Next Article in Journal
Gender Perspective and Women Drug Users: Specific Needs in Relation to Substance Use Treatments
Previous Article in Journal
Personality Phenomena in Women with Alzheimer’s Dementia
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Does Interoceptive Awareness Influence Depression Through Anxiety in Peruvian Adults? A Mediation Analysis

by
Leidy Johana Vivas-Rivas
1,†,
Antonio Serpa-Barrientos
2,†,
Susan M. Oblitas-Guerrero
3,
Sandra P. Carranza-Cubas
3 and
Jacksaint Saintila
3,*
1
Departament of Psychology, Universidad Cesar Vallejo (UCV), Lima 15081, Peru
2
Departament of Psicology, Universidad Nacional Mayor de San Marcos, Lima 15081, Peru
3
Faculty of Health Sciences, Universidad Señor de Sipán, Chiclayo 14001, Peru
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Psychiatry Int. 2024, 5(4), 927-938; https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint5040063
Submission received: 28 August 2024 / Revised: 29 October 2024 / Accepted: 5 November 2024 / Published: 19 November 2024

Abstract

:
Interoceptive awareness is currently gaining considerable attention and is in an active phase of research, supported by a growing body of empirical evidence from diverse fields such as neurophysiology, cognitive neuroscience, mind–body therapies, psychiatry, and psychometrics. The aim of this study was to examine the relationship between interoceptive awareness, anxiety, and depression in Peruvian adults. A total of 414 participants residing in Lima, Peru, were included, with 85% being young adults aged 18–27 years (252 women = 61%; 162 men = 39%; age range = 18–64; M age = 23.4). The results indicated that the indirect effect of interoceptive awareness on depression through anxiety accounted for 61.7% of the mediation, while the direct effect of interoceptive awareness on depression accounted for 38.3%. In conclusion, the hypothesis that anxiety negatively mediates the relationship between interoceptive awareness and depression in Peruvian adults was confirmed. This research may have implications in clinical settings, particularly for the prevention and psychotherapeutic intervention of anxiety and depression, through the promotion of adaptive interoceptive awareness.

1. Introduction

Definitions of interoception have remained consistent since 2000, although the construct has gained greater precision due to advances in neurophysiological research. Khalsa et al. [1] described interoception as the process by which the nervous system detects, represents, interprets, stores, and integrates sensations, whether conscious or unconscious, of the current state of the body. This refers to a constant mapping of the individual’s internal landscape, including aspects such as hunger, thirst, body temperature, and muscle pain [2,3].
The interoceptive process involves sensory information transmitted through the lamina I spinothalamocortical system, originating from visceral organs and body tissues, including the skin and smooth and skeletal muscles [2,4]. Currently, the term interoception is distinguished from visceroception [1]. Visceroception specifically refers to the perception of sensations from various organ systems, such as the respiratory, cardiovascular, gastrointestinal, urinary, and pancreatic systems, as well as blood vessels [5].
The concept of interoceptive awareness was first introduced in the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI) questionnaire to describe the Interoceptive Awareness dimension. This term referred to a lack of confidence in accurately recognizing sensations and emotions related to hunger and satiety [6]. Since the research by Critchley et al. [7], the term interoceptive awareness has evolved to refer to sensations derived from interoception that are consciously experienced and can be reported by the individual [1]. This conceptualization allows for a deeper understanding of how individuals perceive and report their internal sensations, which is fundamental to various research and clinical applications.
Mehling et al. [8] defined interoceptive awareness as the various dimensions of conscious interoception measurable through self-report; this involves intentional attention to bodily sensations [9], which is influenced by elements such as thoughts, beliefs, attitudes, and emotions [10]. This definition emphasizes the multidimensional nature of interoceptive awareness and the interaction between cognition and emotion in its experience. There are two types of attention directed toward bodily sensations: one characterized by mindfulness (adaptive interoceptive awareness) and the other by anxiety (maladaptive interoceptive awareness) [11]. In this framework, mindfulness fosters an adaptive and healthy relationship with bodily sensations, while anxiety can result in a maladaptive relationship, potentially perpetuating negative emotional states or stress.
Interoceptive awareness has significant implications in clinical and therapeutic settings [12,13], as it influences both physical and psychological health [12,13,14]. In this context, interoceptive awareness emerges as a key component in several interventions known as mind–body or manual therapies [14]. These interventions incorporate meditation practices or body movements (e.g., Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction [MBSR], mindfulness, yoga, tai chi, breathing therapy, or the Alexander Technique) [1]. They have been investigated for their effects on various painful conditions and psychological disorders, including chronic low back pain, fibromyalgia, eating disorders, anxiety, and depression [11,15]. The practices integrated into these therapies aim to enhance awareness and understanding of bodily sensations, contributing to better emotional regulation and management of pain or stress. By fostering a more harmonious relationship between mind and body, these therapies offer valuable tools for improving physical and psychological well-being.
Alterations in interoception may significantly contribute to the development of psychological disorders, including anxiety and depressive disorders [16]. Many psychological disorders involve interoceptive irregularities, often presenting through somatization [1,17]. For example, individuals with generalized anxiety disorder report symptoms such as headaches, gastrointestinal discomfort, muscle tension, and fatigue, while those with depression often experience fatigue, sleepiness, and changes in appetite, either increased or decreased [1]. Anxiety and mood disorders are the most prevalent psychopathological categories in the general population [18]. Nearly half of individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD) also suffer from one or more anxiety disorders [19], highlighting the close association between depression and anxiety [20].
Recent empirical evidence suggests that interoceptive awareness is directly linked to psychological well-being [21] and mindfulness [10,11,21,22,23], and in turn, mindfulness positively influences psychological health [24]. Interoceptive awareness is negatively associated with anxiety [10,11,22,25], depression [26,27], suicidal ideation, and non-suicidal self-injury [28]. In addition, interoceptive awareness appears to be a central aspect in the treatment of multiple sclerosis [29], post-traumatic stress disorder [30], depression [27,31], and anxiety [32].
Several mediation and moderation analyses of interoceptive awareness have been conducted [27,32,33,34,35,36,37], offering insights into potentially more effective treatments for various diseases and psychological disorders involving interoceptive disturbances. Some of these analyses indicated that gender moderated the relationship between interoceptive awareness and individual treatment responses for MDD [27]; interoceptive awareness partially mediated the reduction in somatic symptoms related to MDD treatment outcomes [27]; both alexithymia and emotional dysregulation mediated the impact of interoceptive awareness on depression [34]; central sensitization fully mediated the relationship between interoceptive awareness and pain intensity [35]; and interoceptive awareness mediated the connection between mindfulness treatment and anxiety levels [32].
To date, there is no record of any analysis that has explored the interrelationship between interoceptive awareness, depression, and anxiety. Based on the existing literature, we hypothesize that anxiety acts as a negative mediator in the relationship between interoceptive awareness and depression, meaning that an increase in interoceptive awareness would reduce anxiety, which, in turn, would decrease depression. The aim of this research was to investigate the relationship between interoceptive awareness, anxiety, and depression in Peruvian adults.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

In this study, 491 individuals participated: 128 virtually and 363 face-to-face, between 2 February and 27 May 2023. However, 77 participants were excluded for various reasons: 31 for residing outside Lima, 14 for being of Venezuelan nationality, 12 for being under 18 years old, 8 for missing sociodemographic data such as weight and sex, 7 for not providing informed consent, and 5 for having incomplete protocols with at least 5 missing responses in the questionnaires. A non-probabilistic “snowball” sampling method was used. We included cases with 1 to 4 missing data points by calculating the mean for each missing item. As a result, we analyzed data from 414 Peruvian individuals residing in Lima. Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 64 years (M age = 23.4), with most (85%) being young adults aged 18–27 years. Women represented 61% of the sample. Regarding educational level, 71% reported incomplete higher education. The average reported weight was 65 kg, and the average height was 1.63 m (Table 1).

2.2. Instruments

2.2.1. Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2)

This is a self-report questionnaire developed by Mehling et al. [8] designed for the following purposes: measuring interoceptive awareness and distinguishing different modes of attention to bodily sensations. One of these modes is characterized by mindfulness, referred to as adaptive interoceptive awareness, and the other by anxiety, referred to as maladaptive interoceptive awareness. Additionally, this questionnaire is used to assess mind–body therapies by analyzing mind–body interaction processes, detecting changes in interoceptive awareness associated with these therapies, and evaluating the appropriateness of the applied therapies [11].
The MAIA-2 consists of eight dimensions and 37 items that sequentially represent increasing levels of interoceptive awareness: noticing (4 items), not distracting (6 reverse-scored items), not worrying (5 items, 3 of which are reverse-scored), attention regulation (7 items), emotional awareness (5 items), self-regulation (4 items), body listening (3 items), and trusting (3 items) [8]. The scale is ordinal with six Likert-type response options ranging from never (0) to always (5) [31,38]. The eight factors demonstrated internal consistency values of α = 0.64–0.83 [8]; ω = 0.82–0.93 [39].

2.2.2. Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9)

It is a self-report measure created by Kroenke et al. [40], designed for the screening of MDD based on the measurement of the severity of symptoms present during the last two weeks [41]. The symptoms include depressed mood, loss of interest in activities, changes in appetite, sleep disturbances, psychomotor changes, fatigue, feelings of guilt, difficulty concentrating, and suicidal ideation [42], based on the diagnostic criteria for MDD from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) of the American Psychiatric Association [43]. The measure consists of nine items with four response options: not at all (0), several days (1), more than half the days (2), and almost every day (3). The internal consistency reliability was α = 0.81 [42] and α = 0.87 [41].

2.2.3. Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7-Item (GAD-7)

It is a self-report measure developed by Spitzer et al. [44] to screen for generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) by assessing the severity of symptoms experienced over the past two weeks [44]. The symptoms include nervousness, inability to stop worrying, excessive worry, restlessness, difficulty relaxing, irritability, and fear that something terrible might happen [45], based on DSM-IV diagnostic criteria A, B, and C for GAD [43]. The measure consists of seven items with four response options: not at all (0), several days (1), more than half the days (2), and almost every day (3) [44]. The internal consistency reliability presented an α = 0.89 [45].

2.2.4. Sociodemographic Data Registration Form

A registration form was designed to collect information on various sociodemographic variables, including sex (categorized as men and women), age (grouped into ranges of 18–27, 28–37, 38–47, 48–57, and 58–64), weight in kilograms (kg), height in meters (m), nationality, current department or province of residence, and education level (classified as high school completed, higher education complete, and higher education incomplete).

2.2.5. Procedures

This study was part of a broader instrumental research project aimed at translating and obtaining evidence of validity, reliability, and fairness for the MAIA-2 in Peruvian adults. A newly translated version of the MAIA-2, specifically adapted for the Peruvian context, was used in this study. Before initiating the translation and adaptation process, permission was obtained from the original author of the instrument, ensuring adherence to the intended structure and content of the MAIA-2.
The translation followed an iterative adaptation process, beginning with a dialogue between the lead author and a bilingual native Spanish speaker to achieve a culturally relevant translation. Expert assessments and cognitive interviews were then conducted to refine language clarity and contextual accuracy. After these steps, the final version was reviewed and adjusted in a collaborative discussion between the lead author and co-author.
Additional adaptations were made to improve usability and comprehension in the Peruvian context: coding in the translated version was modified, and the frequency options for the response format were updated (never = 1; almost never = 2; sometimes = 3; frequently = 4; almost always = 5; always = 6). These changes also led to modifications in the instructions for calculating reverse-scored items (7 − x). The completed version thus provided a linguistically and culturally adapted measure to assess interoceptive awareness in Peruvian adults.
The virtual survey was designed using the Google Forms application and included the following sections: informed consent, sociodemographic data, and instruments (GAD-7, PHQ-9, and MAIA-2). The informed consent section provided details on the duration of the survey and participation procedure, the purpose of the research, the right to refuse or discontinue participation after starting, the absence of consequences for refusal or withdrawal, the lack of direct benefits from participation, and the guarantee of confidentiality and anonymity. It also included the contact information of the lead author for any questions regarding the research. Additionally, a protocol for face-to-face administration was developed, containing the same content as the virtual survey, with approximately 370 printed copies. The survey was disseminated via WhatsApp and Facebook, as well as through face-to-face administration. Anonymity, privacy, and confidentiality were ensured throughout the data collection and research process.
The research protocol was reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Cesar Vallejo University. Additionally, the guidelines for the use of tests and other assessment instruments for research purposes were followed [46]. The Ethical Principles of Psychologists and the Code of Conduct were adhered to, with particular emphasis on Ethical Standards in research and publication, informed consent for research, and plagiarism [47]. Articles 24, 25, and 26 of Chapter III (research) of the Code of Ethics and Deontology were also followed, covering informed consent, the protection of individual well-being and dignity, and the rejection of falsification and plagiarism [48].

2.2.6. Data Analysis

A descriptive statistical analysis of the sociodemographic information was conducted, using measures such as frequencies, percentages, and the arithmetic mean. The indirect mediation model of interoceptive awareness in depression was derived and interpreted based on the indirect, direct, and total effects [49]. A bootstrap procedure (10,000 replicates) was applied to calculate mediation and trajectory estimates, along with the estimation plot. Effect sizes were categorized as minimal (β = 0.20), moderate (β = 0.50), and strong (β = 0.80), with practical significance in social research [50] and statistical significance set at p < 0.05. [51].
The Jamovi software (version 2.3) of the Jamovi project [52] was used to carry out the descriptive statistical analysis of the sociodemographic information. On the other hand, IBM SPSS Amos (version 24.0) was used to develop the mediation model. In addition, Jamovi (version 2.2) of the Jamovi project [52] was used to obtain the mediation estimates, the trajectories, and the estimation graph, using the bootstrap method with 10,000 replicates.

3. Results

Based on existing studies on interoceptive awareness, we hypothesized that anxiety negatively mediates the relationship between interoceptive awareness and depression. In other words, we propose that an increase in interoceptive awareness leads to a reduction in anxiety, which subsequently decreases depression. The following section details the findings related to this theoretical model.
The indirect effect showed an estimate of −0.036 (β = −0.117; p = 0.002), accounting for 61.7% of the mediation. For the components of the indirect effect, the direct relationship between interoceptive awareness and anxiety had an estimate of −0.036 (β = −0.153; p = 0.002), and the relationship between anxiety and depression showed an estimate of 0.985 (β = 0.768; p < 0.001). The total effect of interoceptive awareness on depression showed an estimate of −0.058 (β = −0.190; p < 0.001), representing 100% mediation (see Table 2 and Figure 1). The direct effect of interoceptive awareness on depression had an estimate of −0.022 (β = −0.073; p = 0.019), representing 38.3% of the mediation (Figure 2).

4. Discussion

Interoceptive awareness has become an increasing focus of interest in contemporary research, with its relevance supported by findings from diverse fields such as neurophysiology, cognitive neuroscience, mind–body therapies, psychiatry, and psychometrics. This research highlights the critical importance of interoceptive awareness for both physical health and psychological well-being. The aim of this study was to explore the interrelationship between interoceptive awareness, anxiety, and depression in a group of adults in Peru. The results confirmed the initial hypothesis, showing that anxiety mediates the relationship between interoceptive awareness and depression.
In previous research, factors such as alexithymia and emotional dysregulation were found to influence the relationship between interoceptive awareness and depression, accounting for approximately 31% of the mediation [34]. However, the current findings emphasize the prominence of anxiety as a key mediator in this dynamic. The indirect effect of interoceptive awareness on depression, through anxiety, was statistically significant, accounting for an impressive 61.7% of the total mediation. This contrasts sharply with the less prominent role that alexithymia and emotional dysregulation played in earlier studies. Additionally, the direct effect of interoceptive awareness on depression alone accounted for 38.3% of the mediation, exceeding the percentage previously reported by Desdentado et al. [34].
The mediation analysis in this study suggests that greater adaptive interoceptive awareness reduces anxiety, which in turn decreases depression. The role of interoceptive awareness in psychological well-being is undeniable, as evidenced by the findings of this research and previous studies [21]. These results emphasize how an accurate and adaptive understanding of the body’s internal signals can significantly influence the regulation of emotions such as anxiety, thereby affecting the onset and severity of depression. The consistency of these findings with the MAIA conceptual framework reinforces the validity of the MAIA model and provides a solid foundation for understanding how interoception can be utilized as a therapeutic tool. A clear example of this is the study by Lima-Araujo et al. [32], which demonstrates how interventions based on interoceptive awareness, such as mindfulness practices, can effectively improve mental health. These practices not only enhance interoception but also alleviate symptoms associated with disorders like anxiety. The therapeutic potential of interoceptive awareness goes beyond simple self-awareness; by understanding and appropriately responding to the body’s internal signals, more effective emotional regulation can be achieved. This underscores the need for continued research and the application of these techniques in clinical practice for individuals struggling with disorders such as anxiety and depression.
Interoception, or the ability to perceive and understand the body’s internal signals, plays a crucial role in how individuals experience and regulate their emotions. Emotional disorders such as anxiety and depression are closely linked to dysfunctions in interoceptive perception, leading to emotional distress and difficulties in emotion regulation [53]. It is suggested that these interoceptive disturbances may be a defining feature of these disorders, providing an important perspective for understanding and addressing them [1]. On the other hand, two distinct ways in which individuals can tune into their bodily sensations have been identified [11]: (1) one, driven by anxiety, can lead to hyperawareness or negative interpretation of bodily sensations, resulting in “maladaptive interoceptive awareness” [11]; (2) a mindfulness-based approach fosters “adaptive interoceptive awareness”, where attention to bodily sensations is more neutral or positive, allowing for more effective emotional regulation [11]. What is particularly promising is the idea that interoceptive awareness is not merely an innate trait but can be cultivated and enhanced. Through practices such as meditation and mindfulness, individuals can learn to observe their body’s sensations with acceptance and curiosity rather than judgment or fear [21,24]. This shift in orientation toward the body can have significant benefits for mental and emotional health [21,24]. This suggests a great opportunity in the therapeutic field to integrate practices that promote adaptive interoceptive awareness in the treatment of anxiety and depression. By teaching individuals to connect more healthfully with their bodily signals, we could offer a powerful tool for improving emotional regulation and overall well-being.

4.1. Cultural Considerations and Limitations

It is important to acknowledge the cultural context in which this study was conducted, as it focused specifically on a Peruvian population. Interoceptive awareness and its relationship with mental health outcomes, such as anxiety and depression, may be influenced by cultural factors [54]. Cultural norms, beliefs, and practices surrounding the perception and regulation of emotions, body awareness, and mental health vary significantly across different societies [55]. In Peru, cultural values related to emotional expression, body image, and coping mechanisms may differ from those in Western populations, where much of the existing research on interoceptive awareness has been conducted.
These cultural factors may have played a role in shaping the participants’ interoceptive awareness and emotional regulation strategies. For example, in some cultures, there may be a greater focus on somatic symptoms as expressions of emotional distress, which could affect how individuals perceive and report their bodily sensations [55]. Furthermore, the emphasis on community and social support in Latin American cultures, such as Peru, may interact with interoceptive awareness and mental health outcomes in unique ways.
The current study had certain limitations. Given that this study was conducted exclusively with Peruvian adults, the findings may not be fully generalizable to populations from other cultural backgrounds. Future research should explore whether the relationships between interoceptive awareness, anxiety, and depression observed in this study hold true in other cultural contexts. Additionally, studies that compare diverse cultural groups could provide valuable insights into how cultural factors influence the development and application of interoceptive awareness-based interventions.
Additionally, the use of mixed methods, both virtual and face-to-face, for survey administration could have introduced variations in the accuracy of the data collected. In the virtual mode, although it facilitated access to a wider range of participants in a reduced time frame, it was not possible to fully control the environment in which respondents provided their answers. Elements such as distractions, misunderstandings of the questions without the possibility of immediate clarification, or even the temptation to seek additional information while answering could have influenced the answers. Additionally, one of the main limitations of this study is the use of a non-probability sampling method, specifically snowball sampling. This approach may introduce bias in the results and limits the representativeness of the findings, restricting the ability to generalize them to the broader Peruvian population. Given this sampling method, the results should be interpreted with caution and are applicable only to the specific group of individuals who participated in the study. It is not appropriate to assume that these findings are representative of all Peruvian adults or other populations. To advance this line of research, we recommend that future studies employ probability sampling methods to enhance representativeness and allow for broader generalization of the findings. It is suggested to continue to advance this line of research and to test more complex and refined models that build on both the existing literature on interoceptive awareness and the MAIA theoretical framework. Future studies could incorporate more variables related to psychopathology, such as insomnia or anorexia nervosa, and aspects of psychological well-being, such as mindfulness and self-compassion. These studies should cover both general populations and specific subgroups with subclinical or clinical conditions and consider the variability of cultural and geographic contexts. Such an approach will further enrich our understanding of interoceptive awareness and its interaction with various psychological and contextual variables. Furthermore, while this study focused on anxiety as a mediator in the relationship between interoceptive awareness and depression, other important variables such as emotion regulation and cognitive functioning were not included in the analysis. This may limit the breadth of the conclusions drawn. Future research should aim to investigate more complex models incorporating these additional mediators to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the mechanisms underlying the relationship between interoceptive awareness and mental health. Finally, the limited sample size in the older adult group in this study may restrict the generalizability of the findings to this population. Specific characteristics of older adults may not be adequately represented, suggesting that the results should be interpreted cautiously when applied to this group. Future studies should aim to increase the sample size in this subpopulation, enabling a more thorough evaluation of findings for older adults.

4.2. Clinical Implications

This research could have clinical implications for the prevention and psychotherapeutic intervention of anxiety and depression in adults due to the finding that greater interoceptive awareness decreases anxiety, which then decreases depression. Patients with depression who present with anxiety symptoms, or a comorbid anxiety disorder could benefit from an intervention that includes adaptive interoceptive awareness as a central aspect to decrease anxiety, which would then decrease depression. Also, individuals with anxiety symptoms could benefit from an intervention based on adaptive interoceptive awareness to prevent the development of anxiety and depressive disorders, which is consistent with the findings of a previous study [32].
Mind–body therapies may be particularly effective for interoceptive awareness-based interventions in adults with anxiety and depression, as interoceptive awareness is a key component of these therapies. These interventions, which include practices such as meditation, yoga, and tai chi, largely originate from Eastern traditions. At their core, all these practices aim to create a deep connection between the mind and body through focused and conscious attention. A common denominator in these therapies is the idea of an integrated entity, where the mind and body are not seen as separate but as part of an interconnected system. This perspective is based on the concept of an “integrated self”, viewing the mind and body as an indivisible unit. Additionally, these therapies emphasize the human capacity for “embodiment”, the ability to feel and experience from within one’s own body. This concept, which refers to the intimate sense of being anchored and present in one’s own body, is key to understanding the effectiveness of these practices [15,56]. Adopting these practices can offer multiple benefits, including greater awareness of one’s emotions, sensations, and bodily states, contributing to overall well-being and helping manage stress, anxiety, and other emotional challenges.
A brief mindfulness treatment with a 30 min session format and a frequency of three consecutive days, which consisted of consciously attending to bodily sensations and breathing, showed effectiveness in increasing various aspects of interoceptive awareness and reducing anxiety symptoms [32]. Therefore, a mindfulness-based interoceptive awareness intervention could be an effective treatment to reduce symptoms of anxiety and depression in adults.

5. Conclusions

The results obtained in this cross-sectional investigation highlight the role of anxiety as a mediator in the connection between interoceptive awareness and depression. That is, the ability of individuals to be aware of and recognize internal sensations, known as interoceptive awareness, can influence anxiety levels, which, in turn, can affect susceptibility to depression. This finding underscores the relevance of understanding and fostering healthy interoceptive awareness, as it may offer an avenue for managing and potentially mitigating anxiety-related symptoms and, consequently, depression.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.J.V.-R. and A.S.-B.; methodology, A.S.-B.; formal analysis; writing—original draft preparation, S.M.O.-G.; writing—review and editing, S.P.C.-C. and J.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Universidad Cesar Vallejo.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data sets generated to support the findings of this study are not publicly available, but can be requested from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Khalsa, S.S.; Adolphs, R.; Cameron, O.G.; Critchley, H.D.; Davenport, P.W.; Feinstein, J.S.; Feusner, J.D.; Garfinkel, S.N.; Lane, R.D.; Mehling, W.E.; et al. Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2018, 3, 501–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Craig, A.D. How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 655–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Craig, A.D. How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Cameron, O.G. Interoception: The Inside Story—A Model for Psychosomatic Processes. Psychosom. Med. 2001, 63, 697–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Jänig, W. Neurobiology of visceral afferent neurons: Neuroanatomy, functions, organ regulations and sensations. Biol. Psychol. 1996, 42, 29–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Garner, D.M.; Olmstead, M.P.; Polivy, J. Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 1983, 2, 15–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Critchley, H.D.; Wiens, S.; Rotshtein, P.; Öhman, A.; Dolan, R.J. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 189–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Mehling, W.E.; Acree, M.; Stewart, A.; Silas, J.; Jones, A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0208034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Nayok, S.B.; Sreeraj, V.S.; Shivakumar, V.; Venkatasubramanian, G. A Primer on Interoception and its Importance in Psychiatry. Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci. 2023, 21, 252–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bornemann, B.; Herbert, B.M.; Mehling, W.E.; Singer, T. Differential changes in self-reported aspects of interoceptive awareness through 3 months of contemplative training. Front. Psychol. 2015, 5, 1504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Mehling, W.E.; Price, C.; Daubenmier, J.J.; Acree, M.; Bartmess, E.; Stewart, A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS ONE. 2012, 7, e48230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Suzuki, N.; Yamamoto, T.; Uchiumi, C.; Sugaya, N. Effects of Interoceptive Sensibility on Mental Health during the Coronavirus Disease 2019 Pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Vabba, A.; Porciello, G.; Monti, A.; Panasiti, M.S.; Aglioti, S.M. A longitudinal study of interoception changes in the times of COVID-19: Effects on psychophysiological health and well-being. Heliyon 2023, 9, e14951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Khalsa, P.S.; Eberhart, A.; Cotler, A.; Nahin, R. The 2005 Conference on the Biology of Manual Therapies. J. Manip. Physiol. Ther. 2006, 29, 341–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. E Mehling, W.; Wrubel, J.; Daubenmier, J.J.; Price, C.J.; E Kerr, C.; Silow, T.; Gopisetty, V.; Stewart, A.L. Body Awareness: A phenomenological inquiry into the common ground of mind-body therapies. Philos. Ethics Humanit. Med. 2011, 6, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Nord, C.L.; Garfinkel, S.N. Interoceptive pathways to understand and treat mental health conditions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2022, 26, 499–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Pollatos, O.; Schandry, R.; Auer, D.P.; Kaufmann, C. Brain structures mediating cardiovascular arousal and interoceptive awareness. Brain Res. 2007, 1141, 178–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kessler, R.C.; Aguilar-Gaxiola, S.; Alonso, J.; Chatterji, S.; Lee, S.; Ormel, J.; Üstün, T.B.; Wang, P.S. The global burden of mental disorders: An update from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) Surveys. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 2009, 18, 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kessler, R.C.; Sampson, N.A.; Berglund, P.; Gruber, M.J.; Al-Hamzawi, A.; Andrade, L.; Bunting, B.; Demyttenaere, K.; Florescu, S.; De Girolamo, G.; et al. Anxious and non-anxious major depressive disorder in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 2015, 24, 210–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Di Lernia, D.; Lacerenza, M.; Ainley, V.; Riva, G. Altered Interoceptive Perception and the Effects of Interoceptive Analgesia in Musculoskeletal, Primary, and Neuropathic Chronic Pain Conditions. J. Pers. Med. 2020, 10, 201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hanley, A.W.; Mehling, W.E.; Garland, E.L. Holding the body in mind: Interoceptive awareness, dispositional mindfulness and psychological well-being. J. Psychosom. Res. 2017, 99, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Teng, B.; Wang, D.; Su, C.; Zhou, H.; Wang, T.; Mehling, W.E.; Hu, Y. The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness, version 2: Translation and psychometric properties of the Chinese version. Front. Psychiatry 2022, 13, 970982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Silva, L.D.C.; Belrose, C.; Trousselard, M.; Rea, B.; Seery, E.; Verdonk, C.; Duffaud, A.M.; Verdonk, C. Self-Reported Body Awareness: Validation of the Postural Awareness Scale and the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (Version 2) in a Non-clinical Adult French-Speaking Sample. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 946271. [Google Scholar]
  24. Xu, F.; Zhu, W.; Chen, Q.; Tang, Y. The relationship between mindfulness, anxiety and depression during the COVID-19 pandemic: A meta-analysis of correlational studies. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 994205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Willem, C.; Gandolphe, M.C.; Nandrino, J.L.; Grynberg, D. French translation and validation of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA-FR). Can. J. Behav. Sci. 2022, 54, 234–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Dunne, J.; Flores, M.; Gawande, R.; Schuman-Olivier, Z. Losing trust in body sensations: Interoceptive awareness and depression symptom severity among primary care patients. J. Affect. Disord. 2021, 282, 1210–1219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Eggart, M.; Valdés-Stauber, J. Can changes in multidimensional self-reported interoception be considered as outcome predictors in severely depressed patients? A moderation and mediation analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 2021, 141, 110331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Gioia, A.N.; Forrest, L.N.; Smith, A.R. Diminished body trust uniquely predicts suicidal ideation and nonsuicidal self-injury among people with recent self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Suicide Life-Threat. Behav. 2022, 52, 1205–1216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Gunnersen, S.R.; Hanehøj, K.; Gro, S.; Petersen, C.M.; Skovgaard, L. The effects of a 12-week yoga intervention on body awareness in people with multiple sclerosis: A non-controlled pilot study. Adv. Integr. Med. 2022, 9, 167–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Mehling, W.E.; Chesney, M.A.; Metzler, T.J.; Goldstein, L.A.; Maguen, S.; Geronimo, C.; Agcaoili, G.; Barnes, D.E.; Hlavin, J.A.; Neylan, T.C. A 12-week integrative exercise program improves self-reported mindfulness and interoceptive awareness in war veterans with posttraumatic stress symptoms. J. Clin. Psychol. 2018, 74, 554–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Eggart, M.; Todd, J.; Valdés-Stauber, J. Validation of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA-2) questionnaire in hospitalized patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0253913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lima-Araujo, G.L.D.; de Sousa Júnior, G.M.; Mendes, T.; Demarzo, M.; Farb, N.; Barros de Araujo, D.; Sousa, M.B.C.D. The impact of a brief mindfulness training on interoception: A randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0273864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Edwards, D.J.; Lowe, R. Associations Between Mental Health, Interoception, Psychological Flexibility, and Self-as-Context, as Predictors for Alexithymia: A Deep Artificial Neural Network Approach. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 637802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Desdentado, L.; Miragall, M.; Llorens, R.; Baños, R.M. Disentangling the role of interoceptive sensibility in alexithymia, emotion dysregulation, and depression in healthy individuals. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 20570–20582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Colgan, D.D.; Eddy, A.; Green, K.; Oken, B. Adaptive body awareness predicts fewer central sensitization-related symptoms and explains relationship between central sensitization-related symptoms and pain intensity: A cross-sectional study among individuals with chronic pain. Pain Pract. 2022, 22, 222–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sweetnam, T.J.; Flack, M. Ready, set, … and difficultly slowing down: What role does alexithymia, emotional regulation and interoceptive awareness play in exercise dependence? Acta Psychol. 2023, 237, 103958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lyvers, M.; Thorberg, F.A. Alexithymia and Alcohol Use: Evaluating the Role of Interoceptive Sensibility with the Revised Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2023, 46, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Valenzuela-Moguillansky, C.; Reyes-Reyes, A. Psychometric properties of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA) in a Chilean population. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Fekih-Romdhane, F.; Malaeb, D.; Fawaz, M.; Chammas, N.; Soufia, M.; Obeid, S.; Hallit, S. Psychometric properties of an Arabic translation of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA-2) questionnaire in a non-clinical sample of Arabic-speaking adults. BMC Psychiatry 2023, 23, 577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kroenke, K.; Spitzer, R.L.; Williams, J.B.W. The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2001, 16, 606–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Villarreal-Zegarra, D.; Copez-Lonzoy, A.; Bernabé-Ortiz, A.; Melendez-Torres, G.J.; Bazo-Alvarez, J.C. Valid group comparisons can be made with the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9): A measurement invariance study across groups by demographic characteristics. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0221717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Zhong, Q.; Gelaye, B.; Rondon, M.; Sánchez, S.E.; García, P.J.; Sánchez, E.; Barrios, Y.V.; Simon, G.E.; Henderson, D.C.; Cripe, S.M.; et al. Comparative performance of Patient Health Questionnaire-9 and Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale for screening antepartum depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2014, 162, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Vahia, V.N. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5: A quick glance. Indian J. Psychiatry 2013, 55, 220–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Spitzer, R.L.; Kroenke, K.; Williams, J.B.W.; Löwe, B. A Brief Measure for Assessing Generalized Anxiety Disorder. Arch. Intern. Med. 2006, 166, 1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Zhong, Q.-Y.; Gelaye, B.; Zaslavsky, A.M.; Fann, J.R.; Rondon, M.B.; Sánchez, S.E.; Williams, M.A. Diagnostic Validity of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder—7 (GAD-7) among Pregnant Women. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0125096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. International Test Commission. The ITC Guidelines for Translating and Adapting Tests (Second Edition) [Internet]. International Test Commission. 2017. Available online: https://www.intestcom.org/ (accessed on 26 September 2023).
  47. American Psychological Association. Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct [Internet]. American Psychological Association. 2017. Available online: https://www.apa.org/ethics/code (accessed on 26 September 2023).
  48. Colegio de Psicólogos del Perú. Código de ética profesional [Internet]. Consejo Directivo Nacional. 2017. Available online: https://www.cpsp.pe/documentos/marco_legal/codigo_de_etica_y_deontologia.pdf (accessed on 9 October 2022).
  49. Igartua, J.J.; Hayes, A.F. Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: Concepts, Computations, and Some Common Confusions. Span. J. Psychol. 2021, 24, e49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Ferguson, C.J. An Effect Size Primer: A Guide for Clinicians and Researchers. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 2009, 40, 532–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Skinner, E.A.; Kindermann, T.A.; Furrer, C.J. A Motivational Perspective on Engagement and Disaffection. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2008, 69, 493–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Gallucci, M. jAMM: Jamovi Advanced Mediation Models [Internet]. 2020. Available online: https://jamovi-amm.github.io/ (accessed on 23 August 2022).
  53. Harshaw, C. Interoceptive dysfunction: Toward an integrated framework for understanding somatic and affective disturbance in depression. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 141, 311–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Farb, N.; Daubenmier, J.; Price, C.J.; Gard, T.; Kerr, C.; Dunn, B.D.; Klein, A.C.; Paulus, M.P.; Mehling, W.E. Interoception, contemplative practice, and health. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Zhou, P.; Critchley, H.; Garfinkel, S.; Gao, Y. The conceptualization of emotions across cultures: A model based on interoceptive neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021, 125, 314–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Arzy, S.; Thut, G.; Mohr, C.; Michel, C.M.; Blanke, O. Neural Basis of Embodiment: Distinct Contributions of Temporoparietal Junction and Extrastriate Body Area. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 8074–8081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Estimating the effect of interoceptive awareness on depression. Note: Application of bootstrap (10,000 replicates).
Figure 1. Estimating the effect of interoceptive awareness on depression. Note: Application of bootstrap (10,000 replicates).
Psychiatryint 05 00063 g001
Figure 2. Indirect mediation model of interoceptive awareness in depression.
Figure 2. Indirect mediation model of interoceptive awareness in depression.
Psychiatryint 05 00063 g002
Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants.
Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants.
Characteristicsn/M%/SD
Sex
 Male 16239
 Female 25261
Age
 18–2735185
 28–374210
 38–4782
 48–5772
 58–6461
Level of education
 High school completed4310
 Superior incomplete29471
 Superior complete7719
Weight (kg) 6513
Height (m)1.630.09
BMI24.43.4
Note. n: frequencies; BMI: body mass index; kg: kilograms; m: meters; M: arithmetic mean.
Table 2. Indirect mediation and total effect of interoceptive awareness on depression.
Table 2. Indirect mediation and total effect of interoceptive awareness on depression.
TypeEffectEstimateSD95% CIβZp% Mediation
LowerUpper
IndirectMAIA ⇒ Anxiety ⇒ Depression−0.0360.011−0.058−0.013−0.117−3.1200.00261.7
ComponentMAIA ⇒ Anxiety−0.0360.011−0.059−0.014−0.153−3.1400.002
Anxiety ⇒ Depression0.9850.0400.9071.0630.76824.800<0.001
DirectMAIA ⇒ Depression−0.0220.009−0.040−0.004−0.073−2.3500.01938.3
TotalMAIA ⇒ Depression−0.0580.015−0.086−0.029−0.190−3.930<0.001100
Note. MAIA: interoceptive awareness; CI, confidence interval; SD, standard deviation; β: standardized regression coefficient (effect size measure); Z: Z scores; p: p values; bootstrap application (10,000 replicates).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Vivas-Rivas, L.J.; Serpa-Barrientos, A.; Oblitas-Guerrero, S.M.; Carranza-Cubas, S.P.; Saintila, J. Does Interoceptive Awareness Influence Depression Through Anxiety in Peruvian Adults? A Mediation Analysis. Psychiatry Int. 2024, 5, 927-938. https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint5040063

AMA Style

Vivas-Rivas LJ, Serpa-Barrientos A, Oblitas-Guerrero SM, Carranza-Cubas SP, Saintila J. Does Interoceptive Awareness Influence Depression Through Anxiety in Peruvian Adults? A Mediation Analysis. Psychiatry International. 2024; 5(4):927-938. https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint5040063

Chicago/Turabian Style

Vivas-Rivas, Leidy Johana, Antonio Serpa-Barrientos, Susan M. Oblitas-Guerrero, Sandra P. Carranza-Cubas, and Jacksaint Saintila. 2024. "Does Interoceptive Awareness Influence Depression Through Anxiety in Peruvian Adults? A Mediation Analysis" Psychiatry International 5, no. 4: 927-938. https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint5040063

APA Style

Vivas-Rivas, L. J., Serpa-Barrientos, A., Oblitas-Guerrero, S. M., Carranza-Cubas, S. P., & Saintila, J. (2024). Does Interoceptive Awareness Influence Depression Through Anxiety in Peruvian Adults? A Mediation Analysis. Psychiatry International, 5(4), 927-938. https://doi.org/10.3390/psychiatryint5040063

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop