Next Article in Journal
Timing of Primary Tooth Eruption in Infants Observed by Their Parents
Previous Article in Journal
Effect of Clemastine on Neurophysiological Outcomes in an Ovine Model of Neonatal Hypoxic-Ischemic Encephalopathy
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Does Educational Status Influence Parents’ Response to Bad News in the NICU?

1
Children’s Hospital, University Hospital, Philipps University of Marburg, 35033 Marburg, Germany
2
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Philipps University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
3
Faculty of Medicine, Deans Office, Research Group Medical Ethics, Philipps University of Marburg, 35033 Marburg, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2023, 10(11), 1729; https://doi.org/10.3390/children10111729
Submission received: 3 September 2023 / Revised: 22 October 2023 / Accepted: 23 October 2023 / Published: 25 October 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Neonatology)

Abstract

:
Communication in neonatal intensive care units and the relationship between families and staff have been reported to influence parental mental well-being. Research has also shown an impact of parental educational level on their well-being. However, whether different educational levels result in different reactions to breaking bad news (BBN) by physicians remains unanswered so far. We therefore examined the impact of parental level of education on their mental state after a BBN conversation and their relation to physicians. A prospective quantitative survey was conducted amongst 54 parents whose preterm or term infants were hospitalized in three German neonatal units. Parental education was classified as low (lower secondary/less (1), n: 23) or high (higher secondary/more (2), n: 31). Parents answered questions about certain aspects of and their mental state after BBN and their trust in physicians. The two groups did not differ significantly in their mental condition after BBN, with both reporting high levels of exhaustion and worries, each (median (min;max): (1): 16 (6;20) vs. (2): 14 (5;20), (scaling: 5–20)). However, lower-educated parents reported a lower trust in physicians (median (min;max): (1): 2 (0;9) vs. (2): 1 (0;6), p < 0.05 (scaling: 0–10)) and felt less safe during BBN (median (min;max): (1): 15 (9;35) vs. (2): 13 (9;33), p < 0.05). Only among higher-educated parents was trust in physicians significantly correlated with the safety and orientation provided during BBN (r: 0.583, p < 0.05, r: 0.584, p < 0.01). Concurrently, only among less-educated parents was safety correlated with the hope conveyed during BBN (r: 0.763, p < 0.01). Therefore, in BBN discussions with less-educated parents, physicians should focus more on giving them hope to promote safety.

1. Introduction

Premature birth and congenital malformations or diseases in newborn babies are an inherent and recurrent aspect of human life. Globally, approximately one in ten infants is born prematurely [1] and one in six newborns requires further medical support after birth [2]. Consequently, admission to a neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) becomes necessary to provide appropriate medical care, not seldomly resulting in the separation of the newborn from her/his parents.
Prematurity carries a significant risk of serious health problems for the newborn [3,4]. A newborn’s stay in a NICU can induce stress in parents, leading to various psychological symptoms such as depression, anxiety, or even post-traumatic stress symptoms [5,6,7]. Stress and worry increase with the occurrence of complications or illness of the infant.
In addition, further factors influence parents’ experience of stress and well-being, including their personal characteristics [8], social support [9], and their relationships and communication with nurses and physicians [10,11,12], which may be characterized by trust or lack thereof [9,13].
When medical problems arise, physicians have to inform parents about the situation. Despite the negative content, an empathic, patient-/recipient-centered way of communication with room for hope can sustain patients/relatives [14,15,16,17]. BBN is a challenging task for physicians and can affect the physician–parent relationship, with an insensitive communication of diagnoses having a negative impact [18]. This underscores the importance of guidelines for BBN for professionals. SPIKES is the only widely taught protocol [16], suggesting an orientation toward the needs of the recipient. However, existing protocols do not provide specific recommendations for BBN to different groups of recipients, e.g., patients with lower educational levels compared to patients with higher educational levels [15,16,18].
This is a gap, as Seifart et al. [19] demonstrated the influence of educational level on wishes and preferences regarding BBN. In addition, Baird [20] and Enke [12] reported effects of various parental- or child-related variables on stress or satisfaction in BBN in pediatrics too. In general, studies state an influence of recipient variables such as the socioeconomic status, measured by educational level, on health behaviors per se [21,22,23] as well as parenting stress [24].
Specifically, in neonatology, the influence of parental socioeconomic status on various aspects is well documented: it can impact the type of therapeutic decisions parents make, neonatal morbidity, and the long-term developmental outcome of preterm infants [3,25,26,27,28,29,30]. Therefore, socioeconomic status seems to play an important role in NICUs whereat an infant´s health risks generally increase with the lower educational status of the parents [3,25,26,27,28,29,30]. Additionally, factors such as parents´ level of education, their age, and social support have been found to influence the level of stress experienced in NICUs [31,32]. Nevertheless, to our knowledge, no surveys have been conducted to investigate the influence of educational level on stress, well-being, and trust in physicians in NICUs in relation to BBN (Box 1).
Box 1. Research in Context.
Research in context
Evidence before this study
We started searching PubMed between September and November 2018 before submitting the study to the institutional review board of the Philipps University of Marburg. We updated our PubMed search on 6 December 2022 and 29 December  2022, while preparing and before submitting this manuscript, more than four years after we had registered our study. Our search terms were the following:
(((((parent[All Fields] OR parent’s[All Fields] OR parental[All Fields] OR parenthood[All Fields] OR parenting[All Fields] OR parents[All Fields] OR parents’[All Fields]) OR (relative[All Fields] OR relatives[All Fields]) OR (““infant, newborn””[MeSH Terms] OR newborn[Text Word]) OR (neonatal[All Fields] OR neonatale[All Fields] OR neonatally[All Fields] OR neonate[All Fields] OR neonates[All Fields] OR neonati[All Fields] OR neonatologist[All Fields] OR neonatologists[All Fields] OR neonatology[All Fields] OR neonatus[All Fields]) OR (““premature birth””[MeSH Terms] OR preterm[Text Word]) OR (““intensive care units, neonatal””[MeSH Terms] OR NICU[Text Word]))) AND (breaking bad new* OR bad new* deliver* OR SPIKES OR communication OR doctor OR pediatr* OR paediatr*)) AND (educat* OR income OR education level)) AND (stress OR distress OR trust OR satisfaction OR well-being OR mental health OR depress OR anxiety OR health)”.
We identified no studies focusing on mental well-being in parents of newborns in a neonatal intensive care unit after having been delivered bad news, searching for differences in the level of education. As outlined in the introduction and discussion, we did find a few studies related to the present research in that the same target population was focused. However, comparators differed as well as endpoints.
 
Added value of this study
This analysis of prospectively collected data revealed for the first time that parents with a lower educational status reported less trust in attending/treating physicians than those with a higher educational level and that physicians failed to communicate safety for lower-educated parents. It was found that only among higher-educated parents were the studied parameters of the BBN conversation correlated with trust in physicians. Moreover, only among parents with lower levels of education did hope and security conveyed in a BBN conversation correlate strongly with each other.
In previous analyses we defined determinants influencing BBN in neonatology [33] and compared parental preferences with their actual BBN experiences [34] and found that BBN not only burdened the recipients but also the messengers. The majority of parental top preferences differed significantly in their implementation in the real-life interview, and a compassionate delivery of bad news correlated highest with a parent-rated quality of BBN conversation. Based on this work and the mentioned influence of parental educational level in neonatology on various aspects, we wanted to know whether parents respond differently to BBN according to their educational level. Additionally, we wanted to know whether there was a relationship between the parent–physician relationship and the parental sense of safety and orientation elicited by the physician’s BBN interview. This connection is based on Grawe’s consistency theory, which names basic human needs, such as self-efficacy, safety, and orientation. It can be used to explain the reactions of people in exceptional psychological situations [35,36] and helps to derive communication strategies for them [37].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Ethics

The institutional review board of the Philipps University of Marburg approved this study (ID-No.: 164/18). It was conducted according to the Helsinki Declaration, Good Clinical Practice guidelines, and the EU data collection directive. Parents were informed about the study before participation in written form. They consented by completing the questionnaire.

2.2. Participants and Procedure

This study was conducted in identical paper/pencil and online versions. From December 2018 to September 2020, data were collected prospectively in three neonatal units: the University Hospital Marburg, the Carl-Thiem-University-Hospital Cottbus (each highest care level) and the Hospital of Bad Hersfeld (middle care level). Usually, both parents received one questionnaire each and were approached by the researchers after bad news concerning their infant had been broken to them. Additionally, a freely accessible QR code, to be found on the web page of the German federation ‘The preterm Infant’, gave access to the online version. Inclusion criteria contained a serious diagnosis of the infant, such as serious congenital malformation, intraventricular hemorrhage, pneumothorax, or necrotizing enterocolitis, about which neonatologists or pediatric surgeons informed the parents in terms of delivering bad news. Usually, only attending physicians broke bad news. However, information about the level of education of the physician and the type of residency training was not collected. Participants were asked to refer to the first time bad news had been broken to them. The conversation should not have been more than six weeks ago. Questionnaires returned after this period were nevertheless accepted. Usually, the questionnaire was answered within two weeks after bad news’ delivery. Sufficient German language skills were required, as well as a minimum age of 18 years. Exclusion criteria were an age of less than 18 years, insufficient German language, and lack of consent to participate in the study.

2.3. Demographic Characteristics

Twenty-two items collected demographic data. These included age, sex, marital and educational status of the responding parent, complications during pregnancy, illness or complication of the infant, gestational age at time of birth, and parental evaluation of the infant’s current health condition (see Table 1).

2.4. Parental Mental Condition, Self-Efficacy, and Trust as well as Certain Aspects of BBN

The questionnaire asked for the procedure and perception of the first BBN consultation as well as preferences for BBN additionally to the quality of BBN in terms of satisfaction with the conversation, the condition after bad news’ delivery, self-efficacy, and their relationship with the treating physicians. The development and structure of the questionnaire as well as results concerning parental experiences, preferences, and satisfaction with BBN are presented elsewhere [34].
In contrast to that first report, we now focus on the following parameters: parents’ mental state after a BBN conversation, their self-efficacy, their assessment of certain behaviors of physicians in the BBN conversation that convey either ‘safety’ or ‘orientation’, and their relationship with the physicians as a function of parental educational level.
To survey the parental condition past BBN, five questions asked for the condition in the first few days after BBN, concerning aspects like sleep, concentration, worry, or exhaustion (for further information, see Appendix A Table A1 and Table A2).
To check for the mental condition, we used the following questionnaires: The PHQ-4 (Patient Health Questionnaire-4 [38]), the PSS-10 (Perceived Stress Scale-10 [39,40]), one question about current satisfaction with physical and mental well-being, and one for the current emotional burden/stress of the ‘present situation’. Four questions were asked for the overall self-estimation of self-efficacy. One item referred to the current relationship with physicians in terms of trust (for further information, see Appendix A Table A1 and Table A2).
Based on Grawe’s consistency theory, nine and eight options, respectively, were used to query whether certain behaviors of physicians in the BBN interview elicited ‘safety’ or ‘orientation’. Parents were asked to rate nine or eight statements, respectively, which followed the following phrase: ‘Physician conveyed safety/orientation in the conversation in that he/she…’ (for further information, see Appendix A Table A1 and Table A3). Additionally, in the section asking for parental experiences with their first BBN interview (see [34]), parents were asked to respond to singular statements that were rated on a four-point Likert scale (1 = ‘entirely’ to 4 = ‘not at all’). Four of them corresponded to ‘safety’ and ‘orientation’ too and were therefore also evaluated (see Section 3.4). In our former report, the five items related to the parental experience of the BBN conversation with the lowest agreement rates were listed. Two of the items identified in this way related to the communication of ‘hope’ and ‘safety’ too [34].
Seven items asked about certain characteristics of the setting and physician.

2.5. Data Analysis

Low parental education (lower secondary education or less in accordance with the International Standard Classification of Education, revised 2011 (ISCED-2011)) was compared with higher secondary education or more (according to Sentenac et al. [25]) for all reported data. Descriptive data were calculated with median and range, and mean and standard deviation for PSS-10, due to available validating data [40]. For the PHQ-4, the valid percentage of subjects who exceeded the clinical cut-off was calculated. Differences in demographic data according to parental educational level were calculated with Chi2 and Mann–Whitney U tests. To examine the difference for certain characteristics of the BBN interview, in the reported condition after BBN and trust in physicians according to the level of education, Mann–Whitney U tests were calculated. Cohen’s d was calculated as an effect size measure [41,42]. To examine the relationship between trust in physicians and medical behavior during BBN that elicited either safety or orientation, correlation coefficients (Spearman) were calculated. Fisher’s Z was used to test the differences in correlations between the two groups for significance. Comparatively, simple linear regressions were also calculated to assess the influence of educational status. To check for internal consistency, Cronbach’s alpha was calculated for items referring to either eliciting safety or orientation (see Appendix A Table A3).

3. Results

3.1. Sample and Demographic Data

Fifty-four questionnaires were returned altogether, seven of which were in the online version, with 23 parents with a lower secondary education or less (group 1) and 31 parents with an upper secondary education or more (group 2). The demographic and medical characteristics of the parents and infants are listed in Table 1. Most of the respondents were mothers ((group 1): 87%, (group 2): 81%) and were between 30–39 years old ((group 1): 65%, (group 2): 77%). Antenatal fetal problems were reported by 26% (group 1) versus 13% (group 2) of the respondents. Nine percent (group 1) versus 19% (group 2) stated a twin pregnancy. The rate of complications during pregnancy was high for both groups ((group 1): 44%, (group 2): 52%). In group 1, 43% of the infants were born mature and in group 2, 29% were. None of the demographic data differed significantly between the two groups. The survey was anonymized. Therefore, we could not match the physician’s diagnoses with information of the parents about their newborn’s disease and their evaluation of the infant’s health status and not specify the level of care of the corresponding hospitals. The parental responses to diagnoses were too imprecise for further analysis. We were unable to verify whether the seven participants who completed the questionnaire online actually had an infant with a serious diagnosis. However, we asked for the type of complication/disease, and answers seemed plausible, such as severe asphyxia or intraventricular hemorrhage.

3.2. Mental Condition after the Delivery of Bad News

The mental condition for a lower versus higher educational status is listed in Table 2. Looking at the valid percentage in group 1, 19% exceeded the clinical cut-off for depression (>3) and 20% in group 2 did, with no significant difference between the two groups (U: 301.00, Z: −0.273, p: 0.785). For anxiety, 28.6% of persons in group 1 exceeded the clinical cut-off (>3) and 21.4% in group 2 did, with no significant difference between the groups either (U: 293.50, Z: −0.010, p: 0.992). This incidence was higher compared to the general population [43,44]. Both groups reported a high burden in terms of exhaustion, sleeplessness, or worries the first few days after BBN (’well-being and condition after BBN’: median (min;max): (group 1): 16.00 (6;20), (group 2): 14.00 (5;20)). Looking at Cohen’s d, we found only small effect sizes. Our comparatively small sample size makes it difficult to actually detect differences between the two educational groups.

3.3. Stress, Self-Efficacy, Helplessness, and Life Satisfaction

The results for the PSS-10, current self-estimated stress, current satisfaction with physical and mental well-being, and overall self-estimation of self-efficacy, are listed in Table 2 as well. All parents reported a high ‘perceived helplessness’ (PSS-10: mean (sd): (group 1): 13.67 (3.864), (group 2): 14.36 (5.539)), exceeding the means of the German comparison group by more than one standard deviation (for values of the comparison group, see [40]). Self-estimated stress and self-efficacy as well as satisfaction with well-being were in a medium range each (stress: median (min;max): (group 1): 5.00 (1;10), (group 2): 5.50 (1;10)/self-efficacy: median (min;max): (group 1): 9.00 (5;12), (group 2): 8.00 (4;12)/satisfaction with well-being: median (min;max): (group 1): 2.50 (1;4), (group 2): 2.00 (1;4)).

3.4. Certain Aspects of the BBN Conversation and Trust in Physicians

Results for current trust in physicians and physicians’ behavior in the BBN interview that elicited safety or orientation are listed in Table 3. Responding parents with a lower educational status reported significantly less trust in physicians (U: 244.50, Z: −2.035, p: 0.042, d: 0.598) than did those with a higher educational status. For both the nine items that asked about a particular physician’s behavior, which was supposed to provide reassurance (U: 196.50, Z: −2.136, p: 0.033, d: 0.513) and the single item ‘Physician gave me safety’ (U: 190.00, Z: −2.188, p: 0.029, d: 0.654), less-educated parents reported that physicians failed to make them feel safe. There was a tendency, though not significantly, for less-educated parents to not have sufficient opportunities for questions, and they felt that physicians transported only little hope. Despite our small sample size, effect size measures were predominantly found in a medium range (see Table 3), which supports this being a real effect, detectable even in a small sample.

3.5. Correlation of Safety, Orientation, Certain Characteristics of the BBN Conversation, and Trust in Physicians

To check if the BBN conversation elicited safety and orientation among parents and if this was related to trust in physicians, Spearman correlation coefficients were calculated (see Table 4). A high transmission of safety went along with a high transmission of orientation for both groups ((group 1): r: 0.784, (group 2): r: 0.848, with p < 0.01 each). In both groups, the ability to ask questions correlated significantly with safety and orientation (see Table 4), whereby the correlation between asking questions and orientation tended to be higher for less-educated parents ((group 1): r: 0.820, (group 2): r: 0.508, Z: 1.864, p: 0.062). Only among higher-educated parents did we find significant correlations between trust in physicians and safety (‘item safety’: (group 1): r: 0.198, (group 2): r: 0.715, Z: −2.2538, p: 0.024) and understanding ((group 1): r: −0.036, (group 2): r: 0.493, Z: −1.9675, p: 0.049), with significant differences of the correlation coefficients between the two groups. Additionally, only among less-educated parents did hope correlate with safety (‘item safety’: (group 1): r: 0.763, (group 2): r: 0.208, Z: 2.5630, p: 0.01). The correlation between hope and ‘asking questions’ tended to be higher for less-educated parents as well ((group 1): r: 0.588, (group 2): r: 0.171, Z: 1.6237, p = 0.104). A linear regression analysis supported Fisher’s Z results.

4. Discussion

In our investigation of the differences in the mental reactions in relation to BBN conversations and the relationship with physicians based on parental education level, we observed that lower-educated parents reported less trust in physicians.
In addition, these parents felt less secure from the BBN conversation than did parents with higher levels of education. They also tended to say that physicians did not give them enough hope and they did not have enough opportunities to ask questions. We found no differences in mental condition in comparison, due to the different parental levels of education. The rate of those reporting elevated levels of depression and anxiety was higher than that of the general population in both groups. What stood out was the significantly increased ‘perceived helplessness’ for both groups compared to the norm sample. The transmission of safety and orientation through the BBN conversation as well as asking questions correlated highly for either educational group. Only among higher-educated parents did we find a correlation between safety and trust in physicians and an understanding of the content and trust. For less-educated parents, we did not find any correlation between the different aspects of the BBN conversation and their confidence. However, for them, the communication of hope seemed to be of greater importance, as hope here correlated strongly with safety and the opportunity to ask questions.
We can assume that less-educated parents have different needs for support to develop trust and may feel uncomfortable in a conversation with highly skilled physicians, perceiving an imbalance of power. These findings align with October et al. [45], who found that language disparities between healthcare teams and families in intensive care unit conferences could hinder family engagement and satisfaction with BBN. Limiting medical jargon could improve communication and satisfaction with BBN [45,46,47]. According to Rudd and Katz, lower (health) literacy also impairs understanding [48] and is associated with a passive communication style with physicians in which fewer or irrelevant questions are asked, challenging physicians to engage their counterparts as actively as possible using various conversational techniques [49]. In addition, higher educational levels may be associated with “similar world views about medical practice” (p. 11) [50] between parents and physicians, leading to greater trust, as reported by Krupat [50].
A look at the results of the correlation calculations can additionally complete the picture. Based on Grawe’s consistency theory, we initially hypothesized that specific aspects of the BBN conversation would influence the physician–parent relationship. However, we only observed this connection among higher-educated parents, with strong correlations between trust and a sense of safety and orientation. Apparently, for parents with lower levels of education, the BBN interview has a lesser effect on the relationship with the physicians than it does for higher-educated parents. Krupat found that patients with higher levels of education have a stronger preference for receiving information during medical consultations and thus develop a greater sense of control [50]. This, in turn, may affect trust in the interlocutor, as well as feelings of safety, and fits with our finding that better-educated parents reported feeling significantly safer as a result of the BBN interview. Another explanation may be that clinicians simply failed to create a sense of security in parents with lower levels of education during the BBN interview, resulting in a lack of correlation with confidence.
Even though for both groups the opportunity to ask questions correlated strongly with the safety and orientation provided by physicians, the less educated tendentially showed a stronger correlation between asking questions and orientation. Moreover, for less-educated parents, the ability to ask questions additionally correlated strongly with the hope conveyed. This once again underscores the need to work harder to ensure that less-educated parents in particular actively participate in the conversation and actually ask their questions [49]. For parents with lower levels of education, this seems to be even more important than actually understanding, which underscores the importance of this simple communication technique for achieving higher-level, desired goals such as hope, security, and orientation among parents. Hope, which is often highlighted as one of the main goals of successful BBN in general [15,51], only correlated with safety and orientation among less-educated parents. On the other hand, only among higher-educated parents did an understanding of the BBN content correlate with safety and orientation. In summary, then, in addition to encouraging questions, physicians should focus more on addressing the emotions in the case of parents with lower levels of education and on addressing their need for understanding in the case of parents with higher levels of education. For a brief overview of our recommendations for BBN in NICUs based on our findings, see Box 2).
The topic of hope is worth taking a closer look at. Undoubtedly, a type of diagnosis and its resulting prognosis are related to hope. Unfortunately, due to anonymous data collection, we were unable to validly relate an infant’s diagnosis as a confounding variable to parental responses about whether physicians conveyed hope. However, for the ‘demographic’ data, we found no significant difference in the parental assessment of the infant’s prognosis between the two groups (see Table 1), although tendentially, only lower-educated parents reported that physicians did not give them enough hope. There is evidence in the literature that the understanding of hope varies between physicians and parents, which reduces trust in physicians, complicates communication between each other, and may offer an explanation for why parents indicated that little hope was conveyed in the BBN conversation [52]. For parents, hope functions as an emotional motivator [52] and arises from the opening of a future perspective of whatever kind [53] and by focusing on the positive [54]. This must not be confused with a hope for healing that is far removed from reality, as is sometimes perceived by physicians [52].
Regarding physical and psychological well-being, both groups of parents reported similarly high levels of stress in the week following BBN and of depression and anxiety. So, we dealt with highly burdened parents but to a lesser extent than reported by Soghier et al. [32], who found levels of depression in NICU parents by up to 45%, possibly due to the different survey tools. Younge et al. [31] also found differences in stress scores among parents with different educational levels in NICUs, which may be due to the larger sample and a broader range of stress-related parameters that were collected. The detected differences only appeared in relation to parental and NICU-elicited stress parameters but not in the PSS-10. Nevertheless, the consistently high stress levels, mainly due to high reported helplessness, remind NICU staff of the importance of highly empathetic communication during and after BBN discussions, as highlighted by Enke et al. [12]. Regarding anxiety and depression, it should be noted that we collected the data shortly after BBN. To our knowledge, there are no data on the temporal evolution of anxiety and depression directly related to BBN in NICUs. However, data on postnatal anxiety and depression in parents in general show a decrease in psychological symptoms over time [55,56]. At the same time, socioeconomic status is reported to influence the likelihood of occurrence of anxiety and depression in the way that mothers with higher levels of education are at a lower risk for it. In this respect, perhaps a larger sample would have led to a somewhat different picture.
The high reported helplessness of all parents deserves special attention, since helplessness can indicate that basic psychological human needs, as stated by Grawe, may not have been met [35,36]. This is problematic, because a low satisfaction of needs makes it difficult to cope well with challenging borderline situations (e.g., [57]) and to provide good, supportive care for one’s own infant [9,58,59]. An infant’s stay in a NICU in itself is often a highly stressful situation for parents [60,61], even without bad news about newly diagnosed diseases, and helplessness-based liminality [62] shapes parental NICU experiences anyway. Thus, it is indispensable for the entire health care team to orient its interaction with parents so that they can experience and behave in a self-efficient and competent manner [11,58] and reduce helplessness by actively integrating parents in their daily care of their infants [10,63]. Related to the delivery of bad news, Fallowfield broadens the view to the entire NICU team in emphasizing that too often a BBN conversation is reduced to one informing physician while the surrounding team, with its ongoing communication before and after BBN discussions, is underappreciated and overlooked in their influence of parents [18].
There are some limitations that should be noted. Since medical complications of preterms may as well occur after the family has already spent weeks in a NICU, a trustful relationship may have already been built up, independent of the first breaking bad news conversation. Future studies should control for this in asking for trust in physicians before and after bad news has been broken or if BBN influenced trust in physicians at all from the parent’s point of view. In addition, we did not ask about mediating or moderating variables that influence the parameters we queried other than educational status or a conversation about bad news, such as general social support, certain attitudes, response tendencies, medical factors such as the number of pregnancies, or others. To control for this, the study population needs to be larger too. Moreover, more fathers should be included as well as parents with a primary educational level or no graduation to better represent this group. In general, future studies should include a larger sample to generalize results more validly.
Box 2. Recommendation box.
Recommendation box for the delivery of bad news in the NICU to evoke ‘safety’ and ‘orientation’ in parents
  • Limit medical jargon in BBN conversations wherever possible and use plain ‘everyday’ language.
  • Actively encourage parents to ask questions (i.e., “For many parents questions arise in such a situation. What are yours?”), especially lower-educated parents.
  • Focus more on addressing emotions among lower-educated parents and especially convey hope, e.g., by creating a future perspective.
  • In contrast, among higher-educated parents, focus more on explanations and their understanding of the content

5. Conclusions

Higher-educated parents generally reported more trust in physicians and a higher sense of safety elicited by the BBN conversation than less-educated parents. Therefore, future research should focus specifically on what factors might be important in developing trust for parents with lower levels of education. The ability to ask questions, which is highly correlated with safety and orientation, appeared to be vital for all parents, so physicians should pay particular attention to encouraging parents to ask their questions. Only among less-educated parents does safety correlate highly with hope, which is why physicians should take greater care to convey hope when delivering bad news to them. Because of the high level of parental helplessness, regardless of educational level, the general interaction of all NICU staff with parents should aim to reduce their helplessness in order to reduce symptoms of stress and thus strengthen their parenting. In the sense of stepwise research, the present exploratory work may lay a foundation for subsequent experimental studies on the delivery of bad news in neonatology.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: R.F.M., C.S. and M.W.; methodology: C.S. and M.W.; formal analysis: P.v.B. and M.W.; investigation: M.W.; data curation: M.W.; writing—original draft preparation: M.W.; writing—review and editing: P.v.B., R.F.M. and C.S.; supervision: P.v.B., R.F.M. and C.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Research involving human subjects complied with all relevant national regulations and institutional policies. This study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Helsinki Declaration (as revised in 2013) and has been approved by the authors’ Institutional Review Board of the Philipps University of Marburg (ID-No.: 164/18, date of approval: 13 December 2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all individuals included in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available out of ethical reasons and respect to participating families.

Acknowledgments

The authors especially want to thank all families willing to support this study while being in a very special state of their family´s history.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Structure of the used questionnaires.
Table A1. Structure of the used questionnaires.
Number of ItemsScaling
Demographic variables22 itemsYes/No/selection of various alternatives
Parents’ experiences with BBN
 Informative items of consultation7 itemsYes/No/selection of various alternatives
 Singular statements corresponding to ‘safety’/‘orientation’4 items1–4 Likert scale
Self-reported psychological instruments
 Condition after BBN consultation 5 items1–4 Likert scale
 PHQ-4 depression 2 items0–3 Likert scale
 PHQ-4 anxiety 2 items0–3 Likert scale
 PSS-10 perceived self-efficacy 4 items0–4 Likert scale (inverted)
 PSS-10 perceived helplessness 6 items0–4 Likert scale
 Satisfaction with physical and mental well-being 1 item1–4 Likert scale
 Perceived stress caused by situation/emotional burden 1 item0–10 Likert scale
 Overall self-estimation of self-efficacy 4 items1–4 Likert scale
Aspects of BBN conversation
 ‘Safety’9 items1–4 Likert scale
 ‘Orientation’8 item1-4 Likert scale
Relationship with physicians (trust)1 item0-10 Likert scale
Table A2. Items for well-being after BBN, current satisfaction with well-being, current perceived stress, current self-estimation of self-efficacy, and current relationship with physicians.
Table A2. Items for well-being after BBN, current satisfaction with well-being, current perceived stress, current self-estimation of self-efficacy, and current relationship with physicians.
ItemsScaling
Well-being and condition first few days after BBN
In the first few days after the first BBN conversation…
… I had trouble sleeping
…I worried a lot about the future
…I had difficulty concentrating
…I felt exhausted
…I was hopelessly
1: entirely, 2: pretty much, 3: a little, 4: not at all
Current satisfaction with physical and mental well-being after BBN
How satisfied are you with your physical and mental well-being? 1: entirely, 2: pretty much, 3: a little, 4: not at all
Current perceived stress caused by situation/emotional burden
Please tick on the scale how much you feel burdened by the problematic situation0: not at all, 10: extremely
Overall self-estimation of self-efficacy
How much do you agree with the following statement?
I am not afraid because of my baby’s illness/disability
I can integrate the problematic situation of my baby well into my life
I am satisfied with the course of my baby’s illness/disability
I am coping well overall with my baby’s illness/disability
1: entirely, 2: pretty much, 3: a little, 4: not at all
Current relationship with physicians
Please tick on the scale how much trust you have in the physicians treating your baby0: not at all, 10: very much
Table A3. Cronbach’s alpha for ‘safety’ and ‘orientation’.
Table A3. Cronbach’s alpha for ‘safety’ and ‘orientation’.
Items ‘Safety’nMSDα If Item Excluded
Physician conveyed safety in the conversation in that he/she…
…appeared competent (1)441.520.8490.915
…seemed to handle the situation/be up to the situation (2)441.410.7570.924
…showed understanding (3)441.750.9180.915
…radiated calmness (4)441.660.8610.930
…transported knowledge and overview to me (5)441.890.8130.915
…gave me information about the expected course of the disease/impairment (6)442.341.0980.934
…behaved friendly and compassionate (7)441.610.9200.913
…seemed honest and open (8)441.520.8490.914
…transported care for my baby and us as parents (9)441.840.8610.915
αa44 0.928
Items ‘orientation’nMSDα if item excluded
Physician conveyed orientation in the conversation in that he/she…
…asked for my state of knowledge at the beginning of the conversation b (1)412.661.0390.859
…provided as clear time specifications as possible (2)412.830.9720.850
…used x-rays, monitors, drawings, or other aids for better understanding (3)412.981.0840.845
…gave information on the development of the disease (4)412.711.1230.845
…informed as concretely as possible about the next therapy steps (5)412.171.1160.806
…justified the recommended therapy steps (6)412.341.1960.811
…gave information about possible effects on the development of my baby (7)412.441.0740.830
…responded to me and my questions (8)411.760.8880.832
α a41 0.853
Note. a Cronbach’s alpha (reliability). b This statement was excluded because five out of seven inter-item correlations were < 0.3, which maps a low discriminatory power, and α increased slightly if the item was excluded.

References

  1. Chawanpaiboon, S.; Vogel, J.P.; Moller, A.B.; Lumbiganon, P.; Petzold, M.; Hogan, D.; Landoulsi, S.; Jampathong, N.; Kongwattanakul, K.; Laopaiboon, M.; et al. Global, regional, and national estimates of levels of preterm birth in 2014: A systematic review and modelling analysis. Lancet Glob. Health 2019, 7, e37–e46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Christianson, A.; Howson, C.P.; Modell, B. March of Dimes—Global Report on Birth Defects—The Hidden Toll of Dying and Disabled Children [Internet]. White Plains, New York. 2006. Available online: https://www.marchofdimes.org/mission/march-of-dimes-global-report-on-birth-defects.aspx (accessed on 14 April 2022).
  3. O’Reilly, H.; Johnson, S.; Ni, Y.; Wolke, D.; Marlow, N. Neuropsychological Outcomes at 19 Years of Age Following Extremely Preterm Birth. Pediatrics 2020, 145, e20192087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Glass, H.C.; Costarino, A.T.; Stayer, S.A.; Brett, C.M.; Cladis, F.; Davis, P.J. Outcomes for extremely premature infants. Anesth. Analg. 2015, 120, 1337–1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Alkozei, A.; McMahon, E.; Lahav, A. Stress levels and depressive symptoms in NICU mothers in the early postpartum period. J. Matern. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2014, 27, 1738–1743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Caporali, C.; Pisoni, C.; Gasparini, L.; Ballante, E.; Zecca, M.; Orcesi, S.; Provenz, L. A global perspective on parental stress in the neonatal intensive care unit: A meta-analytic study. J. Perinatol. 2020, 40, 1739–1752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Pace, C.C.; Anderson, P.J.; Lee, K.J.; Spittle, A.J.; Treyvaud, K. Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms in Mothers and Fathers of Very Preterm Infants Over the First 2 Years. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2020, 41, 612–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Miles, M.S.R.; Holditch-Davis, D.R.; Schwartz, T.A.; Scher, M.M. Depressive Symptoms in Mothers of Prematurely Born Infants. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2007, 28, 36–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Smith, V.C.; Steelfisher, G.K.; Salhi, C.; Shen, L.Y. Coping with the neonatal intensive care unit experience: Parents’ strategies and views of staff support. J. Perinat. Neonatal Nurs. 2012, 26, 343–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Labrie, N.H.M.; van Veenendaal, N.R.; Ludolph, R.A.; Ket, J.C.F.; van der Schoor, S.R.D.; van Kempen, A.A.M.W. Effects of parent-provider communication during infant hospitalization in the NICU on parents: A systematic review with meta-synthesis and narrative synthesis. Patient Educ. Couns. 2021, 104, 1526–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Lorié, E.S.; Wreesmann, W.J.W.; van Veenendaal, N.R.; van Kempen, A.A.M.W.; Labrie, N.H.M. Parents’ needs and perceived gaps in communication with healthcare professionals in the neonatal (intensive) care unit: A qualitative interview study. Patient Educ. Couns. 2021, 104, 1518–1525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Enke, C.; Oliva, Y.; Hausmann, A.; Miedaner, F.; Roth, B.; Woopen, C. Communicating with parents in neonatal intensive care units: The impact on parental stress. Patient Educ. Couns. 2017, 100, 710–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Mause, L.; Hoffmann, J.; Reimer, A.; Dresbach, T.; Horenkamp-Sonntag, D.; Klein, M.; Scholten, N.; Neo-CamCare. Trust in medical professionals and its influence on the stress experience of parents of premature infants. Acta Paediatr. 2022, 111, 527–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Schmid Mast, M.; Kindlimann, A.; Langewitz, W. Recipients’ perspective on breaking bad news: How you put it really makes a difference. Patient Educ. Couns. 2005, 58, 244–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Bernstein, S.M.; Lemmon, M.E. Counseling parents of premature neonates on neuroimaging findings. Semin. Perinatol. 2021, 45, 151474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Baile, W.F.; Buckman, R.; Lenzi, R.; Glober, G.; Beale, E.A.; Kudelka, A.P. SPIKES—A Six-Step Protocol for Delivering Bad News: Application to the Patient with Cancer. Oncologist 2000, 5, 302–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Zwingmann, J.; Baile, W.F.; Schmier, J.W.; Bernhard, J.; Keller, M. Effects of patient-centered communication on anxiety, negative affect, and trust in the physician in delivering a cancer diagnosis: A randomized, experimental study. Cancer 2017, 123, 3167–3175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Fallowfield, L.; Jenkins, V. Communicating sad, bad, and difficult news in medicine. Lancet 2004, 363, 312–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Seifart, C.; Hofmann, M.; Bär, T.; Riera Knorrenschild, J.; Seifart, U.; Rief, W. Breaking bad news-what patients want and what they get: Evaluating the SPIKES protocol in Germany. Ann. Oncol. 2014, 25, 707–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Baird, G.; McConachie, H.; Scrutton, D. Parents’ perceptions of disclosure of the diagnosis of cerebral palsy. Arch. Dis. Child. 2000, 83, 475–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hvidberg, M.F.; Frølich, A.; Lundstrøm, S.L. Catalogue of socioeconomic disparities and characteristics of 199 chronic conditions—A nationwide register-based population study. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0278380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. WHO. AN Overview of Vulnerabilities and Risk Factors Risks to Mental Health: An Overview of Vulnerabilities and Risk Factors. Background Paper by WHO Secretariat for the Development of a Comprehensive Mental Health Action Plan. 2012. Available online: https://www.who.int/publications/m/item/risks-to-mental-health (accessed on 27 January 2023).
  23. Kieling, C.; Baker-Henningham, H.; Belfer, M.; Conti, G.; Ertem, I.; Omigbodun, O.; Rohde, L.A.; Srinath, S.; Ulkuer, N.; Rahman, A. Child and adolescent mental health worldwide: Evidence for action. Lancet 2011, 378, 1515–1525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Parkes, A.; Sweeting, H.; Wight, D. Parenting stress and parent support among mothers with high and low education. J. Fam. Psychol. 2015, 29, 907–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Sentenac, M.; Benhammou, V.; Aden, U.; Ancel, P.Y.; Bakker, L.A.; Bakoy, H.; Barros, H.; Baumann, N.; Funck Bilsteen, J.; Boerch, K.; et al. Maternal education and cognitive development in 15 European very-preterm birth cohorts from the RECAP Preterm platform. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2021, 50, 1824–1839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Sentenac, M.; Johnson, S.; Charkaluk, M.L.; Sëppanen, A.V.; Aden, U.; Cuttini, M.; Maier, R.; Mannamaa, M.; Zeitlin, J. Maternal education and language development at 2 years corrected age in children born very preterm: Results from a European population-based cohort study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2020, 74, 346–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Marlow, N.; Wolke, D.; Bracewell, M.A.; Samara, M. Neurologic and Developmental Disability at Six Years of Age after Extremely Preterm Birth. N. Engl. J. Med. 2005, 352, 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Ursache, A.; Noble, K.G. Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: Multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology 2017, 53, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Hugh, S.L.; Wong, S.P.S.; Liu, F.Y.B.; Hiu, L.W.; Tai, F.F.; Pak, C.N. Attitudes toward neonatal intensive care treatment of preterm infants with a high risk of developing long-term disabilities. Pediatrics 2009, 123, 1501–1508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Shankaran, S.; Lin, A.; Maller-Kesselman, J.; Zhang, H.; O’Shea, T.M.; Bada, H.S.; Kaiser, J.R.; Lifton, R.P.; Bauer, C.R.; Ment, L.R. Maternal Race, Demography, and Health Care Disparities Impact Risk for Imtraventricular Hemorrhage in Preterm Neonates. J. Pediatr. 2014, 164, 1005–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Younge, T.; Jacobs, M.; Tuchman, L.; Streisand, R.; Soghier, L.; Fratantoni, K. Sociodemographic risk factors, parental stress and social support in the neonatal intensive care unit. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2023, 108, 165–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Soghier, L.M.; Kritikos, K.I.; Carty, C.L.; Glass, P.; Tuchman, L.K.; Streisand, R.; Fratantoni, K.R. Parental Depression Symptoms at Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Discharge and Associated Risk Factors. J. Pediatr. 2020, 227, 163–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Seifart, C.; Falch, M.; Wege, M.; Maier, R.F.; Pedrosa Carrasco, A.J. NEO-SPEAK: A conceptual framework that underpins breaking bad news in neonatology. Front. Pediatr. 2022, 10, 1044210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Wege, M.; von Blanckenburg, P.; Maier, R.F.; Knoeppel, C.; Grunske, A. Do parents get what they want during bad news delivery in NICU? J. Perinat. Med. 2023, 51, 1104–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Dahlitz, M. Neuropsychotherapy: Defining the Emerging Paradigm of Neurobiologically Informed Psychotherapy. Int. J. Neuropsychother. 2015, 3, 47–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Pearson, C. Beyond Rapport: Forging the Therapeutic Alliance. J. Natl. Council Psychother. 2017, 1–14. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/321149528_Beyond_Rapport_Forging_the_Therapeutic_Alliance (accessed on 27 December 2022).
  37. Deffner, T.; Rosendahl, J.; Niecke, A. Psychotraumatological aspects in intensive care medicine. Nervenarzt 2021, 92, 81–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Löwe, B.; Wahl, I.; Rose, M.; Spitzer, C.; Glaesmer, H.; Wingenfeld, K.; Schneider, A.; Brähler, E. A 4-item measure of depression and anxiety: Validation and standardization of the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) in the general population. J. Affect. Disord. 2010, 122, 86–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Cohen, S.; Karnarck, T.; Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983, 24, 385–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Klein, E.M.; Brähler, E.; Dreier, M.; Reinecke, L.; Müller, K.W.; Schmutzer, G.; Wölfing, K.; Beutel, M.E. The German version of the Perceived Stress Scale—Psychometric characteristics in a representative German community sample. BMC Psychiatry 2016, 16, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988; pp. 1–567. [Google Scholar]
  42. Suurmond, R.; van Rhee, H.; Hak, T. Introduction, comparison and validation of Meta-Essentials: A free and simple tool for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 2017, 8, 537–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Jacobi, F.; Höfler, M.; Strehle, J.; Mack, S.; Gerschler, A.; Scholl, L.; Busch, M.A.; Maske, U.; Hapke, U.; Gaebel, W.; et al. Erratum zu: Psychische Störungen in der Allgemeinbevölkerung. Studie zur Gesundheit Erwachsener in Deutschland und ihr Zusatzmodul “Psychische Gesundheit” (DEGS1-MH)—Erratum to Mental disorders in the general population. Study on the health of adults in Germany. Nervenarzt 2016, 87, 88–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Domschke, K. Update on anxiety disorders—Current state and new developments. Nervenarzt 2021, 92, 415–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. October, T.W.; Dizon, Z.B.; Roter, D.L. Is it my turn to speak? An Analysis of the Dialogue in the Family-Physician Intensive Care Unit Conference. Patient Educ. Couns. 2018, 101, 647–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. McDonagh, J.R.; Elliott, T.B.; Engelberg, R.A.; Treece, P.D.; Shannon, S.E.; Rubenfeld, G.D.; Patrick, D.; Curtis, J. Family satisfaction with family conferences about end-of-life care in the intensive care unit: Increased proportion of family speech is associated with increased satisfaction. Crit. Care Med. 2004, 32, 1484–1488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Brouwer, M.A.; Maeckelberghe, E.L.M.; van der Heide, A.; Hein, I.M.; Verhagen, E.A.A.E. Breaking bad news: What parents would like you to know. Arch. Dis. Child. 2021, 106, 276–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Rudd, R.E. Health literacy skills of U.S. adults. Am. J. Health Behav. 2007, 31 (Suppl. S1), 8–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Katz, M.G.; Jacobson, T.A.; Veledar, E.; Kripalani, S. Patient literacy and question-asking behavior during the medical encounter: A mixed-methods analysis. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2007, 22, 782–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Krupat, E.; Bell, R.A.; Kravitz, R.L.; Thom, D.; Azari, R. When physicians and patients think alike: Patient-centered beliefs and their impact on satisfaction and trust. J. Fam. Pr. 2001, 50, 1057–1062. [Google Scholar]
  51. VandeKieft, G.K. Breaking bad news. Am. Fam. Physician 2001, 64, 1975–1978. [Google Scholar]
  52. Roscigno, C.I.; Savage, T.A.; Kavanaugh, K.; Moro, T.T.; Kilpatrick, S.J.; Strassner, H.T.; Grobman, W.A.; Kimura, R.E. Divergent Views of Hope Influencing Communications Between Parents and Hospital Providers. Qual. Health Res. 2012, 22, 1232–1246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Harnett, A. Informing Families of Their Child’s Disability: National Best Practice Guidelines: Consultation and Research Report. National Federation of Voluntary Bodies [NFVB]. 2007. October 2015. Available online: www.informingfamilies.ie (accessed on 22 April 2022).
  54. Leveton, M. Communicating with Children and Families: From Everyday Interactions to Skill in Conveying Distressing Information. Pediatrics 2008, 121, 31441–31460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Melnyk, B.M.M.; Crean, H.F.; Fischbeck Feinstein, N.; Fairbanks, E. Maternal Anxiety and Depression Following a Premature Infants’ Discharge from the NICU: Explanatory Effects of the COPE Program. Nurs. Res. 2008, 57, 383–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Pace, C.C.; Spittle, A.J.; Molesworth, C.M.-L.; Lee, K.J.; Northam, E.A.; Cheong, J.L.Y.; Davis, P.G.; Doyle, L.W.; Treyvaud, K.; Anderson, P.J. Evolution of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms in Parents of Very Preterm Infants During the Newborn Period. JAMA Pediatr. 2016, 170, 863–870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Hansen, E.; Zech, N. Nocebo Effects and Negative Suggestions in Daily Clinical Practice—Forms, Impact and Approaches to Avoid Them. Front. Pharmacol. 2019, 10, 77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Orzalesi, M.; Aite, L. Communication with parents in neonatal intensive care. J. Matern. Neonatal Med. 2011, 24 (Suppl. S1), 135–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Dyer, K.A. Identifying, understanding, and working with grieving parents in the NICU, part II: Strategies. Neonatal Netw. 2005, 24, 27–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Al Maghaireh, D.F.; Abdullah, K.L.; Chan, C.M.; Piaw, C.Y.; Al Kawafha, M.M. Systematic review of qualitative studies exploring parental experiences in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit. J. Clin. Nurs. 2016, 25, 2745–2756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Franck, L.S.; Cox, S.; Allen, A.; Winter, I. Measuring neonatal intensive care unit-related parental stress. J. Adv. Nurs. 2005, 49, 608–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Watson, G. Parental liminality: A way of understanding the early experiences of parents who have a very preterm infant. J. Clin. Nurs. 2011, 20, 1462–1471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Wigert, H.; Dellenmark Blom, M.; Bry, K. Parents’ experiences of communication with neonatal intensive-care unit staff: An interview study. BMC Pediatr 2014, 14, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Demographic and medical characteristics (total n = 54).
Table 1. Demographic and medical characteristics (total n = 54).
Test Statistics
1: Lower Secondary Education or Less a2: Upper Secondary Education or More aχ2 (1) bp-ValuePhi UZp-Value c
n%n%
Parental characteristics23433157
Age (years) (md (25th/75th p.)32 (29/35)34 (32/36) 251.00−1.8430.065
20–29730413
30–3915652477
40–4900310
50–591500
Sex FeT d0.7170.084
Father313619
Mother20872581
Marital Status FeT d0.5690.118
Single2913
Married/living with partner21913097
Citizenship FeT d0.3800.146
German22962787
Other14413
Pregnancy and delivery
Artificial fertilization (not specified)4172 (1)7 (3)FeT d0.3850.146
Antenatal fetal problems (not specified)6264 (2)13 (7)FeT d0.3070.155
Twin pregnancy (not specified)296 (1)19 (3)FeT d0.4430.195
Complication during pregnancy (not specified)104416 (1)52 (3)0.5060.477/0.5830.098
Complication during delivery (not specified)4 (2)19 (9)8 (2)26 (7)FeT d0.5170.110
Gestational age (GA) at time of birth
GA (weeks (25th/75th p.))33.5 (24.8/37.3)30.0 (26.0/39.5) 303.0−0.0980.922
≤27+6 (very early preterm)9391342
28+0–31+6 (early preterm)00310
32–36+6 (preterm)313310
>37+0 (mature born)1043929
Not specified14310
Condition of infant at time of answering the questionnaire according to parental estimation 341.5−0.3010.727
Very well/well19832271
Moderate313516
Child died0027
Not specified1427
Parents’ self-estimated prognosis at time of answering the questionnaire 344.5−0.4430.684
Very well/well17742477
Moderate/bad522413
Not specified14310
Note. a According to ISCED-2011. b χ2(df): degree of freedom. c Exact two-tailed. d FeT: Fishers exact test due to expected cell sizes of <5.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and group differences for clinical data and self-efficacy according to different levels of education.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and group differences for clinical data and self-efficacy according to different levels of education.
Scale/ItemScale MD/% bMin;MaxTest Statistics
RangeGroup an(Mean)(sd)UZ cpd d
Sum well-being and condition first few days after BBN
low values = good well-being
5–2012316.006;20325.50−0.5450.5860.138
23114.005;20
PHQ-4 depression
low values = no depressive symptoms
0–612119% b0;4301.00−0.2730.7850.012
23020% b0;6
PHQ-4 anxiety
low values = no symptoms of anxiety
0–612128.6% b0;4293.50−0.0100.992−0.088
22821.4% b0;6
PSS-10 perceived self-efficacy
low values = high self-efficacy
0–161218.00 (7.43)1;12 (2.749)263.00−0.6300.5290.288
2286.00 (6.68)0;13 (3.642)
PSS-10 perceived helplessness
low values = little helplessness
0–2412113.00 (13.67 e)8;22 (3.864)276.00−0.3650.715−0.141
22814.50 (14.36 e)4;24 (5.539)
Current satisfaction with physical and mental well-being
low values = high satisfaction with well-being
1–41202.50 1;4 238.50−0.7490.4540.181
2272.00 1;4
Current self-estimated stress caused by situation/emotional burden
low values = no stress
0–101225.001;10325.00−0.0930.9260.026
2305.501;10
Sum overall self-estimation of self-efficacy
low values = high self-efficay
4–161229.005;12315.00−0.0770.939−0.009
2298.004;12
Note. a 1: lower secondary education and less according to ISCED, revised 2011; 2: upper secondary education and higher according to ISCED, revised 2011. b Valid percentage exceeding the clinical cut-off of >3 for PHQ-4 depression and anxiety. c Mann–Whitney U test. d Cohen’s d for effect size. e >1sd higher than the mean of reference group (see [40]).
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and group differences for certain characteristics of the BBN conversation such as safety and orientation/knowledge and the relation to physicians according to different levels of education.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and group differences for certain characteristics of the BBN conversation such as safety and orientation/knowledge and the relation to physicians according to different levels of education.
Scale/ItemScale MDMin;MaxTest Statistics
Certain Aspects of BBN ConversationRangeGroup an UZ bpd c
‘I understood all explanations’ (orientation) d1–41232.001;4302.00−1.0440.2960.296
2311.001;4
‘I had sufficient opportunity for questions’ (orientation) d1–41221.501;4238.00−1.8580.0630.331
2291.001;4
‘Physician transported hope’ (safety) d1–41212.001;4211.00−1.7600.0780.562
2282.001;4
‘Physician gave me safety’ (safety) d1–41213.001;4190.00−2.1880.029 *0.654
2282.001;4
Sum ‘safety’
low values = physicians conveyed a lot of safety
9–3612115.009;35196.50−2.1360.033 *0.513
22913.009;33
Sum ‘orientation’ e
low values = physicians gave a lot of orientation
7–2811918.008;27201.50−1.4020.1610.399
22814.507;27
Relationship with physicians (trust)
low values = a lot of trust
0–101232.000;9244.50−2.0350.042 *0.598
2311.000;6
Note. a 1: lower secondary education and less according to ISCED, revised 2011; 2: upper secondary education and higher according to ISCED, revised 2011. b Mann–Whitney U test. c Cohen’s d. d (1: agree ‘entirely’, 4: agree ‘not at all’). e Due to low Cronbach’s alpha exclusion of one item for ‘orientation’ (see Appendix A Table A3). * p < 0.05.
Table 4. Correlation coefficients for certain characteristics of the BBN conversation concerning safety and orientation, and relation to physicians according to different levels of education.
Table 4. Correlation coefficients for certain characteristics of the BBN conversation concerning safety and orientation, and relation to physicians according to different levels of education.
UnderstandingAsking QuestionsHopeItem SafetySum ‘Safety’Sum ‘Orientation’
Group a121212121212
Asking questionsR b
(N)
0.321
(22)
0.373 *
(29)
____________________
Zc (p d)−0.196 (0.845)
HopeR b
(N)
0.048
(21)
−0.005
(28)
0.588 **
(21)
0.171
(28)
________________
Z c (p d)0.1716 (0.864)1.6237 (0.104)
Item safetyR b
(N)
0.074
(21)
0.467 *
(28)
0.560 **
(21)
0.579 **
(28)
0.763 **
(21)
0.208
(28)
____________
Z c (p d)−1.3978 (0.162)−0.0910 (0.928)2.5630 (0.01 **)
Sum ‘safety’R b
(N)
0.286
(21)
0.140
(29)
0.603 ** (21)0.390 *
(29)
0.598 **
(20)
0.194
(28)
0.702 **
(20)
0.710 **
(28)
________
Z c (p d)0.4999 (0.617)0.9329 (0.351)1.5700 (0.116)−0.0507 (0.960)
Sum ‘orientation’R b
(N)
0.493 *
(19)
0.172
(28)
0.820 **
(19)
0.508 **
(28)
0.492 *
(18)
0.222
(27)
0.659 **
(18)
0.683 **
(27)
0.784 **
(19)
0.848 **
(28)
____
Z c (p d)1.1441 (0.253)1.8640 (0.062)0.9508 (0.342)0.1716 (0.864)−0.1327 (0.894)
Relation (Trust)R b
(N)
−0.036
(23)
0.493 **
(31)
0.175
(22)
0.344
(29)
0.130
(21)
−0.081
(28)
0.198
(21)
0.715 **
(28)
0.125
(21)
0.583 **
(29)
0.187
(19)
0.584 **
(28)
Z c (p d)−1.9675 (0.049 *)−0.6024 (0.547)0.6856 (0.493)−2.2538 (0.024 *)−1.7655 (0.077)−1.4970 (0.134)
Note. a 1: lower secondary education and less according to ISCED, revised 2011; 2: upper secondary education and higher according to ISCED, revised 2011. b Spearmon rho. c Fisher’s Z. d p-value for Fisher’s Z (two-tailed). * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01 (two-tailed).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wege, M.; von Blanckenburg, P.; Maier, R.F.; Seifart, C. Does Educational Status Influence Parents’ Response to Bad News in the NICU? Children 2023, 10, 1729. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10111729

AMA Style

Wege M, von Blanckenburg P, Maier RF, Seifart C. Does Educational Status Influence Parents’ Response to Bad News in the NICU? Children. 2023; 10(11):1729. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10111729

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wege, Mirjam, Pia von Blanckenburg, Rolf Felix Maier, and Carola Seifart. 2023. "Does Educational Status Influence Parents’ Response to Bad News in the NICU?" Children 10, no. 11: 1729. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10111729

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop