthcare. 2024; 12(18):1855. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12181855

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zucco, Gesualdo M., Elena Andretta, and Thomas Hummel. 2024. "Strategies to Improve Bladder Control: A Preliminary Case Study" Healthcare 12, no. 18: 1855. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12181855

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop