A Global Overview of SVA—Spatial–Visual Ability
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Rationale behind the Study
3. Constructs of Spatial–Visual Ability
4. Materials and Methods
4.1. Searching for Information
4.2. Data Analysis
5. Results
5.1. Analysis by the Age Categories and Assessment Tools
5.1.1. Ages 5 Years and Below—Kindergarten Students and Children (A1)
5.1.2. Ages 6 Years to 8 Years—Primary-School Students (A2)
5.1.3. Ages 9 Years to 15 Years—Middle- and High-School Students (A3)
5.1.4. Ages 16 Years to 17 Years and up to 17 Years—Higher-Secondary-School Students (A4)
5.1.5. Ages 18 Years to 22 Years—University and Graduate Students (A5)
Reference | Country | Year | Age | Sector | Objective |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[4] | USA | 2018 | University students | A | Evidence for the beneficial effects of spatial-skill development on potential STEM competence.—MRT |
[5] | South Korea and USA | 2021 | 18–30 years | C + E | Development and validation of the Architecture and Interior Design domain-based spatial ability Test (AISAT).—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 1A (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[6] | USA | 2007 | University students | G | Examine the impact of altering the orientation of the cutting plans and the geometric structure on performance in spatial visualisation tasks and create a psychometric assessment tool to evaluate variations in an individual’s ability to mentally visualize cross-sections of 3D objects.—MRT, SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[9] | France | 2021 | University freshmen | A + B | Examine the implications of spatial visualisation abilities on academic achievement in the study of engineering.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[10] | Japan | 2018 | MA = 20.7 years | B + I | Investigate the interplay between family SES and psychological evaluations to gain insight into the nature of family SES correlates.—NA |
[14] | Spain and Peru | 2021 | First-year and final-year university students | C + D | Examine the progression of spatial cognition in relation to capturing spatial experiences in virtual immersive settings.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[20] | USA | 2005 | Graduate and undergraduate students | B + E + F | Explore the correlation between spatial aptitude and comprehension of visual representations.—NA |
[21] | South Korea | 2016 | University freshmen | B + C | Examine the link among design studio outcomes and innovation, spatial aptitude, and visual cognition.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 1A (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[22] | USA | 2020 | University students | C + E | Investigation of the relationship between multifaced creativity and domain-based spatial skills in the context of 3D explorations within interior design.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 1A (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[33] | USA | 1997 | Undergraduate students | A + E | Examine the correlation between spatial aptitude and academic achievement in entry-level chemistry classes.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) (OB) |
[34] | USA | 2013 | University freshmen | A | Validation with evidence to support the use of Revised PSVT:R [37] in assessing spatial ability in association with other academic markers.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[35] | USA | 2014 | University students | A | Evaluate the effect of a variety of drawing methods on the students’ spatial visualisation abilities in engineering.—MRT |
[36] | Turkey | 2016 | Second- and third-year university students | A | Examine the correlation between potential teachers’ academic success and spatial skills.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) |
[37] | Portugal | 2022 | University students | A | Assess pre- and post-GIS spatial-perception transitions. Explore gender’s influence on engineering education’s STAT test.—NA |
[38] | Canada | 1995 | Undergraduate students MA of males = 21.3 years MA of females = 20.5 years | A + B | Inspect the MRT in comparison to the Card-Rotations Test and the Paper-Folding Test.—MRT |
[39] | USA | 1998 | University students MA = 21.7 years | A + B | Compare gender-based differences in performance on cognitive spatial assessments, particularly assessments of mental rotation competence and object location recall.—MRT |
[40] | USA | 2002 | Undergraduate students | A + E | Develop a standardized self-report scale (SBSOD [45]) to assess environmental spatial skills.—NA |
[41] | Austria and Germany | 2005 | University students | A + B | Assess the variance in achievement in descriptive geometry education between experienced and non-experienced cohorts.—SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[42] | Hungary | 2007 | University students | A + B | Examine the outcomes of the MCT [46], placing emphasis on disparities in spatial acumen between genders.—SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[43] | USA | 2012 | Undergraduate students | A + B | Creation and implementation of a novel assessment designed to gauge an individual’s aptitude in recognizing the 2D cross-section of a 3D geometric solid.—MRT, SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[44] | Sweden | 2021 | University students | A + B | Verification of gender differences in spatial competence via a spatial orientation assessment—Santa Barbara Sense of Direction Scale (SBSOD) [46].—NA |
[46] | USA | 2006 | Graduate and undergraduate students | A + C | Examine the efficacy and fluctuation of various depictions, encompassing spatial and mathematical ones.—MRT |
[47] | USA | 2019 | First-year university students | A + G | Assess the techniques employed by students to solve problems in a class on spatial visualisation.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) (OB) |
[48] | Canada | 1987 | University students | B | Explore the correlation between variations in spatial cognitive skills and the size of visual illusions.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) (OB) |
[49] | USA | 2005 | University students | B | Examine the gender-based cognitive differences in performance regarding verbal and spatial working memory, and comprehension of material provided in both written and graphical formats.—MRT |
[50] | USA | 1980 | First-year college students | B + C | Investigate whether spatial ability can be improved through university-level training.—MRT |
[51] | Australia | 2007 | University graduate students | C + E | Verify a novel psychometric test devised to assess spatial abilities used in technical drawing and evaluate participant performance on activities involving spatial reasoning to identify 3D features from 2D drawings.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[52] | Australia | 2010 | First-year university students | B + C | Explore the correlation between spatial aptitude and creative design skills among freshmen studying design.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[53] | USA | 2012 | 18 years | C | Establish correlations among spatial abilities, creativity, and proficiency in architectural design.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 1A (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[54] | USA | 2006 | MA = 22 years | B + D | Identification of the origins of variability in environmental spatial tasks and determination of whether performance in these tasks is indicative of a unified skill or a diverse range of skills.—MRT, SVA 1A (2D to 3D) |
[55] | Canada | 2021 | First-year university students | B + D | Determine the efficacy of instruction in virtual spatial abilities offered to both genders in a CS1 program.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) (OB) |
[56] | Italy | 2009 | MA = 21.24 years | B + H | Examine the impact of incremental theory on women’s performance in mental rotation tasks.—MRT |
[57] | USA | 1987 | University graduate students | C | Examine the importance of the conventional assessments administered to incoming interior-design students.—SVA 1A (2D to 3D) |
[58] | USA | 2011 | Undergraduate students | C | Examine spatial reasoning in architectural education, with an emphasis on the perception and cognition of primary shapes and their manipulation towards objectives.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 1A (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[59] | Romania | 2013 | Second- and third-year university students | C | Cultivating spatial sensibility in architecture students is vital, especially in relation to the site’s context and socio-cultural dimensions.—NA |
[60] | Australia | 2016 | First-year university students | C | Examine the association between spatial cognition and the university admission rating (ATAR) of design students.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D, SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[61] | Saudi Arabia | 2019 | First-year university students | C | Ascertain whether spatial aptitude influences achievement in the Interior Design program among female students.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) |
[62] | Turkey | 2017 | Second-year university students | C + D | Examine the methods employed by students to improve their technical drawing skills and the role of physical and digital models in enhancing this process.—NA |
[63] | USA | 2008 | University students | D | Examine the merits of imparting spatial visualisations with interactive animations and virtual geometric solids.—SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
5.1.6. Ages 23 Years and above—Post-Graduate Students and Working Professionals (A6)
5.1.7. Mixed Age Groups (A7)
5.1.8. Age—Not Applicable (A8)
6. Discussion
7. Limitations and Future Research
8. Implications of the Study
9. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- McGee, M.G. Human spatial abilities: Psychometric studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal, and neurological influences. Psychol. Bull. 1979, 86, 889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tanweer, M. Spatial Abilities: A Literature Review. Eur. J. Phys. Educ. Sport Sci. 2019, 4, 125–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kurtulus, A.; Uygan, C. The effects of Google Sketchup based geometry activities and projects on spatial visualization ability of student mathematics teachers. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2010, 9, 384–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sorby, S.; Veurink, N.; Streiner, S. Does spatial skills instruction improve STEM outcomes? The answer is ‘yes.’. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2018, 67, 209–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cho, J.Y.; Suh, J. The Architecture and Interior Design Domain-Specific Spatial Ability Test (AISAT): Its Validity and Reliability. J. Inter. Des. 2021, 47, 11–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, C.A.; Hegarty, M. Sources of difficulty in imagining cross sections of 3D objects. In Proceedings of the 29th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Nashville, TN, USA, 1–4 August 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Kell, H.J.; Lubinski, D.; Benbow, C.P.; Steiger, J.H. Creativity and Technical Innovation: Spatial Ability’s Unique Role. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 1831–1836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Humphreys, L.G.; Lubinski, D.; Yao, G. Utility of Predicting Group Membership and the Role of Spatial Visualization in Becoming an Engineer, Physical Scientist, or Artist. J. Appl. Psychol. 1993, 78, 250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Charles, S.; Jaillet, A.; Peyret, N.; Jeannin, L.; Riviere, A. Exploring the relationship between spatial ability, individual characteristics and academic performance of first-year students in a French engineering school. In Proceedings of the SEFI 47th Annual Conference, Budapest, Hungary, 16–19 September 2019; p. 15. [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi, H.; Taki, Y.; Nouchi, R.; Yokoyama, R.; Kotozaki, Y.; Nakagawa, S.; Sekiguchi, A.; Iizuka, K.; Yamamoto, Y.; Hanawa, S.; et al. The Effects of Family Socioeconomic Status on Psychological and Neural Mechanisms as Well as Their Sex Differences. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019, 12, 543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tucker-Drob, E.M. How many pathways underlie socioeconomic differences in the development of cognition and achievement? Learn. Individ. Differ. 2013, 25, 12–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Berkowitz, M.; Gerber, A.; Thurn, C.M.; Emo, B.; Hoelscher, C.; Stern, E. Spatial Abilities for Architecture: Cross Sectional and Longitudinal Assessment with Novel and Existing Spatial Ability Tests. Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 609363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cho, J.Y.; Suh, J. Understanding Spatial Ability in Interior Design Education: 2D-to-3D Visualization Proficiency as a Predictor of Design Performance. J. Inter. Des. 2019, 44, 141–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gómez-Tone, H.C.; Martin-Gutierrez, J.; Bustamante-Escapa, J.; Bustamante-Escapa, P. Spatial Skills and Perceptions of Space: Representing 2D Drawings as 3D Drawings inside Immersive Virtual Reality. Appl. Sci. 2021, 11, 1475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- González Campos, J.S.; Sánchez-Navarro, J.; Arnedo-Moreno, J. An empirical study of the effect that a computer graphics course has on visual-spatial abilities. Int. J. Educ. Technol. High Educ. 2019, 16, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rafi, A.; Samsudin, K.A.; Said, C.S. Training in spatial visualization: The effects of training method and gender. J. Educ. Technol. Soc. 2008, 11, 127–140. [Google Scholar]
- Suharsiwi, S.; Rachmawati, N.I.; Dehham, S.H.; Darmayanti, R. “DINO vs. DINI” educational game to increase children’s cognitive abilities—What are its level elements? Delta-Phi J. Pendidik. Mat. 2023, 1, 107–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newcombe, N.S.; Shipley, T.F. Thinking About Spatial Thinking: New Typology, New Assessments. In Studying Visual and Spatial Reasoning for Design Creativity; Gero, J.S., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2015; pp. 179–192. ISBN 978-94-017-9296-7. [Google Scholar]
- Linn, M.C.; Petersen, A.C. Emergence and Characterization of Sex Differences in Spatial Ability: A Meta-Analysis. Child Dev. 1985, 22, 1479–1498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Velez, M.C.; Silver, D.; Tremaine, M. Understanding Visualization through Spatial Ability Differences. In Proceedings of the VIS 05. IEEE Visualization, Minneapolis, MN, USA, 23–28 October 2005; p. 8. [Google Scholar]
- Cho, J.Y. An investigation of design studio performance in relation to creativity, spatial ability, and visual cognitive style. Think. Ski. Creat. 2016, 23, 67–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suh, J.; Cho, J.Y. Linking spatial ability, spatial strategies, and spatial creativity: A step to clarify the fuzzy relationship between spatial ability and creativity. Think. Ski. Creat. 2020, 35, 100628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosser, R.A.; Ensing, S.S.; Mazzeo, J. The role of stimulus salience in young children’s ability to discriminate two-dimensional rotations: Reflections on a paradigm. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 1985, 10, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fung, W.K.; Chung, K.K.H.; Lam, C.B. Mathematics, executive functioning, and visual–spatial skills in Chinese kindergarten children: Examining the bidirectionality. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2020, 199, 104923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ebert, W.M.; Jost, L.; Jansen, P. Gender stereotypes in preschoolers’ mental rotation. Front. Psychol. 2024, 15, 1284314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yurmalia, D.; Hasanah, A. Student spatial visual in geometry: The case of gender differences. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 2021, 1806, 012083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hernández, A.; Aguilar, C.; Paradell, È.; Muñoz, M.R.; Vannier, L.C.; Vallar, F. The effect of demographic variables on the assessment of cognitive ability. Psicothema 2017, 29, 469–474. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Šafhalter, A.; Glodež, S.; Šorgo, A.; Ploj Virtič, M. Development of spatial thinking abilities in engineering 3D modeling course aimed at lower secondary students. Int. J. Technol. Des. Educ. 2020, 32, 167–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramful, A.; Lowrie, T.; Logan, T. Measurement of spatial ability: Construction and validation of the spatial reasoning instrument for middle school students. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2016, 35, 709–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sorby, S.A.; Drummer, T.; Molzon, R. Experiences in using spatial skills testing instruments with younger audiences. J. Geom. Graph. 2006, 10, 225–235. [Google Scholar]
- Stumpf, H.; Eliot, J. A structural analysis of visual spatial ability in academically talented students. Learn. Individ. Differ. 1999, 11, 137–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandenberg, S.G.; Kuse, A.R. Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1978, 47, 599–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bodner, G.M.; Guay, R.B. The Purdue Visualization of Rotations Test. Chem. Educ. 1997, 2, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maeda, Y.; Yoon, S.Y.; Kim-Kang, G.; Imbrie, P.K. Psychometric properties of the revised PSVT: R for measuring first year engineering students’ spatial ability. Int. J. Eng. Educ. 2013, 29, 763–776. [Google Scholar]
- Katsioloudis, P.; Jovanovic, V.; Jones, M. A Comparative Analysis of Spatial Visualization Ability and Drafting Models for Industrial and Technology Education Students. JTE 2014, 26, 88–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yurt, E.; Tünkler, V. A Study on the Spatial Abilities of Prospective Social Studies Teachers: A Mixed Method Research. Educ. Sci. Theory Pract. 2016, 16, 965–986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duarte, L.; Teodoro, A.C.; Gonçalves, H. Evaluation of spatial thinking ability based on exposure to Geographical Information Systems (GIS) concepts in the context of higher education. ISPRS Int. J. Geo-Inf. 2022, 11, 417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peters, M.; Laeng, B.; Latham, K.; Jackson, M.; Zaiyouna, R.; Richardson, C. A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotations test-different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain Cogn. 1995, 28, 39–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dabbs, J.M.; Chang, E.-L.; Strong, R.A.; Milun, R. Spatial Ability, Navigation Strategy, and Geographic Knowledge among Men and Women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 1998, 19, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quaiser-Pohl, C. The Mental Cutting Test “Schnitte” and the Picture Rotation Test-Two New Measures to Assess Spatial Ability. Int. J. Test. 2003, 3, 219–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsutsumi, E. Evaluation of Students’ Spatial Abilities in Austria and Germany. J. Geom. Graph. 2005, 9, 107–117. [Google Scholar]
- Németh, B. Measurement of the development of spatial ability by Mental Cutting Test. Ann. Math. Informaticae 2007, 34, 123–128. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, C.A.; Hegarty, M. Inferring cross sections of 3D objects: A new spatial thinking test. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2012, 22, 868–874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neagu, T. Is There a Relationship between Spatial Orientation Ability, and Field of Study and Gender? Master’s Thesis, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Hegarty, M.; Richardson, A.E.; Montello, D.R.; Lovelace, K.; Subbiah, I. Development of a self-report measure of environmental spatial ability. Intelligence 2002, 30, 425–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ho, C.-H.; Eastman, C.; Catrambone, R. An investigation of 2D and 3D spatial and mathematical abilities. Des. Stud. 2006, 27, 505–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yeaman, A.; Bairaktarova, D.; Knott, T. A Qualitative Investigation of Students’ Problem Solving Strategies in a Spatial Visualization Course. In Proceedings of the 2019 ASEE Annual Conference & Exposition, Tampa, FL, USA, 16–19 June 2019; ASEE Conferences: Portland, OR, USA, 2019; p. 31985. [Google Scholar]
- Coren, S.; Porac, C. Individual differences in visual-geometric illusions: Predictions from measures of spatial cognitive abilities. Percept. Psychophys. 1987, 41, 211–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Geiger, J.R.; Litvviller, R.M. Spatial Working Memory and Gender Differences in Science. J. Instr. Psychol. 2005, 10, 49. [Google Scholar]
- Burnett, S.A.; Lane, D.M. Effects of academic instruction on spatial visualization. Intelligence 1980, 4, 233–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sutton, K.; Williams, A. Spatial Cognition and Its Implications for Design; International Association of Societies of Design Research: Hong Kong, China, 2007; p. 16. [Google Scholar]
- Sutton, K.; Williams, A. Implications of Spatial Abilities on Design Thinking. In Proceedings of the Design and Complexity—DRS International Conference, Montreal, QC, Canada, 7–9 July 2010; p. 11. [Google Scholar]
- Cho, J.Y. Spatial ability, creativity, and studio performance in architectural design. In Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Computer-Aided Architectural Design Research in Asia, CAADRIA 2012, Chennai, India, 25–28 April 2012; pp. 131–140. [Google Scholar]
- Hegarty, M.; Montello, D.R.; Richardson, A.E.; Ishikawa, T.; Lovelace, K. Spatial abilities at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence 2006, 34, 151–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ly, A.; Parkinson, J.; Cutts, Q.; Liut, M.; Petersen, A. Spatial Skills and Demographic Factors in CS1. In Proceedings of the 21st Koli Calling International Conference on Computing Education Research, Joensuu, Finland, 18–21 November 2021; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2021; pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
- Moè, A.; Meneghetti, C.; Cadinu, M. Women and mental rotation: Incremental theory and spatial strategy use enhance performance. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2009, 46, 187–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kolar, J.; Gorman, M.A. Determining the Significance of Standardized Tests Administered to Entering Interior Design Majors. J. Inter. Des. 1987, 13, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Akin, Ö.; Erem, Ö. Architecture students’ spatial reasoning with 3-D shapes. JDR 2011, 9, 339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitrache, A. Spatial Sensibility in Architectural Education. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2013, 93, 544–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sutton, K.; Williams, A.; Tremain, D.; Kilgour, P. University entry score: Is it a consideration for spatial performance in architecture design students? JEDT 2016, 14, 328–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peterson, H.P. Analysis of Spatial Ability Testing Results and Interior Design Program Success for Female University Students in Saudi Arabia. TEST Eng. Manag. 2019, 81, 1662–1670. [Google Scholar]
- Arslan, A.R.; Dazkir, S. Technical Drafting and Mental Visualization in Interior Architecture Education. Int. J. Sch. Teach. Learn. 2017, 11, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, C.A.; Hegarty, M. Spatial visualization training using interactive animation. In Proceedings of the Conference on Research and Training in Spatial Intelligence, Evanston, IL, USA, 13–15 June 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Gilligan-Lee, K.A.; Bradbury, A.; Bradley, C.; Farran, E.K.; Van Herwegen, J.; Wyse, D.; Outhwaite, L.A. Spatial Thinking in Practice: A Snapshot of teacher’s Spatial Activity Use in the Early Years’ Classroom. Mind Brain Educ. 2023, 17, 107–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blajenkova, O.; Kozhevnikov, M.; Motes, M.A. Object-spatial imagery: A new self-report imagery questionnaire. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 2006, 20, 239–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burton, L.J. Examining the Relation Between Visual Imagery and Spatial Ability Tests. Int. J. Test. 2003, 3, 277–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shepard, R.N.; Metzler, J. Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 1971, 171, 701–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kyllonen, P.C.; Chaiken, S. Dynamic Spatial Ability and Psychomotor Performance. Int. J. Test. 2003, 3, 233–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quaiser-Pohl, C.; Geiser, C.; Lehmann, W. The relationship between computer-game preference, gender, and mental-rotation ability. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2005, 40, 609–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schoenfeld, R.; Lehmann, W.; Leplow, B. Effects of age and sex in mental rotation and spatial learning from virtual environments. J. Individ. Differ. 2010, 31, 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moè, A. Are males always better than females in mental rotation? Exploring a gender belief explanation. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2009, 19, 21–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liao, K.-H. The abilities of understanding spatial relations, spatial orientation, and spatial visualization affect 3D product design performance: Using carton box design as an example. Int. J. Technol. Des. Educ. 2015, 27, 131–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, T.; Goodridge, W.H.; Kane, D.; Shaheen, N.L. Spatial Strategies Employed by Blind and Low-Vision (BLV) Individuals on the Tactile Mental Cutting Test (TMCT). Int. J. Eng. Pedagog. 2023, 13, 42–57. Available online: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&profile=ehost&scope=site&authtype=crawler&jrnl=21924880&AN=164745652&h=d2%2BTDNXAUhevA3JI7PDNNab72hpeZmLEuu7qynPrfw8tbZ7dAz%2FRl9yiIUVRCVlJKEiKxw4RhhmC%2FZ%2BaGVak6g%3D%3D&crl=c (accessed on 11 May 2024). [CrossRef]
- Lee, J.; Bednarz, R. Components of Spatial Thinking: Evidence from a Spatial Thinking Ability Test. J. Geogr. 2012, 111, 15–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eliot, J.; Czarnolewski, M.Y. Development of an Everyday Spatial Behavioral Questionnaire. J. Gen. Psychol. 2007, 134, 361–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- D’Oliveira, T.C. Dynamic spatial ability: An exploratory analysis and a confirmatory study. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 2004, 14, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gluck, J.; Fitting, S. Spatial Strategy Selection: Interesting Incremental Information. Int. J. Test. 2003, 3, 293–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yue, J. Spatial Visualization by Isometric Drawing. In Proceedings of the 2006 IJMEINTERTECH Conference, Union, NJ, USA, 19–21 October 2006; p. 24. [Google Scholar]
- Study, N. Engineering graphics educational outcomes for the global engineer. In Proceedings of the 66th EDGD Mid-Year Conference, Galveston, TX, USA, 22–24 January 2012; p. 109124. [Google Scholar]
- Peters, M.; Battista, C. Applications of mental rotation figures of the Shepard and Metzler type and description of a mental rotation stimulus library. Brain Cogn. 2008, 66, 260–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maeda, Y.; Yoon, S.Y. A meta-analysis on gender differences in mental rotation ability measured by the Purdue spatial visualization tests: Visualization of rotations (PSVT: R). Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2013, 25, 69–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allahyar, M.; Hunt, E. The Assessment of Spatial Orientation Using Virtual Reality Techniques. Int. J. Test. 2003, 3, 263–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, G.L. Functional Families of Spatial Abilities: Poor Relations and Rich Prospects. Int. J. Test. 2003, 3, 251–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mohler, J.L. A Review of Spatial Ability Research. Eng. Des. Graph. J. 2008, 72, 19–30. [Google Scholar]
- Zhu, C.; Leung, C.O.-Y.; Lagoudaki, E.; Velho, M.; Segura-Caballero, N.; Jolles, D.; Duffy, G.; Maresch, G.; Pagkratidou, M.; Klapwijk, R. Fostering spatial ability development in and for authentic STEM learning. Front. Educ. 2023, 8, 1138607. Available online: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/feduc.2023.1138607/full (accessed on 11 May 2024). [CrossRef]
- Lohman, D. Spatial Ability and G. In Human Abilities; Psychology Press: London, UK, 1993; p. 19. [Google Scholar]
- Gorska, R.; Sorby, S. Testing Instruments for the Assessment of 3D Spatial Skills. In Proceedings of the 2008 Annual Conference & Exposition, Pittsburgh, PA, USA, 22–25 June 2008; ASEE Conferences: Portland, OR, USA, 2008; pp. 13.1196.1–13.1196.10. [Google Scholar]
Variable | Categories | Abbreviation |
---|---|---|
Sector | STEM (Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics), geometry and geography | A |
Socioeconomic Factors, Demographics and Gender Differences | B | |
Architecture, Interior Design, Product Design and Design | C | |
Computer Graphics, Computer Games, Google SketchUp, VR and AR | D | |
Development of a new assessment tool | E | |
Literature Studies | F | |
Psychology, Intelligence, and individual differences | G | |
Mental Rotation and Mental Cutting | H | |
Biology and Neurology | I | |
Other | J | |
Year | Articles published in and before the 1970s | Y1 |
Articles published in the 1980s | Y2 | |
Articles published in the 1990s | Y3 | |
Articles published between 2000 and 2005 | Y4 | |
Articles published between 2006 and 2010 | Y5 | |
Articles published between 2011 and 2015 | Y6 | |
Articles published between 2016 and 2020 | Y7 | |
Articles published from 2021 onwards | Y8 | |
Country | America: Canada, Mexico, Peru and USA | C1 |
Europe: Austria, France, Germany, Hungary, Ireland, Italy, Netherlands, Portugal, Romania, Slovenia, Sweden and Switzerland | C2 | |
Asia: China/Hong Kong, India, Indonesia, Israel, Japan, Malaysia, Saudi Arabia, South Korea, Taiwan and Turkey | C3 | |
Australia | C4 | |
Age Group | 5 years and below—kindergarten students and children | A1 |
6 years old to 8 years old—-primary-school students | A2 | |
9 years old to 15 years old—middle- and high-school students | A3 | |
16 years old to 17 years old and up-to 17 years—higher-secondary-school students | A4 | |
18 years old to 22 years old—university and graduate students | A5 | |
23 years old and above—postgraduate students and working professionals | A6 | |
Mixed-age groups | A7 | |
Age not applicable | A8 |
Variable | Category | Frequency | Percentage—% |
---|---|---|---|
Sector | A | 8 | 9.19 |
B | 8 | 9.19 | |
C | 7 | 8.04 | |
D | 6 | 6.89 | |
E | 5 | 5.74 | |
F | 7 | 8.04 | |
G | 3 | 3.44 | |
H | 0 | 0 | |
I | 0 | 0 | |
J | 2 | 2.29 | |
A + B | 9 | 10.34 | |
A + C | 1 | 1.14 | |
A + D | 1 | 1.14 | |
A + E | 2 | 2.29 | |
A + G | 4 | 4.59 | |
A + E + G | 1 | 1.14 | |
B + C | 3 | 3.44 | |
B + D | 4 | 4.59 | |
B + I | 2 | 2.29 | |
B + H | 3 | 3.44 | |
B + C + E | 1 | 1.14 | |
B + E + F | 1 | 1.14 | |
C + D | 2 | 2.29 | |
C + E | 3 | 3.44 | |
E + G | 3 | 3.44 | |
E + F | 1 | 1.14 | |
G + H | 1 | 1.14 | |
Year | Y1 | 3 | 3.44 |
Y2 | 5 | 5.74 | |
Y3 | 6 | 6.89 | |
Y4 | 13 | 14.93 | |
Y5 | 18 | 20.68 | |
Y6 | 13 | 14.93 | |
Y7 | 17 | 19.53 | |
Y8 | 12 | 13.78 | |
Country | C1 | 46 | 52.85 |
C2 | 5 | 5.74 | |
C3 | 19 | 21.83 | |
C4 | 13 | 14.93 | |
C1 + C2 | 2 | 2.29 | |
C1 + C3 | 2 | 2.29 | |
Age Group | A1 | 2 | 2.29 |
A2 | 3 | 3.44 | |
A3 | 5 | 5.74 | |
A4 | 4 | 4.59 | |
A5 | 39 | 44.81 | |
A6 | 6 | 6.89 | |
A7 | 15 | 17.23 | |
A8 | 13 | 14.93 | |
87 1 | 100 |
Reference | Country | Year | Age | Sector | Objective |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[23] | USA | 1985 | 3–4 years | G + H | Examination of young children’s ability to discriminate reflections and rotations of visual stimuli within the confines of a mental-rotation task.—MRT |
[24] | China/Hong Kong | 2020 | Kindergarten children | A + G | Investigation of bidirectionality among Chinese kindergarten children’s mathematics, EF (executive functioning) and VS (visual–spatial skills).—MRT |
Reference | Country | Year | Age | Sector | Objective |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[18] | USA | 2015 | Under 8 years | A + E + G |
|
[17] | Indonesia | 2023 | 5 to 8 years | D |
|
[25] | Germany | 2024 | Kindergarten children | B |
|
Reference | Country | Year | Age | Sector | Objective |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[7] | USA | 2013 | 13 years | A + G | Determining the potential of spatial ability, mathematical reasoning, and verbal reasoning for future psychological research and their role in differential development across the lifespan.—SVA 1 (2D to 3D) |
[26] | Indonesia | 2021 | Grade 7 and junior-high-school students | A + B | Investigate the spatial cognition of students in geometry while differentiating between genders.—MRT |
[27] | Spain | 2017 | 11 years | B | Investigation of how various demographic factors, including parental education, gender, and family setup, impact children’s cognitive aptitude assessment.—NA |
[28] | Slovenia | 2020 | 11–14 years | D | Examine the progress of SVA in students who underwent training in 3D modelling compared to a control group that did not undergo such training.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) |
[29] | Australia | 2016 | 11–13 years | E | Derivation of a comprehensive measure of spatial ability, comprising mental rotation, spatial orientation, and spatial visualisation.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
Reference | Country | Year | Age | Sector | Objective |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[8] | USA | 1993 | Grades 9 to 12 | A + G | Group-membership prediction can improve test-validation designs that are focused on individual differences in standard performance and the significance of SVA in STEM and creative disciplines.—NA |
[16] | Malaysia | 2008 | MA 3 = 15.5 years | D | Examine the implications of a variety of training techniques for SVA enhancement among pupils of secondary school, along with the gender disparity in SVA enhancement and the relationship between training technique and gender.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) |
[30] | Italy | 2006 | 13–17 years | E | Discussion of alterations to the testing instrument and examination of the findings to use them for evaluating the 3D spatial abilities of young learners.—NA |
[31] | USA | 1999 | 12–17 years | F | Facet Theory and Multidimensional Similarity Structure Analysis (SSA) was used to investigate the structure of spatial ability in exceptionally intelligent youth.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
Reference | Country | Year | Age | Sector | Objective |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[3] | Turkey | 2010 | Student mathematics teachers | A + D | Ascertain the impact of geometrical assignments and tasks utilizing Google SketchUp on the spatial–visual ability of student mathematics instructors.—SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[64] | Ireland | 2023 | Classroom teachers | A | Examine early childhood educators’ formal and informal spatial activities with reading and math.—NA |
[65] | USA | 2006 | Working professionals | E + G | Development of a concise survey to evaluate variations in the way individuals perceive and visualize objects and space.—NA |
[66] | Australia | 2003 | MA = 26.32 years | F | Investigate the correlation between spatial aptitude and visual imagery via varied stimuli.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[67] | USA | 1971 | Adults | G | Evaluate the time taken to identify the shape of a 3D object based on the difference in their orientations.—NA |
[68] | USA | 2003 | Enlisted members of the US Air Force | J | Ascertain whether anything akin to spatial ability or temporal ability dominated performance on dynamic spatial-skills tasks.—NA |
Reference | Country | Year | Age | Sector | Objective |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[11] | USA | 2013 | 5 to 18 years | B | Evaluate the degree to which developmental pathways particular to a certain domain or generic to a domain may account for socioeconomic variations in age patterns in various kinds of cognition and success.—MRT (OB) |
[12] | Switzerland | 2020 | University Graduate + Post Graduate students | B + C + E | Evaluate spatial ability tests with varying degrees of domain specificity to architecture while looking into variations in test performance between genders with consideration of general reasoning ability.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[13] | South Korea | 2019 | 21 to 25 years | B + C | Investigate the association between spatial ability details and interior-design performance with two sets of spatial ability-measuring instruments, AISAT [13] being one of them.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 1A (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[15] | Mexico | 2019 | 19 to 38 years | D | Explore the correlation between visual–spatial aptitude and the acquisition of computer-graphics knowledge.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D (OB) |
[32] | USA | 1978 | Elementary school + High School + University students | E | Examine the development and reliability of the Mental Rotations Test in accordance with Shepard and Metzler’s [67] stimulus.—MRT |
[40] | Germany | 2003 | Group A = 15 years to adults Group B = 4 to 6 years | E | Establish and verify two novel assessments for measuring spatial ability.—MRT, SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[69] | Germany | 2005 | 10 to 20 years | B + D | Investigate the association between computer game choice and MRT performance with respect to gender differences with examining gender variations in both.—MRT |
[70] | Germany | 2010 | Group A = 20 to 29 years Group B = 50 to 59 years | B + D | Assess the impact of age and gender on mental rotation and spatial comprehension derived from virtual environments.—MRT |
[71] | Italy | 2009 | 15 to 22 years | B + H | Examine a theoretical rationale for the disparities in gender identified during the administration of the Mental Rotation Test [32].—MRT |
[72] | Taiwan | 2015 | 18 to 25 years | C | Investigate how comprehension of spatial relations, spatial orientation, and spatial–visualisation impact the proficiency of designers in 3D product design.—SVA 1 (2D to 3D) |
[73] | USA | 2023 | Mixed Age Groups | E | Build on prior models of spatial ability and tactics to shed light on BLV spatial strategies.—SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
[74] | USA | 2012 | Junior High + High School + University students | E + F | Establish the fundamental elements of spatial thinking by means of factor analysis and verification of the spatial-thinking ability test (STAT) [74].—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) |
[75] | USA | 2007 | 19 to 64 years | E + G | Establish the Everyday Spatial Behavioural Questionnaire (ESBQ), a tool to gauge several behaviour patterns wherein subjects describe the employment of various aspects of spatial intelligence.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) |
[76] | Portugal | 2004 | MA A = 19.22 years MA B = 21.47 years | F | Examine the spatial literature, evaluating multiple assessments, and empirically investigate and validate the spatial domain’s dimensionality.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
[77] | Austria + Germany | 2003 | MA A = 17.7 years MA B = 46.1 years | G | Justify the significance of individual variations in technique implementation in both study and evaluation.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) |
Reference | Country | Year | Age | Sector | Objective |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[1] | USA | 1979 | NA | B + I | Comprehensive psychometric investigations on human spatial skills, emphasizing on vision and orientation.—NA |
[2] | India | 2018 | NA | F | Examine spatial-skill frameworks, concepts, and experimental studies to better comprehend this area of cognition.—NA |
[19] | Israel + USA | 1985 | NA | B | Quantify sex disparities in spatial ability, establish their sources and determine the time of their appearance in a lifetime.—NA |
[78] | USA | 2006 | NA | A | Discuss the relevance of isometric views in spatial–visualisation assessments, specifically in the PSVT:R [33].—MRT |
[79] | USA | 2012 | NA | A | Summarize tests used to assess cognitive spatial ability in engineering students and evaluate the cognitive skills they look at.—NA |
[80] | Canada | 2007 | NA | B | Provide both figures with stimuli on white and grey backdrops to address background choice.—MRT |
[81] | USA | 2013 | NA | B + H | Estimate the gender difference in 3D mental rotation ability measured by PSVT:R [33] and examine the effects of testing conditions on differential performance by gender.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) (OB) |
[82] | USA | 2003 | NA | D | Discuss the advantages and limitations of Virtual Reality (VR) over conventional spatial ability assessments.—NA |
[83] | USA | 2003 | NA | F | Present the premise of functional groupings of spatial skills as an organisational structure for scientific inquiry.—NA |
[84] | USA | 2008 | NA | F | Investigate the way information processing mechanisms affect spatial abilities and study the gender inequalities via developmental research.—NA |
[85] | Netherlands | 2023 | NA | F | Investigate the link between spatial skills and STEM problem-solving. Explore formal and informal spatial education methods to improve spatial skills. Examine gender and socio-economic status impacts on STEM and spatial aptitude.—NA |
[86] | USA | 1993 | NA | G | Analyse and discuss the connection between G—general intelligence and spatial abilities.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D) |
[87] | Europe + USA | 2008 | NA | J | Assess the efficacy of instruments employed to gauge the 3D spatial skills of engineering students.—MRT, SVA 1 (2D to 3D), SVA 2 (3D to 2D) (OB) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Tiwari, S.; Shah, B.; Muthiah, A. A Global Overview of SVA—Spatial–Visual Ability. Appl. Syst. Innov. 2024, 7, 48. https://doi.org/10.3390/asi7030048
Tiwari S, Shah B, Muthiah A. A Global Overview of SVA—Spatial–Visual Ability. Applied System Innovation. 2024; 7(3):48. https://doi.org/10.3390/asi7030048
Chicago/Turabian StyleTiwari, Shweta, Bhavesh Shah, and Arunachalam Muthiah. 2024. "A Global Overview of SVA—Spatial–Visual Ability" Applied System Innovation 7, no. 3: 48. https://doi.org/10.3390/asi7030048
APA StyleTiwari, S., Shah, B., & Muthiah, A. (2024). A Global Overview of SVA—Spatial–Visual Ability. Applied System Innovation, 7(3), 48. https://doi.org/10.3390/asi7030048