Next Article in Journal
Sacral Bioneuromodulation: The Role of Bone Marrow Aspirate in Spinal Cord Injuries
Next Article in Special Issue
Discriminant Input Processing Scheme for Self-Assisted Intelligent Healthcare Systems
Previous Article in Journal
Intelligent Human–Computer Interaction: Combined Wrist and Forearm Myoelectric Signals for Handwriting Recognition
Previous Article in Special Issue
Attention-ProNet: A Prototype Network with Hybrid Attention Mechanisms Applied to Zero Calibration in Rapid Serial Visual Presentation-Based Brain–Computer Interface
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Overt Word Reading and Visual Object Naming in Adults with Dyslexia: Electroencephalography Study in Transparent Orthography

by
Maja Perkušić Čović
1,
Igor Vujović
2,
Joško Šoda
2,
Marijan Palmović
3 and
Maja Rogić Vidaković
4,*
1
Polyclinic for Rehabilitation of People with Developmental Disorders, 21000 Split, Croatia
2
Signal Processing, Analysis, and Advanced Diagnostics Research and Education Laboratory (SPAADREL), Faculty of Maritime Studies, University of Split, 21000 Split, Croatia
3
Laboratory for Psycholinguistic Research, Department of Speech and Language Pathology, University of Zagreb, 10000 Zagreb, Croatia
4
Laboratory for Human and Experimental Neurophysiology, Department of Neuroscience, School of Medicine, University of Split, 21000 Split, Croatia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Bioengineering 2024, 11(5), 459; https://doi.org/10.3390/bioengineering11050459
Submission received: 26 March 2024 / Revised: 1 May 2024 / Accepted: 2 May 2024 / Published: 4 May 2024

Abstract

:
The study aimed to investigate overt reading and naming processes in adult people with dyslexia (PDs) in shallow (transparent) language orthography. The results of adult PDs are compared with adult healthy controls HCs. Comparisons are made in three phases: pre-lexical (150–260 ms), lexical (280–700 ms), and post-lexical stage of processing (750–1000 ms) time window. Twelve PDs and HCs performed overt reading and naming tasks under EEG recording. The word reading and naming task consisted of sparse neighborhoods with closed phonemic onset (words/objects sharing the same onset). For the analysis of the mean ERP amplitude for pre-lexical, lexical, and post-lexical time window, a mixed design ANOVA was performed with the right (F4, FC2, FC6, C4, T8, CP2, CP6, P4) and left (F3, FC5, FC1, T7, C3, CP5, CP1, P7, P3) electrode sites, within-subject factors and group (PD vs. HC) as between-subject factor. Behavioral response latency results revealed significantly prolonged reading latency between HCs and PDs, while no difference was detected in naming response latency. ERP differences were found between PDs and HCs in the right hemisphere’s pre-lexical time window (160–200 ms) for word reading aloud. For visual object naming aloud, ERP differences were found between PDs and HCs in the right hemisphere’s post-lexical time window (900–1000 ms). The present study demonstrated different distributions of the electric field at the scalp in specific time windows between two groups in the right hemisphere in both word reading and visual object naming aloud, suggesting alternative processing strategies in adult PDs. These results indirectly support the view that adult PDs in shallow language orthography probably rely on the grapho-phonological route during overt word reading and have difficulties with phoneme and word retrieval during overt visual object naming in adulthood.

1. Introduction

Adult dyslexia affects about 4% of the population, while in childhood, it occurs in 5–17% of the pediatric population [1,2]. Dyslexia is defined as persistent reading difficulty affecting male and female subjects equally [2], and sensory deficits, cognitive deficits, lack of motivation, or lack of adequate reading instruction cannot explain its symptomatology. According to the International Dyslexia Association, dyslexia is defined as a specific learning disability with a neurobiological origin and is characterized by difficulties with accurate and/or fluent word recognition and poor spelling and decoding abilities [3]. These difficulties typically result from a deficit in the phonological component of language that is often unexpected in relation to other cognitive abilities [1]. Deficits at the level of the phonologic module impair the ability to segment the written words into their underlying phonologic elements [1]. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) classifies dyslexia as a form of neurodevelopmental disorder and defines dyslexia as a difficulty in learning to decode (read aloud) and to spell [4]. Dyslexia can occur across the IQ range, and poor decoding skills require the same kind of intervention regardless of IQ [4]. Therefore, according to DSM-5, the term “specific learning disability (difficulty)” has been replaced with “difficulty in learning to decode”. Secondary consequences may lead to problems in reading comprehension and reduced reading experience that can impede the growth of vocabulary and background knowledge [3]. The diagnostics of dyslexia are troublesome when comparing different countries with respect to the tools used in the definition and support for people with dyslexia (PD) at local and regional sites [5,6,7,8,9,10].
The fundamental prerequisite skills necessary for reading ability are phonological processing skills (phonological awareness and phonemic awareness) that begin to develop even before the beginning of formal schooling and continue through third grade and beyond. Phonological processing is usually described as having three main components that can be detected in PDs as: poor phonological awareness (the ability to access and manipulate speech sounds consciously), slow lexical retrieval evidenced in rapid automatized naming tasks [11,12], and poor verbal short-memory [13,14]. Some authors found that deficits in lexical retrieval [12,15], verbal short-term memory, and naming processing speed persist into adulthood [16,17]. Phonological deficits can be regarded as a valid explanation for dyslexia symptoms in a wide variety of spoken languages and writing systems [18], however, it is still a great challenge to differentiate underlying processes involved in word recognition and reading [19,20,21,22,23]. Reading can be explained as a complex cognitive process of decoding orthographic symbols (orthography) into corresponding graphemes/phonemes (phonology) of language to link these symbols with representations of meaning stored in mental lexicon (semantics) [24]. The final goal of the reading process is to comprehend decoded text [25]. Several reading computational models contributed to the understanding of the reading process: The dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud (DRC) [19], the triangle model [26], and the connectionist dual-process model (CDP) [27]. According to a mostly accepted DRC model [19], reading can be achieved through two distinct routes: the direct semantic route (ventral route) and the indirect grapho-phonological route (dorsal route). Furthermore, different language processing stages might impact reading and naming processes. Visual and semantic processing is required to accomplish the naming process while reading could be established directly through orthographic and phonological representations, particularly seen in shallow (transparent orthographies), a type of orthography where a word’s written and spoken forms are very similar [19,21]. Different underlying processes are involved in picture naming speed (e.g., visual complexity, frequency, name agreement, age of acquisition, etc.) [28]. Both frequency and age of acquisition influence picture naming speed [28,29]. Further, differences in language processing (especially evidenced in naming tasks) are strongly correlated with language transparency and orthography [30,31,32,33,34]. It is assumed that results on rapid automatized naming tasks can predict reading development in languages with shallow (transparent) orthographies [31,33], while phoneme awareness was found to be the strongest predictor of decoding skills in different languages [30].
Regarding findings of electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies, the most frequently reported are structural and functional impairments in dyslexia [34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47]. Aberrant activation reported for PD relates to the left hemisphere brain areas, including the cerebellar area, inferior frontal area, situated near Broca’s area, implicated in motor speech production, a peri-sylvian temporo-parietal region implicated in speech comprehension, and inferior temporo-occipital region (very often referred as the visual word form area) implicated in fast word decoding. Structural impairments in PD are associated with reduced grey matter volume in the left temporo-parietal cortex, decreased white matter connectivity between reading networks, and hypo-activation of the left temporo-occipital cortex and temporo-parietal cortex. Electroencephalographic (EEG) studies have mostly highlighted the role of lower frequency bands in dyslexia, especially an increase in delta and theta waves and a reduction in alpha and beta waves [48]. Homologous structures of abnormally activated hemisphere cortical regions are shown to be hyperactivated or under-activated in the right hemisphere [35,49,50,51,52]. Furthermore, studies employing event-related potentials (ERP) have provided the temporal course of activation of anatomical structures during specific reading tasks (overt and covert reading and naming tasks) [53,54,55,56,57]. In both reading and naming tasks, the analysis of visual characteristics of words/pictures most likely occurs between 50 and 100 ms after stimulus presentation, while the perception of the word/object shape begins at approximately 150 ms and is regarded as a pre-lexical stage [53,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66]. Further, frontal areas become active at approximately 180 ms, and semantic analysis (lexical stage) of words/objects begins at about 200 ms and unfolds around 500–600 ms involving the frontal and left superior temporal cortex and this phase is regarded as a lexical stage [57,60,63,65,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79]. The post-lexical stage occurs approximately after the first 500 ms and unfolds around 1000 ms [53,80,81,82,83].
The earliest signs of differences between PDs and healthy control subjects (HCs) during word reading were seen in the pre-lexical stage, approximately around 150 ms to 250 ms post-stimulus onset [21,84], usually indexed by N170 ERP component [21,40,56,84,85,86,87,88,89], in lexical stage around 250 to 600 ms and indexed by N320 [40,56] component and N400 [64,90,91,92] activated by repetition tasks or grapheme to phoneme conversion mechanisms. Differences in the post-lexical stage between PDs and HCs were seen approximately after the 500 ms to 1000 ms post-stimulus onset indexed by P600 ERP component [53,91,93,94] or late positive complex (LPC) [91,95] engaged during the repetition of words/pseudo words and articulatory processes.
In pre-lexical naming processes, PDs and HCs differ approximately between 170–200 ms to 400 ms post-stimulus (visual object) onset, indexed by N170 and N400 ERP components [54,63,96,97]. In the lexical stage of the naming process, the differences between PDs and HCs were seen around 250 ms to 450 ms indexed by N2, N3, and N400 ERP components [59,61,71,72,98,99,100] post-stimulus onset usually engaged in semantics or phonological processing. In the post-lexical stage of the naming process, the differences between PDs and HCs arise approximately after the first 500 ms [63,83,101,102,103] with subjects engaged in high level visual, conceptual processing.
The majority of ERP studies have been conducted on non-transparent orthographies (i.e., English, French, Portuguese) using covert reading and naming designs in adult PDs [35,40,64,81,87,89] and children with dyslexia [48,104] investigating pre-lexical, lexical and post-lexical stage of processing [35,40,47,59,64,81,87,89,91]. Lately, two studies on dyslexia have been conducted by using an overt design in adult PDs [56] and children with dyslexia [54] in non-transparent orthographies. Mahe et al. [56] investigated electrophysiological correlates of dyslexia in adult French PDs and expert readers engaged in reading aloud and lexical decision tasks. The study reported a similar electrophysiological pattern between groups in lexical decision tasks, while reduced ERP amplitudes in the left hemisphere were found approximately after the first 100 to 300 ms in adult PDs during reading aloud compared to the control group. In line with the study results, it was proposed that the overt reading task might be more suitable to tap underlying phonological deficits of adult PDs, while the lexical decision task would be more predictable to tap lexical access and word retrieval. Furthermore, Bakos et al. [54] investigated the underlying architecture of naming speed differences in 10-year-old children with reading or spelling disorders and typically developing children using digit naming tasks. The study reported reduced ERP amplitudes in the pre-lexical period of approximately 300 ms post-stimulus in the left hemisphere in the reading-disabled group but not in children with isolated spelling disorders.
Overall, research on reading and naming processes in PDs in languages with shallow (transparent) orthographies is still minor [84,105] with ongoing debate which underlying neurocognitive mechanisms are associated with phonological processing and how orthography can affect those processes [18]. The Croatian language is considered to be a language with shallow (transparent) orthography. But, beyond that, in everyday clinical practice, we see students who cannot easily overcome phonological difficulties and have reading and naming fluency problems that persist into adulthood. In line with that, we aimed to capture subtle differences in electrophysiological level in pre-lexical, lexical, and post-lexical processing stages in overt reading and naming in adult PDs compared to control subjects. As far as we know, the present ERP study is the first one exploring the reading and naming process in overt design in shallow orthography in adult PDs.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Twelve healthy adult control subjects (HCs) (mean age: 29.5 ± 8.9, 6 males, 6 females) and twelve adult participants with dyslexia (PD) (mean age: 22.08 ± 8.4, 6 males, 6 females) participated in the study. Due to technical issues (signal noise, bad signal from two or more electrodes, major motor artifacts), 2 HC subjects were excluded from electrophysiological data analysis. The final sample included 10 healthy HCs (mean age 27.8 ± 8.21, 6 females, 4 males) and 12 adult PDs (mean age 22.08 ± 8.4). No significant difference was found between age (t = −1.03; p = 0.31) and gender (χ2 = 0.22; p = 0.63) between two groups. HC and PD subjects voluntarily participated in the study and were native Croatian speakers. All participants were right-handed dominant, according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [106]. None of the participants had a history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, and they were free of other diagnoses (i.e., attention deficit disorder, dysphasia). PD subjects were diagnosed by a speech and language pathologist (SLP) during their early childhood education. They all had normal or corrected to normal vision. Prior to electrophysiological testing, participants’ cognitive abilities were tested by a cognitive nonverbal test [107], which showed normal findings. Both groups achieved nonverbal normal intelligence results. According to the Cognitive nonverbal test, average normal nonverbal intelligence was in the range −1z-value to 1z-value, 15 centile-85 centile, 40T-value-60T-value. Deviation IQ, according to the Wechsler scale, was 85 to 115. Both groups were within normal IQ findings.
The study procedure was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Polyclinic for Rehabilitation of People with Developmental Disorders, Split, Croatia, Class: 135-01/17-01/01, No.: 2181-164-17-01.

2.2. Materials and Stimuli

The stimuli were 64 words (for the word reading task) and their corresponding color drawings of objects (for visual object naming task) selected from Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart’s pictorial object set [108] and from Corel GalleryTMMagic [109]. Picture stimuli are provided in Supplementary Materials (Table S1). Words and corresponding pictures were taken from the Croatian word frequency dictionary [110] (Supplementary Materials Table S2). The length of target words varied from 4 to 8 phonemes and consisted of 2 to 4 syllables. Two reading/naming blocks started with /b/ and /d/ consonants, and two reading/naming blocks with consonants /k/ and /g/. The graphemes /b/, /d/, /g/, and /k/ are chosen because PDs in the Croatian language often make visual or auditive mistakes during reading [111]. Word and picture stimuli were divided into four blocks consisting of 16 words/pictures each (Figure 1).

2.3. Procedure

Participants were tested individually in a quiet room in the EEG laboratory of Polyclinic for People with Disabilities, Split, Croatia. They sat 60 cm in front of the computer screen. Pictures were presented in constant size of 9.5 cm × 9.5 cm on a white screen. Participants were asked to read words overtly and name the pictures overtly presented on the computer screen as fast and accurately as possible. Before the experiment, they were familiarized with the procedure during one practice trial block of words and pictures. The stimuli were presented using Presentation® software (Version 20.0, Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Berkeley, CA, USA, www.neurobs.com, accessed on 15 January 2018) [112]. An experimental trial had the following structure: fixation cross was presented for 850 ms, followed by the presentation of picture/word for 2000 ms, and finally, a blank screen was presented for 750 ms on a white background. The participants had 2000 ms to respond verbally in both experimental tasks. Experimental blocks consisted of 16 trials per block (four for word reading and four for visual object naming). There were 64 trials in the word reading task and 64 trials in the visual object naming task for each participant, in sum 128 trials for both conditions.
Figure 1 presents a single trial for word reading and visual object naming tasks. Recording of the subject’s response started at zero when the word/picture was presented to the subject (microphone symbol, Figure 1). The order of picture naming blocks and word reading blocks was randomized across participants. The entire experimental session lasted approximately 45 min for each subject.

2.4. ERP Acquisition and Pre-Processing

Continuous electroencephalographic (EEG) activity was recorded with electrodes mounted in an elastic cap (Figure 2) from 22 sites placed according to the International 10/20 system (Natus® EEG32U™, NicoletOneTM EEG system, Planegg, Germany). Participants were asked to avoid eye blinks and to be calm during recording. One additional electrode was placed at the left mastoid for online referencing. A technician directly marked reading and naming epochs on EEG, while the subject’s responses for reading and naming tasks were recorded simultaneously and stored for offline analysis. The recording was controlled using Nicolet EEG study room software (version 5.91.0.248). Two additional electrodes were placed on the participants’ heads (EOG1, EOG2) near the left and right eye for online referencing, and one was placed on the participants’ nose as an off-line reference channel. Furthermore, the EEG was continuously recorded and digitized at 250 Hz, the impedance was kept below 25 kΩ, and the sensitivity was 70 microvolts.
The complete EEG analysis was performed using EEGLAB (version 15.x (dev)) [113] integrated into MATLAB software (9.4.0.949201 (R2018a)) [114]. Before the segmentation, the EEG signal was filtered with a bandpass filter between 1 and 30 Hz. Removing bad channels was performed using the pop_rejchan (EEG) code implemented in MATLAB [114]. In spectral eegplot view, a visual inspection of bad channels proposed to remove was performed. If the proposed channels had an abnormal appearance, abnormal distributions, having major artifacts, noise was removed. After removing bad channels that were identified with spectrum criteria, all data were re-reference again but without rejected channels. Furthermore, epochs of interest were extracted. Bad segments in epochs of interest were removed using code pop_rejmenu (EEG, 1) and an independent component analysis (ICA) algorithm implemented in MATLAB software [114]. From a large variety of ICA algorithms for the purpose of this study, runica (), a function for automated infomax ICA decomposition, was selected [115].
Epochs that consisted of events of interest (related to reading and naming responses) were extracted. Epochs without response (no-response/answer), incorrect response, or having major motor artifacts were excluded from further analysis. The 1999 ms long epochs were averaged in reference to the 1000 ms pre-stimulus baseline correction to ensure that segmented epochs will start with reading and naming epochs and to avoid motor artifacts in epochs without answer and epochs with incorrect answers. Overall ERP analysis consisted of 45–64 epochs (mean 56) in HCs, 45–64 epochs (mean 58) in PDs for word reading task, 40–64 epochs (mean 56) in HCs and 54–64 (mean 59) in PDs for visual object naming task. The number of averaged trials and electrodes did not differ between groups.
The present ERP study explored pre-lexical (150–260 ms), lexical (280–700 ms), and post-lexical stages of processing (750–1000 ms) in both overt reading and naming tasks [40,53,54,56,59,83,102,116,117]. The spatial and temporal topographical analysis of ERPs for reading and naming was conducted using parametric statistical methods incorporated in EEGLAB software. It used multiple channel selection of ERP plotting format in specified time windows. Reading was explored in a pre-lexical stage in the time window 160–260 ms [21,40,56,84,87], a lexical stage in a time window 450–700 ms [40,56,64,90,117], and a post-lexical stage in time window 750–900 ms [53,91,94,116]. Naming processing was explored in pre-lexical stage 150–200 ms [54,63,96,97], lexical stage 280–440 ms [59,71,72,99,100,117] and post-lexical stage 900–1000 ms [63,83,102,103,116].
Furthermore, reading and naming ERPs plotted in a scalp array were plotted in EEGLAB software using the analyzed time windows. Before plotting, ERPs were normalized to spectrum criteria using data statistics incorporated in EEGLAB software. Before displaying topographic maps, all channels were checked due to abnormal appearance, abnormal values, abnormal distributions, abnormal trends, and abnormal spectra.
Behavioral responses on reading and naming reaction time (response latency) were estimated in Praat (Version 5.3.56) [118] from the onset of the stimulus (word or visually presented object) to the onset of the subject’s verbal response (Figure 3). Verbal response longer than 2000 ms and wrong answers provided by subjects were excluded from further analysis.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Skewness and kurtosis parameters were tested for the individually averaged evoked response potential ERP amplitude at each electrode, time point, and reading and naming response latency results. Results indicated acceptable values for the parametric statistic. Descriptive statistics of relevant parameters were summarized by N, mean, and standard deviations. The chi-square test (χ2) was used to determine differences in qualitative error frequency between groups. Mean value comparisons were tested using the T-test. For the analysis of the mean ERP amplitude for pre-lexical, lexical, and post-lexical time window, a mixed design Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed with Hemisphere (Left vs. Right) (Right: F4; FC2; FC6; C4; T8; CP2; CP6; P4; vs. Left: F3; FC5; FC1; T7; C3; CP5; CP1; P7; P3) as within-subject factors and Group (Control vs. Dyslexia) as between-subject factor. The effect size was presented as Cohen’s d (t-test), eta squared η2 (ANOVA) and Yule’s Q (chi-square test). Fisher LSD post hoc test was further calculated. In all calculations, a p-value of <0.05 was considered statistically significant. Data analyses were performed using the software Statistica Soft 12.

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral Results

Qualitative analysis showed that PDs made significantly more errors in the word reading task (2.47%) compared to HCs (0.52%) (χ2 = 9.93, p < 0.01, Q = 0.6) and significantly more errors in the visual object naming task (35.54%) compared to HCs (24.60%) (χ2 = 21.84, p < 0.001, Q = 0.2) (Table 1). In the word reading task, PDs made significantly more phonological errors (1.43%) compared to HCs (0.52%) (χ2 = 4.61, p = 0.03, Q = 0.5). In the visual object naming task, PDs provided significantly more “no answer” (10.02%) compared to HCs (1.72%) (χ2 = 48.34, p < 0.001, Q = 0.7) (Table 1). Behavioral analysis of the response latency revealed significant differences in word reading latency (t = 17.81; p < 0.001; d = 3.14) between PD and HC subjects, with PDs having prolonged reading latency compared to HCs (t = 0.16; p = 0.86). (Table 2).

3.2. ERP Results

The topographic view of the electric signal of word reading ERPs (Figure 4) and visual object naming ERPs (Figure 5) are presented in PD and HC subjects’ pre-lexical, lexical, and post-lexical time windows. Descriptive ERP data for amplitude values of the electric signal in the pre-lexical, lexical, and post-lexical window are provided in Supplementary Materials (Table S3). Topographic plots of ERP data were normalized to mean amplitudes of the electric signal in microvolts in respective time windows.

3.2.1. Reading Task—ERP Results

Notable ERP amplitude differences concerning right vs. left hemisphere for pre-lexical (Fhemisphere = 13.09, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.39; Fgroup x hemisphere = 4.79, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.19), lexical (Fhemisphere = 12.50, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.38), and post-lexical (Fhemisphere = 6.82, p = 0.009, η2 = 0.25) time window were found for word reading (Table 3).
The Fisher LSD post hoc test further revealed differences in right hemisphere ERPs between HC and PD subjects for a pre-lexical time window (p = 0.02) (Figure 6 and Figure 7) (Table 3). Furthermore, a significant difference was calculated for HC between right and left hemisphere potentials for lexical (p = 0.01) and post-lexical (p = 0.004) time window and for PD group for pre-lexical (p < 0.001) and lexical (p = 0.01) time window (Table 3). Grand averaged reading ERPs of ROI electrodes in the pre-lexical stage are provided in Supplementary Materials (Figures S1 and S2).

3.2.2. Naming Task—ERP Results

Results for ERPs indicate significant differences between groups and hemispheres for pre-lexical (Fhemisphere = 17.03, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.46), lexical (Fhemisphere = 7.94, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.28), and post-lexical (Fgroup = 6.19, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.23; Fhemisphere = 5.62, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.21) time window for naming task (Table 4). The Fisher LSD post hoc test revealed significant differences for right hemisphere ERPs between HCs and PDs for the post-lexical time window (p = 0.02) (Figure 8 and Figure 9) (Table 4). Furthermore, a significant difference was calculated for HC between right and left hemisphere potentials for pre-lexical (p = 0.04) and post-lexical (p = 0.02) time window and for PDs for pre-lexical (p < 0.001) and lexical (p = 0.02) time window (Table 4). Grand averaged naming ERPs of ROI electrodes in the post-lexical window are provided in Supplementary Materials (Figures S3 and S4).

4. Discussion

The main findings of the present study revealed ERP mean amplitude differences between PDs and HCs in the right hemisphere in the pre-lexical time window (160–200 ms) for word reading aloud and in the post-lexical window (900–1000 ms) for visual object naming aloud.
Behavioral analysis of the word reading latency and naming proved significantly prolonged word reading latency (t = 17.81; p < 0.001) between PDs and HCs subjects, which is similar to findings of previously reported studies [36,56,87]. For the naming response latency, no differences were found between PDs and HCs, similar to previously reported findings [34,36].

4.1. Overt Reading of Words and ERP

Reading aloud requires an explicit grapho-phonological route, also called a non-lexical route [19]. As people progress in reading, they rely more on the semantic route (lexical route) [19]. However, adult PDs probably rely more on grapho-phonological route [15,19,40]. According to cross-cultural studies, reading and naming deficits in developmental dyslexia can be affected by orthographic depth and orthographic consistency [84,119]. Carioti et al. [119] reported that orthographic depth and consistency might have an impact on the manifestations and symptoms of adult PDs in deep (non-transparent) and shallow (transparent) orthographies. Regarding orthographic depth and consistency, the Croatian language is considered a language with shallow (transparent) orthography, such as Italian or Greek. Beyond transparency, the Croatian language has a complex syllabic system, double graphemes, and graphemes with diacritics [111].
The present study shows that the word reading mean ERP amplitude for the pre-lexical time window (160–260 ms) was significantly increased over the right hemisphere for adult PDs compared to HCs. Previous ERP studies reported that grapho-phoneme conversion takes place in the pre-lexical window approximately 160–260 ms post-stimuli [40,56,120]. In comparison to the present study results, Mahe et al. [56] found significantly reduced ERP amplitudes in the pre-lexical stage (100 ms post-stimulus) over the left hemisphere in adult PDs compared to HCs during overt word and pseudo words reading in non-transparent French language. Further, previously reported studies on non-transparent languages investigating the pre-lexical stage of processing to orthographic stimuli (reading words) vs. non-orthographic stimuli using covert design (lexical decision judgment task) found greater activation in the right hemisphere for orthographic stimuli in adult PDs [40,87,89,121], as well as in children with dyslexia [54]. The design applied in the present study consisted of sparse neighborhoods with closed phonemic onset (words sharing the same onset, i.e., b–d, g–k) [122,123] that might have had an impact on the prolonged duration of phoneme code retrieval in adult PDs in the pre-lexical stage of processing during overt reading. Further, it is assumed that adult PDs might use an alternative strategy of decoding processes relying on the right hemisphere in the pre-lexical time window [56,121]. Also, according to previously reported findings, adult PDs might have persistent phonological processing difficulties during reading and exhibit a lower score in rapid naming, working memory, and visual-attention [84,124,125,126]. Contrary to PDs, HCs use more efficiently a direct lexical route in decoding familiar words during word reading, while adult PDs rely on a non-lexical route and require extra time for decoding, similar to the decoding process seen in dyslexic children [126] as well in adult PDs [127,128,129]. However, several ERP studies conducted on non-transparent languages using covert design (delayed phonological task, auditory lexical decision task) failed to find differences in the pre-lexical processing stage in adult PDs compared to controls [127,128]. The differences in adult PDs compared to HCs were reported in the post-lexical stage after the first 300 ms in the left hemisphere with reduced ERP amplitudes to words compared to pseudo words during the delayed phonological task and auditory lexical decision task [127,128]. Contrary to these findings, Mahe et al. [56] failed to find differences in the pre-lexical stage of processing during lexical decision task in adult PDs compared to HCs on non-transparent language by using lexical decision task, assuming that overt (reading aloud) task might be more suitable to investigate phonological processing compared to lexical decision task.
To summarize, phonological abilities and reading outcomes are strongly associated in transparent and non-transparent orthographies, but still, there is a gap in cross-cultural studies in investigating systematic differences in the orthographic and phonological characteristics of the languages in dyslexia [18]. According to Zoccolotti [130], future studies could design the type of phonological task associated with specific language orthography to ensure the replicability and comparison of results that would eventually lead to understanding an underlying phonological mechanism in languages with transparent and non-transparent orthographies.

4.2. Overt Visual Object Naming and ERP

Regarding Levelt’s model of visual word production [22], visual object naming tasks can be divided into different stages. During the pre-lexical stage of processing, within first 150 ms, the brain is engaged in visual processing, lexical stage lemma selection (150–275 ms), post-lexical stage phonological encoding (275–400 ms), and articulatory processing (after the first 500 ms). It is believed that reading and naming might share similar neurocognitive processes (word/picture retrieval, storing phonological information, visual processing, semantic analysis) [36,96]. In the reading process, phonological information is retrieved from orthography, compared to visual object naming, where phonological information is retrieved from the semantic system [19,36,131]. During visual object naming, the brain is engaged in the selection of semantically related names that are competing with each other (names/words represented or belonging to different lexical categories), while during the reading of words, only one phonological information can be uttered [132]. Further, orthographic–phoneme conversion during word reading is faster than semantic conversion in naming processing [19]. Furthermore, different underlying neurocognitive mechanisms might influence naming speed during visual object naming, such as, e.g.., the lexicality effect [57,132], phonological neighborhood effects (dense/sparse objects) [122,123], visual processing [133], and recognition memory [81,134,135].
The majority of ERP studies investigating naming processing of adult PDs and children with dyslexia were conducted on non-transparent orthographies using covert lexical decision task with subjects engaged in recognizing visually presented objects, false fonts, strings, visual recognition memory of previously seen words/pseudo words, graphic symbols [81,89,136,137]. These studies found reduced amplitudes of the electric signal in the post-lexical stage (i.e., 500–900 ms) in the left and right hemisphere in adult PDs compared to HCs during visual recognition memory of previously seen/learned words/pseudo words, graphic symbols, and false fonts/consonant strings [81,89,136,137]. Further, regarding ERP studies investigating the naming process of adult PDs and children in the overt design, according to our knowledge, several studies reported findings on non-transparent orthographies in adult PDs [129,133] and children [54]. Araújo, et al. [133] investigated the relationship between how related and unrelated objects were processed on the visual, phonological, and semantic level in adult Portuguese PDs engaged in the sequential covert and overt naming task. The differences were found at a phonological level in a post-lexical stage in the right hemisphere after, i.e., the first 400 ms during overt naming of phonemic related pictures vs. covert naming of phonemic unrelated pictures [133]. Furthermore, Perera et al. [129] reported distributed electrical activity bilaterally over the parieto-occipital region in the left and right hemispheres during overt rapid naming in adult PDs compared to HCs. In the present study, the ERP amplitudes in the post-lexical time window (900–1000 ms) were significantly reduced in the right hemisphere for adult PDs compared to HCs in the overt visual object naming task, similar to the findings of Araujo et al. [133] reporting reduced ERP amplitudes in the right hemisphere in the post-lexical stage during overt naming of phonemic related vs. unrelated pictures in adult PDs compared to HCs. Similar findings of the present study with findings of Araujo et al. [133] could be explained by the previously reported findings of greater ERP amplitudes in the post-lexical stage during covert visual recognition memory [134] of pictures vs. words [135] and overt naming pictures [63] in non-transparent languages in HCs. Therefore, reduced ERP amplitudes in adult PDs compared to HCs during covert visual object naming task could indicate that PDs might use different strategies in recollection processes of previously seen words, pseudo words, objects, and pseudo-objects [81,89,105,134,136], even though behavioral differences in naming response latency were not consistently evident [34,36]. Also, in overt studies, pre-lexical grapho-phonological processes and post-lexical articulatory processes can be assumed to be slightly shifted to later stages in PDs and HCs in transparent and non-transparent orthographies [53,55,56,133]. Therefore, during post-lexical naming process, adult PDs might also probably use the indirect grapho-phonological route to tap phonology and semantics [95].

4.3. Study Limitations and Future Directions

There are several limitation factors of the present study, such as the relatively small sample of subjects included in the study (total of 24 subjects) and the small sample of electrodes used for ERP recording. The sample size was determined according to the number of participants included in previously reported ERP studies investigating reading and naming processes in PDs and HCs [62,73,81,83,136,138]. In future studies, according to G*Power 3.1., a sample of at least of 34 subjects in both independent group (dyslexia and control) is recommended to obtain a power of the test of 80%. However, comparable to the present study, several previously reported studies used a similar sample of electrodes, i.e., [57,81,105,121], and included a similar number of PD subjects, i.e., [81,83,138] as in the present study. Further, prior to ERP testing, participants underwent psychological testing and hand dominance tests, while standardized reading tests for adult PDs in Croatian were not available (no available standardized tests in Croatian language for PD subjects). Regarding behavioral measures for inclusion criteria of the HC participants in other ERP studies, few of them used hand dominance and neuropsychological tests similar to the present study [80,99,100]. Furthermore, due to only several previously published studies investigating overt word reading and visual object naming processes in adult PDs in non-transparent languages [56,129,133], make it difficult to compare with the present study’s results. Concerning other methodological issues, the set of words and pictures corresponded (sharing phonological information) in both visual object naming and reading tasks. A similar design has already been used in studies investigating overlapping processes between reading and naming tasks [36,132].
The future ERP studies could further observe cross-cultural electrophysiological correlates of dyslexia in adult PDs in the pre-lexical, lexical, and post-lexical stages of reading and naming processes in other shallow orthographies using overt design and compare these findings with findings for non-transparent orthographies. Also, future studies in different orthographies might observe the multidimensional approach analyzing different neurocognitive processes (rapid automatized naming, word retrieval, visual attention), not solely phonological processing in dyslexia [15,18,84,129,130,131].

5. Conclusions

The present ERP study aimed to investigate differences in electrophysiological correlates in the overt reading of words and visual object naming in PDs compared to HCs in the pre-lexical, lexical, and post-lexical processing stages. The results point to the differences in the pre-lexical time window (160–260 ms) in overt reading and the post-lexical time window (900–1000 ms) in overt visual object naming in the right hemisphere in adult PDs compared to HCs. In adult PDs, greater ERP amplitudes were distributed in the right hemisphere in the pre-lexical window in overt, while lowered ERP amplitudes were detected in PDs in the right hemisphere during visual object naming. These study results are supported by findings from two overt studies conducted on non-transparent orthographies reporting greater amplitudes of the electric signal in the right hemisphere in the pre-lexical stage during overt word reading in adult French PDs compared to HCs [56] and reduced amplitudes of the electric signal in post-lexical stage in the right hemisphere during overt naming of phoneme related pictures in adult Portuguese PDs compared to HCs [132].
Overall, the findings of the present study support the view that adult PDs in shallow (transparent orthography) (Croatian language) applying overt design have difficulties: (a) in the pre-lexical stage associated with grapho-phonological processing during reading of words with a combination of sparse neighborhood phonemes, and (b) difficulties in post-lexical stage possible associated with phoneme and word retrieval during overt naming of objects with a combination of sparse neighborhood phonemes.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/bioengineering11050459/s1, Table S1: Naming blocks (picture stimuli); Table S2: Reading blocks (word stimuli); Table S3: Descriptive means and standard deviations based on observation of amplitudes (μV) for different time windows; Figure S1: Grand average ERPs from ROI electrodes in overt reading task; Figure S2: Schematic view of two ROI electrodes in overt reading task; Figure S3: Grand average ERPs from ROI electrodes in overt object naming task; Figure S4: Schematic view of two ROI electrodes in overt object naming task.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.P.Č. and M.P.; methodology M.P.Č. and M.P.; software M.P.Č. and M.P.; validation, M.R.V. and J.Š.; formal analysis, M.P.Č. and M.R.V.; investigation, M.P.Č.; resources, M.P.Č., M.P., I.V., M.R.V. and J.Š.; data.; writing—original draft preparation, M.P.Č. and M.R.V.; writing—review and editing, M.P.Č., M.R.V., M.P., I.V., J.Š. and M.R.V.; visualization, M.P.Č., M.R.V., J.Š. and I.V.; supervision M.R.V., M.P., I.V. and J.Š. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study procedure was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Polyclinic for Rehabilitation of People with Developmental Disorders, Put Meja 5, 21000 Split, Croatia (10 October 2017), Class: 135-01/17-01/01, No.: 2181-164-17-01.

Informed Consent Statement

All subjects gave informed consent for inclusion before participating in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available with a granted proposal upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We especially thank Zoran Vujnović, the head of the Department for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry of the Polyclinic for Rehabilitation of People with Developmental Disorders in Split for supporting this study, Tonka Šamadan Vukojević in assisting ERP recordings, Mate Jurić from the Department of Information Sciences, University of Zadar for valuable advices regarding data analysis as well as Siniša Čović for EEG support in data analysis. Furthermore, we especially thank Dajana Knežević, SLP in ERP assisting and regrutation of subjects.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Soriano-Ferrer, M.; Piedra Martínez, E. A review of the neurobiological basis of dyslexia in the adult population. Neurologia 2017, 32, 50–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, J.E.; Shaywitz, B.A. Dyslexia in the 21st century. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2021, 34, 80–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. International Dyslexia Association. Available online: https://dyslexiaida.org/definition-of-dyslexia/ (accessed on 12 December 2023).
  4. Snowling, M.J.; Hulme, C.; Nation, K. Defining and understanding dyslexia: Past, present and future. Oxf. Rev. Educ. 2020, 46, 501–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Poulsen, M.; Juul, H.; Elbro, C. A national test of dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 2023, 73, 337–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Thongseiratch, T.; Traipidok, P.; Charleowsak, P.; Kraiwong, T.; Geater, A.F. Development and diagnostic accuracy of dyslexia early identification flowchart for pediatric practice. Asian J. Psychiatr. 2023, 89, 103795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Reynolds, A.E.; Caravolas, M. Evaluation of the Bangor Dyslexia Test (BDT) for use with Adults. Dyslexia 2016, 22, 27–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Hou, F.; Qi, L.; Liu, L.; Luo, X.; Gu, H.; Xie, X.; Li, X.; Zhang, J.; Song, R. Validity and Reliability of the Dyslexia Checklist for Chinese Children. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1909–1915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Jap, B.A.J.; Borleffs, E.; Maassen, B.A.M. Towards identifying dyslexia in Standard Indonesian: The development of a reading assessment battery. Read. Writ. 2017, 30, 1729–1751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Tamboer, P.; Vorst, H.C. A new self-report inventory of dyslexia for students: Criterion and construct validity. Dyslexia 2015, 21, 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Denckla, M.B.; Rudel, R.G. Rapid a ‘utomatized’ naming (R.A.N): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia 1976, 14, 471–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Wolf, M. Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Read. Res. Q. 1991, 26, 123–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Majerus, S.; Cowan, N. The nature of verbal short-term impairment in dyslexia: The importance of serial order. Front. Psychol. 2016, 3, 1507–1522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Turkeltaub, P.E.; Coslett, H.B. Localization of sublexical speech perception components. Brain Lang. 2010, 114, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Felton, R.H.; Naylor, C.E.; Wood, F.B. Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics. Brain Lang. 1990, 39, 485–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Avons, S.E.; Hanna, C. The memory-span deficit in children with specific reading disability: Is speech rate responsible? Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 1995, 13, 303–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Snowling, M.J.; Goulandris, N.; Defty, N. A longitudinal study of reading development in dyslexic children. J. Educ. Psychol. 1996, 88, 653–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Navas, A.L.; FerrazÉde, C.; Borges, J.P. Phonological processing deficits as a universal model for dyslexia: Evidence from different orthographies. Codas 2014, 26, 509–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Coltheart, M.; Rastle, K.; Perry, C.; Langdon, R.; Ziegler, J. DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 2001, 108, 204–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Indefrey, P. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: A critical update. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Indefrey, P.; Levelt, W.J. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition 2004, 92, 101–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Levelt, W.J. Models of word production. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1999, 3, 223–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Taylor, J.S.; Rastle, K.; Davis, M.H. Can cognitive models explain brain activation during word and pseudoword reading? A meta-analysis of 36 neuroimaging studies. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 766–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Hauk, O.; Coutout, C.; Holden, A.; Chen, Y. The time-course of single word reading: Evidence from fast behavioral and brain responses. Neuroimage 2012, 60, 1462–1477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Landi, N.; Frost, S.J.; Menc, W.E.; Sandak, R.; Pugh, K.R. Neurobiological bases of reading comprehension: Insights from neuroimaging studies of word level and text level processing in skilled and impaired readers. Read. Writ. Q. 2013, 29, 145–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Harm, M.W.; Seidenberg, M.S. Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 111, 662–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Perry, C.; Ziegler, J.C.; Zorzi, M. Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 2007, 114, 273–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Alario, F.X.; Ferrand, L.; Laganaro, M.; New, B.; Frauenfelder, U.H.; Segui, J. Predictors of picture naming speed. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 2004, 36, 140–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Barry, C.; Morrison, C.M.; Ellis, A.W. Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart Pictures: Effects of Age of Acquisition, Frequency, and Name Agreement. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 1997, 50, 560–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Caravolas, M.; Volín, J.; Hulme, C. Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2005, 92, 107–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. de Jong, P.F.; van der Leij, A. Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. J. Educ. Psychol. 1999, 91, 450–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Georgiou, G.K.; Torppa, M.; Landerl, K.; Desrochers, A.; Manolitsis, G.; de Jong, P.F.; Parrila, R. Reading and Spelling Development Across Languages Varying in Orthographic Consistency: Do Their Paths Cross? Child. Dev. 2020, 91, e266–e279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Wimmer, H.; Mayringer, H.; Landerl, K. The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. J. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 92, 668–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Trauzettel-Klosinski, S.; Dürrwächter, U.; Klosinski, G.; Braun, C. Cortical activation during word reading and picture naming in dyslexic and non-reading-impaired children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006, 117, 1085–1097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Hoeft, F.; Meyler, A.; Hernandez, A.; Juel, C.; Taylor-Hill, H.; Martindale, J.L.; McMillon, G.; Kolchugina, G.; Black, J.M.; Faizi, A.; et al. Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 4234–4239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. McCrory, E.J.; Mechelli, A.; Frith, U.; Price, C.J. More than words: A common neural basis for reading and naming deficits in developmental dyslexia? Brain 2005, 128 Pt 2, 261–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Paulesu, E.; Frith, C.D.; Frackowiak, R.S. The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature 1993, 362, 342–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Richlan, F.; Kronbichler, M.; Wimmer, H. Structural abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013, 34, 3055–3065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Saygin, Z.M.; Norton, E.S.; Osher, D.E.; Beach, S.D.; Cyr, A.B.; Ozernov-Palchik, O.; Yendiki, A.; Fischl, B.; Gaab, N.; Gabrieli, J.D. Tracking the roots of reading ability: White matter volume and integrity correlate with phonological awareness in prereading and early-reading kindergarten children. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 13251–13258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Araújo, S.; Faísca, L.; Bramão, I.; Reis, A.; Petersson, K.M. Lexical and sublexical orthographic processing: An ERP study with skilled and dyslexic adult readers. Brain Lang. 2015, 141, 16–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Pugh, K.R.; Mencl, W.E.; Jenner, A.R.; Katz, L.; Frost, S.J.; Lee, J.R.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A. Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia). Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2000, 6, 207–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Shaywitz, B.A.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Blachman, B.A.; Pugh, K.R.; Fulbright, R.K.; Skudlarski, P.; Mencl, W.E.; Constable, R.T.; Holahan, J.M.; Marchione, K.E.; et al. Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically- based intervention. Biol. Psychiatry 2004, 55, 926–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L. The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 254–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Norton, E.S.; Beach, S.D.; Gabrieli, J.D. Neurobiology of dyslexia. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2015, 30, 73–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Ramus, F.; Altarelli, I.; Jednoróg, K.; Zhao, J.; Scotto di Covella, L. Neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia: Pitfalls and promise. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 84, 434–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Kim, S.K. Recent update on reading disability (dyslexia) focused on neurobiology. Clin. Exp. Pediatr. 2021, 64, 497–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Hernández-Vásquez, R.; Córdova García, U.; Barreto, A.M.B.; Rojas, M.L.R.; Ponce-Meza, J.; Saavedra-López, M. An Overview on Electrophysiological and Neuroimaging Findings in Dyslexia. Iran. J. Psychiatry 2023, 18, 503–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Cainelli, E.; Vedovelli, L.; Carretti, B.; Bisiacchi, P. EEG correlates of developmental dyslexia: A systematic review. Ann. Dyslexia 2023, 73, 184–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Finn, E.S.; Shen, X.; Holahan, J.M.; Scheinost, D.; Lacadie, C.; Papademetris, X.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A.; Constable, R.T. Disruption of functional networks in dyslexia: A whole-brain, data-driven analysis of connectivity. Biol. Psychiatry 2014, 76, 397–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Shaywitz, B.A.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Pugh, K.R.; Mencl, W.E.; Fulbright, R.K.; Skudlarski, P.; Constable, R.T.; Marchione, K.E.; Fletcher, J.M.; Lyon, G.R.; et al. Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biol. Psychiatry 2002, 52, 101–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pagnotta, M.F.; Zouridakis, G.; Li, L.; Lizarazu, M.; Lallier, M.; Molinaro, N.; Carreiras, M. Low frequency overactivation in dyslexia: Evidence from resting state Magnetoencephalography. In Proceedings of the 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Milan, Italy, 25–29 August 2015; pp. 6959–6962. [Google Scholar]
  52. Paz-Alonso, P.M.; Oliver, M.; Lerma-Usabiaga, G.; Caballero-Gaudes, C.; Quiñones, I.; Suárez-Coalla, P.; Duñabeitia, J.A.; Cuetos, F.; Carreiras, M. Neural correlates of phonological, orthographic and semantic reading processing in dyslexia. Neuroimage Clin. 2018, 20, 433–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Chiarenza, G.A.; Olgiati, P.; Trevisan, C.; Marchi, I.D.; Casarotto, S. Reading aloud: A psychophysiological investigation in children. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 425–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Bakos, S.; Mehlhase, H.; Landerl, K.; Bartling, J.; Schulte-Körne, G.; Moll, K. Naming processes in reading and spelling disorders: An electrophysiological investigation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2020, 131, 351–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Ganushchak, L.Y.; Christoffels, I.K.; Schiller, N.O. The use of electroencephalography in language production research: A review. Front. Psychol. 2011, 202, 208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Mahé, G.; Pont, C.; Zesiger, P.; Laganaro, M. The electrophysiological correlates of developmental dyslexia: New insights from lexical decision and reading aloud in adults. Neuropsychologia 2018, 121, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Strijkers, K.; Holcomb, P.J.; Costa, A. Conscious intention to speak proactively facilitates lexical access during overt object naming. J. Mem. Lang. 2012, 65, 345–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Thorpe, S.; Fize, D.; Marlot, C. Speed of processing in the human visual system. Nature 1996, 381, 520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Strijkers, K.; Costa, A. Riding the lexical speedway: A critical review on the time course of lexical selection in speech production. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Dirani, J.; Pylkkänen, L. The time course of cross-modal representations of conceptual categories. Neuroimage 2023, 277, 120254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Stephan, F.; Saalbach, H.; Rossi, S. The Brain Differentially Prepares Inner and Overt Speech Production: Electrophysiological and Vascular Evidence. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Strijkers, K.; Costa, A.; Pulvermüller, F. The cortical dynamics of speaking: Lexical and phonological knowledge simultaneously recruit the frontal and temporal cortex within 200 ms. Neuroimage 2017, 163, 206–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Eulitz, C.; Hauk, O.; Cohen, R. Electroencephalographic activity over temporal brain areas during phonological encoding in picture naming. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2000, 111, 2088–2097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Evans, J.L.; Maguire, M.J.; Sizemore, M.L. Neural patterns elicited by lexical processing in adolescents with specific language impairment: Support for the procedural deficit hypothesis? J. Neurodev. Disord. 2022, 14, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Paulitzki, J.R.; Risko, E.F.; O’Malley, S.; Stolz, J.A.; Besner, D. On the role of set when reading aloud: A dissociation between prelexical and lexical processing. Conscious. Cogn. 2009, 18, 135–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Schmitt, B.M.; Münte, T.F.; Kutas, M. Electrophysiological estimates of the time course of semantic and phonological encoding during implicit picture naming. Psychophysiology 2000, 37, 473–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Costa, A.; Strijkers, K.; Martin, C.; Thierry, G. The time course of word retrieval revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 21442–21446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Graves, W.W.; Grabowski, T.J.; Mehta, S.; Gordon, J.K. A neural signature of phonological access: Distinguishing the effects of word frequency from familiarity and length in overt picture naming. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007, 19, 617–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Kan, I.P.; Thompson-Schill, S.L. Effect of name agreement on prefrontal activity during overt and covert picture naming. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2004, 4, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Koester, D.; Schiller, N.O. Morphological priming in overt language production: Electrophysiological evidence from Dutch. Neuroimage 2008, 42, 1622–1630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Krott, A.; Medaglia, M.T.; Porcaro, C. Early and Late Effects of Semantic Distractors on Electroencephalographic Responses During Overt Picture Naming. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Piai, V.; Roelofs, A.; van der Meij, R. Event-related potentials and oscillatory brain responses associated with semantic and Stroop-like interference effects in overt naming. Brain Res. 2012, 1450, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Verhoef, K.; Roelofs, A.; Chwilla, D.J. Role of inhibition in language switching: Evidence from event-related brain potentials in overt picture naming. Cognition 2009, 110, 84–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Verhoef, K.M.; Roelofs, A.; Chwilla, D.J. Electrophysiological evidence for endogenous control of attention in switching between languages in overt picture naming. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 1832–1843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Wang, M.; Chen, Y.; Schiller, N.O. Lexico-syntactic features are activated but not selected in bare noun production: Electrophysiological evidence from overt picture naming. Cortex 2019, 116, 294–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Clarke, A.; Taylor, K.I.; Devereux, B.; Randall, B.; Tyler, L.K. From perception to conception: How meaningful objects are processed over time. Cereb. Cortex 2013, 23, 187–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Clarke, A.; Taylor, K.I.; Tyler, L.K. The evolution of meaning: Spatio-temporal dynamics of visual object recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 1887–1899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Scholl, C.A.; Jiang, X.; Martin, J.G.; Riesenhuber, M. Time course of shape and category selectivity revealed by EEG rapid adaptation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 26, 408–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Branzi, F.M.; Martin, C.D.; Biau, E. Activating words without language: Beta and theta oscillations reflect lexical access and control processes during verbal and non-verbal object recognition tasks. Cereb. Cortex 2023, 33, 6228–6240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Bermúdez-Margaretto, B.; Beltrán, D.; Domínguez, A.; Cuetos, F. Repeated Exposure to “meaningless” Pseudowords Modulates LPC, but Not N(FN)400. Brain Topogr. 2015, 28, 838–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Rüsseler, J.; Probst, S.; Johannes, S.; Münte, T. Recognition memory for high- and low-frequency words in adult normal and dyslexic readers: An event-related brain potential study. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2003, 25, 815–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Perfetti, C.A.; Wlotko, E.W.; Hart, L.A. Word learning and individual differences in word learning reflected in event-related potentials. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2005, 31, 1281–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Liotti, M.; Woldorff, M.G.; Perez, R.; Mayberg, H.S. An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect. Neuropsychologia 2000, 38, 701–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Reis, A.; Araújo, S.; Morais, I.S.; Faísca, L. Reading and reading-related skills in adults with dyslexia from different orthographic systems: A review and meta-analysis. Ann. Dyslexia 2020, 70, 339–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Salmelin, R.; Hari, R.; Lounasmaa, O.V.; Sams, M. Dynamics of brain activation during picture naming. Nature 1994, 368, 463–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Mahé, G.; Zesiger, P.; Laganaro, M. Beyond the initial 140 ms, lexical decision and reading aloud are different tasks: An ERP study with topographic analysis. Neuroimage 2015, 122, 65–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Mahé, G.; Bonnefond, A.; Gavens, N.; Dufour, A.; Doignon-Camus, N. Impaired visual expertise for print in French adults with dyslexia as shown by N170 tuning. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 3200–3206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Amora, K.K.; Tretow, A.; Verwimp, C.; Tijms, J.; Leppänen, P.H.T.; Csépe, V. Typical and Atypical Development of Visual Expertise for Print as Indexed by the Visual Word N1 (N170w): A Systematic Review. Front. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 898800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Silva, P.B.; Oliveira, D.G.; Cardoso, A.D.; Laurence, P.G.; Boggio, P.S.; Macedo, E.C. Event-related potential and lexical decision task in dyslexic adults: Lexical and lateralization effects. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 852219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Helenius, P.; Parviainen, T.; Paetau, R.; Salmelin, R. Neural processing of spoken words in specific language impairment and dyslexia. Brain 2009, 132 Pt 7, 1918–1927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Premeti, A.; Bucci, M.P.; Isel, F. Evidence from ERP and Eye Movements as Markers of Language Dysfunction in Dyslexia. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Egan, C.; Payne, J.S.; Jones, M.W. The impact of phonological relatedness on semantic congruency judgements in readers with dyslexia: Evidence from behavioural judgements, event related potentials and pupillometry. Neuropsychologia 2023, 184, 108548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Friedman, D.; Johnson, R., Jr. Event-related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: A selective review. Microsc. Res. Tech. 2000, 51, 6–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Emerson, S.N.; Conway, C.M.; Özçalışkan, Ş. Semantic P600-but not N400-effects index crosslinguistic variability in speakers’ expectancies for expression of motion. Neuropsychologia 2020, 149, 107638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Wachinger, C.; Volkmer, S.; Bublath, K.; Bruder, J.; Bartling, J.; Schulte-Körne, G. Does the late positive component reflect successful reading acquisition? A longitudinal ERP study. Neuroimage Clin. 2017, 17, 232–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Riès, S.; Legou, T.; Burle, B.; Alario, F.X.; Malfait, N. Corrigendum to “Why does picture naming take longer than word naming? The contribution of articulatory processes”. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2015, 22, 309–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Price, C.J.; McCrory, E.; Noppeney, U.; Mechelli, A.; Moore, C.J.; Biggio, N.; Devlin, J.T. How reading differs from object naming at the neuronal level. Neuroimage 2006, 29, 643–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Aristei, S.; Melinger, A.; Abdel Rahman, R. Electrophysiological chronometry of semantic context effects in language production. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 1567–1586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Schendan, H.E.; Maher, S.M. Object knowledge during entry-level categorization is activated and modified by implicit memory after 200 ms. Neuroimage 2009, 44, 1423–1438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Schendan, H.E.; Lucia, L.C. Object-sensitive activity reflects earlier perceptual and later cognitive processing of visual objects between 95 and 500 ms. Brain Res. 2010, 1329, 124–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. von Seth, J.; Nicholls, V.I.; Tyler, L.K.; Clarke, A. Recurrent connectivity supports higher-level visual and semantic object representations in the brain. Commun. Biol. 2023, 6, 1207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Wong, A.W.; Wang, J.; Ng, T.Y.; Chen, H.C. Syllabic encoding during overt speech production in Cantonese: Evidence from temporal brain responses. Brain Res. 2016, 1648 Pt A, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Laganaro, M.; Valente, A.; Perret, C. Time course of word production in fast and slow speakers: A high density ERP topographic study. Neuroimage 2012, 59, 3881–3888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Kast, M.; Elmer, S.; Jancke, L.; Meyer, M. ERP differences of pre-lexical processing between dyslexic and non-dyslexic children. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2010, 77, 59–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Mayseless, N.; Breznitz, Z. Brain activity during processing objects and pseudo-objects: Comparison between adult regular and dyslexic readers. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2011, 122, 284–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Oldfield, R.C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9, 97–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Sučević, Đ.; Momirović, A.; Goran Fruk, G.; Auguštin, B. Kognitivni Neverbalni Test—KNT; Naklada Slap: Zagreb, Croatia, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  108. Rossion, B.; Pourtois, G. Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart’s object pictorial set: The role of surface detail in basic-level object recognition. Perception 2004, 33, 217–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Corel GalleryTMMagic 65,000 Software, version revised September 1998; Corel Corporation: Ottawa, Canada, 1998.
  110. Moguš, M.; Bratanić, M.; Tadić, M. Hrvatski Čestotni Rječnik; Školska knjiga d.d.; Zavod za lingvistiku Filozofskog fakulteta Sveučilišta u Zagrebu: Zagrebu, Croatia, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  111. Lenček, M. Assessment of Dyslexia in Croatian: Some Characteristics of Reading and Writing in Students with Dyslexia. Hrvat. Rev. Za Rehabil. Istraživanja 2012, 48, 11–26. [Google Scholar]
  112. Presentation® Software, Version 20.0; Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.: Berkeley, CA, USA, 2017. Available online: www.neurobs.com(accessed on 15 January 2018).
  113. Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2004, 134, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. MATLAB, version: 9.4.0.949201 (R2018a); The MathWorks Inc.: Natick, MA, USA, 2018.
  115. Makeig, S.; Bell, A.J.; Jung, T.P.; Sejnowski, T.J. Independent component analysis of electroencephalographic data. Adv. Neural Inf. Process Syst. 1996, 8, 145–151. [Google Scholar]
  116. Stekić, K.; Ilić, O.; Ković, V.; Savić, A.M. ERP Indicators of Phonological Awareness Development in Children: A Systematic Review. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Basma, B.; Savage, R.; Bertone, A. The N400 in readers with dyslexia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2024, 196, 112283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Boersma, P.; Weenink, D. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer [Computer Program]. Version 5.3.56. Available online: http://www.praat.org/ (accessed on 2 May 2023).
  119. Carioti, D.; Masia, M.F.; Travellini, S.; Berlingeri, M. Orthographic depth and developmental dyslexia: A meta-analytic study. Ann. Dyslexia 2021, 71, 399–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Bentin, S.; Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y.; Giard, M.H.; Echallier, J.F.; Pernier, J. ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: Time course and scalp distribution. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1999, 11, 235–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Dujardin, T.; Etienne, Y.; Contentin, C.; Bernard, C.; Largy, P.; Mellier, D.; Lalonde, R.; Rebaï, M. Behavioral performances in participants with phonological dyslexia and different patterns on the N170 component. Brain Cogn. 2011, 75, 91–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Vitevitch, M.S.; Luce, P.A. Phonological neighborhood effects in spoken word perception and production. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2016, 2, 75–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Araújo, S.; Fernandes, T.; Huettig, F. Learning to read facilitates the retrieval of phonological representations in rapid automatized naming: Evidence from unschooled illiterate, ex-illiterate, and schooled literate adults. Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Eloranta, A.K.; Närhi, V.M.; Eklund, K.M.; Ahonen, T.P.S.; Aro, T.I. Resolving reading disability-Childhood predictors and adult-age outcomes. Dyslexia 2019, 25, 20–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Valdois, S. The visual-attention span deficit in developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a visual-attention-based deficit. Dyslexia 2022, 28, 397–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Werth, R. Dyslexia Due to Visual Impairments. Biomedicines 2023, 11, 2559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Premeti, A.; Bucci, M.P.; Heidlmayr, K.; Vigneron, P.; Isel, F. Neurodynamics of selected language processes involved in word reading: An EEG study with French dyslexic adults. J. Neurolinguist. 2024, 71, 101201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Denis-Noël, A.; Colé, P.; Bolger, D.; Pattamadilok, C. How do adults with dyslexia recognize spoken words? Evidence from behavioral and EEG data. Sci. Stud. Read. 2024, 28, 21–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Perera, H.; Shiratuddin, M.F.; Wong, K.W.; Fullarton, K. EEG signal analysis of passage reading and rapid automatized naming between adults with dyslexia and normal controls. In Proceedings of the 8th IEEE International Conference on Software Engineering and Service Science, Beijing, China, 24–26 November 2017; pp. 104–108. [Google Scholar]
  130. Zoccolotti, P. Success is not the entire story for a scientific theory: The case of the Phonological Deficit Theory of dyslexia. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Werth, R. Dyslexia: Causes and Concomitant Impairments. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Mousikou, P.; Rastle, K. Lexical frequency effects on articulation: A comparison of picture naming and reading aloud. Front. Psychol. 2015, 15, 1571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Araújo, S.; Faísca, L.; Reis, A.; Marques, J.F.; Petersson, K.M. Visual naming deficits in dyslexia: An ERP investigation of different processing domains. Neuropsychologia 2016, 91, 61–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  134. Rugg, M.D.; Curran, T. Event-related potentials and recognition memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 251–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Azizian, A.; Watson, T.D.; Parvaz, M.A.; Squires, N.K. Time course of processes underlying picture and word evaluation: An event-related potential approach. Brain Topogr. 2006, 18, 213–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Schulte-Körne, G.; Deimel, W.; Bartling, J.; Remschmidt, H. Neurophysiological correlates of word recognition in dyslexia. J. Neural Transm. 2004, 111, 971–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  137. Hasko, S.; Groth, K.; Bruder, J.; Bartling, J.; Schulte-Körne, G. The time course of reading processes in children with and without dyslexia: An ERP study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Proverbio, A.M.; Zani, A. Time course of brain activation during graphemic/phonologic processing in reading: An ERP study. Brain Lang. 2003, 87, 412–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Schematic view of the single trial for reading (left) and naming task (right). Note: “košara” is the Croatian term for “basket” (English language).
Figure 1. Schematic view of the single trial for reading (left) and naming task (right). Note: “košara” is the Croatian term for “basket” (English language).
Bioengineering 11 00459 g001
Figure 2. An example of an elastic cap used in the study (left), and the channel data location of the electrodes (right).
Figure 2. An example of an elastic cap used in the study (left), and the channel data location of the electrodes (right).
Bioengineering 11 00459 g002
Figure 3. Schematic view of the single trial for word reading (A) and visual object naming (B) response latency of control (4C) and dyslexia subject (4D). Note: “košara” is the Croatian term for “basket” (English language).
Figure 3. Schematic view of the single trial for word reading (A) and visual object naming (B) response latency of control (4C) and dyslexia subject (4D). Note: “košara” is the Croatian term for “basket” (English language).
Bioengineering 11 00459 g003
Figure 4. The topographic view of electric signal of word reading ERPs in pre-lexical (160–260 ms), lexical (450–700 ms) and post-lexical (750–900 ms) time windows in PD and HC subjects (color bar represent relative scaling min/max −3/9.9 μV) in control and dyslexic participants.
Figure 4. The topographic view of electric signal of word reading ERPs in pre-lexical (160–260 ms), lexical (450–700 ms) and post-lexical (750–900 ms) time windows in PD and HC subjects (color bar represent relative scaling min/max −3/9.9 μV) in control and dyslexic participants.
Bioengineering 11 00459 g004
Figure 5. The topographic view of electric signal of object naming ERPs in pre-lexical (150–200 ms), lexical (280–440 ms), and post-lexical (900–1000 ms) time windows in PD and HC subjects (color bar represent relative scaling min/max −2.9/12 μV) in control and dyslexic participants.
Figure 5. The topographic view of electric signal of object naming ERPs in pre-lexical (150–200 ms), lexical (280–440 ms), and post-lexical (900–1000 ms) time windows in PD and HC subjects (color bar represent relative scaling min/max −2.9/12 μV) in control and dyslexic participants.
Bioengineering 11 00459 g005
Figure 6. Word reading mean ERP amplitude for a pre-lexical time window (160–260 ms) for the right and left hemispheres in control and dyslexia subjects. * p < 0.05. Note: Statistica Soft 12 automatically generates comma sign for decimal separator.
Figure 6. Word reading mean ERP amplitude for a pre-lexical time window (160–260 ms) for the right and left hemispheres in control and dyslexia subjects. * p < 0.05. Note: Statistica Soft 12 automatically generates comma sign for decimal separator.
Bioengineering 11 00459 g006
Figure 7. The topographic view of electric signal of right hemisphere electrodes in pre-lexical reading window (160–260 ms) in subjects with dyslexia and control subjects (color bar represents relative scaling min/max –2.1/10 μV) in control and dyslexic participants.
Figure 7. The topographic view of electric signal of right hemisphere electrodes in pre-lexical reading window (160–260 ms) in subjects with dyslexia and control subjects (color bar represents relative scaling min/max –2.1/10 μV) in control and dyslexic participants.
Bioengineering 11 00459 g007
Figure 8. Visual object naming mean ERP amplitude for post-lexical time window (900–1000 ms) for the right and left hemisphere in control and dyslexia subjects. * p < 0.05. Note: Statistica Soft 12 automatically generates comma sign for decimal separator.
Figure 8. Visual object naming mean ERP amplitude for post-lexical time window (900–1000 ms) for the right and left hemisphere in control and dyslexia subjects. * p < 0.05. Note: Statistica Soft 12 automatically generates comma sign for decimal separator.
Bioengineering 11 00459 g008
Figure 9. The topographic view of electric signal of the right hemisphere in the post-lexical naming window (900–1000 ms) in control and dyslexia subjects. Color bar represents relative scaling min/max –2.1/12 μV) in control and dyslexic participants.
Figure 9. The topographic view of electric signal of the right hemisphere in the post-lexical naming window (900–1000 ms) in control and dyslexia subjects. Color bar represents relative scaling min/max –2.1/12 μV) in control and dyslexic participants.
Bioengineering 11 00459 g009
Table 1. Qualitative error differences between PD and HC groups for word reading and visual object naming task.
Table 1. Qualitative error differences between PD and HC groups for word reading and visual object naming task.
Qualitative ErrorsControl n(%)Dyslexia n(%)χ2p
ReadingNo response0 (0)3 (0.39)3.010.08
Semantic error0 (0)0 (0)--
Phonological error3 (0.39)11 (1.43)4.610.03 *
Hesitation1 (0.13)5 (0.65)2.680.10
Total4 (0.52)19 (2.47)9.930.002
Visual object namingNo response13 (1.72)77 (10.02)48.34<0.001
Semantic error125 (16.28)121 (15.75)0.080.78
Phonological error14 (1.82)26 (3.38)3.700.05
Hesitation37 (4.81)49 (6.38)1.770.18
Total189 (24.60)273 (35.54)21.84<0.001
Note: χ2—Chi square test; * p < 0.05.
Table 2. Word reading and visual object naming response latency differences for PD and HC subjects.
Table 2. Word reading and visual object naming response latency differences for PD and HC subjects.
ControlDyslexiatp
M ± SDM ± SD
Reading latency663.63 ± 37.09892.89 ± 96.0517.81<0.001
Naming latency794.44 ± 298.75803.38 ± 302.030.160.86
Table 3. Reading task: two-way ANOVA results of ERPs.
Table 3. Reading task: two-way ANOVA results of ERPs.
Time WindowSource of VarianceSSFpPost hoc Test
Pre-lexical
160–260 ms
Group8.640.540.46HCright-PDright p = 0.02
PDright-PDleft p < 0.001
Hemisphere88.5613.09<0.001
Group x Hemisphere41.594.790.03
Lexical
450–700 ms
Group0.230.020.88HCright-HCleft p = 0.01
PDright-PDleft p = 0.01
Hemisphere86.3212.50<0.001
Group x Hemisphere0.010.0020.96
Post-lexical
750–900 ms
Group0.040.0040.94HCright-HCleft p = 0.004
Hemisphere57.886.820.009
Group x Hemisphere15.852.070.15
Note: SS—the sum of squares; post hoc test—Fishers LSD test. HC—healthy subjects, PD—people with dyslexia.
Table 4. Naming task: two-way ANOVA results of ERPs.
Table 4. Naming task: two-way ANOVA results of ERPs.
Time WindowSource of VarianceSSFpPost hoc
Pre-lexical
150–200 ms
Group8.070.320.56 HCright-HCleft p = 0.04
PDright-PDleft p < 0.001
Hemisphere220.3717.03<0.001
Group x Hemisphere25.272.120.14
Lexical
280–440 ms
Group1.680.080.77 PDright-PDleft p = 0.02
Hemisphere77.057.940.006
Group x Hemisphere0.800.080.76
Post-lexical
900–1000 ms
Group41.776.190.01 HCright-HCleft p = 0.04
HCright-PDright p = 0.02
Hemisphere30.875.620.02
Group x Hemisphere9.831.440.23
Note: SS—the sum of squares; post hoc test—Fishers LSD test. HC—healthy subjects, PD—people with dyslexia.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Perkušić Čović, M.; Vujović, I.; Šoda, J.; Palmović, M.; Rogić Vidaković, M. Overt Word Reading and Visual Object Naming in Adults with Dyslexia: Electroencephalography Study in Transparent Orthography. Bioengineering 2024, 11, 459. https://doi.org/10.3390/bioengineering11050459

AMA Style

Perkušić Čović M, Vujović I, Šoda J, Palmović M, Rogić Vidaković M. Overt Word Reading and Visual Object Naming in Adults with Dyslexia: Electroencephalography Study in Transparent Orthography. Bioengineering. 2024; 11(5):459. https://doi.org/10.3390/bioengineering11050459

Chicago/Turabian Style

Perkušić Čović, Maja, Igor Vujović, Joško Šoda, Marijan Palmović, and Maja Rogić Vidaković. 2024. "Overt Word Reading and Visual Object Naming in Adults with Dyslexia: Electroencephalography Study in Transparent Orthography" Bioengineering 11, no. 5: 459. https://doi.org/10.3390/bioengineering11050459

APA Style

Perkušić Čović, M., Vujović, I., Šoda, J., Palmović, M., & Rogić Vidaković, M. (2024). Overt Word Reading and Visual Object Naming in Adults with Dyslexia: Electroencephalography Study in Transparent Orthography. Bioengineering, 11(5), 459. https://doi.org/10.3390/bioengineering11050459

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop