Next Article in Journal
Life History and Sociality Predict Variation in Eye Size across Birds
Previous Article in Journal
Perceptions of Birds by Urban Residents in an Australian Regional City and Implications for Conservation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Communication

Leash Status of Approaching Dogs Mediates Escape Modality but Not Flight-Initiation Distance in a Common Urban Bird

by
Skye C. Barnett
1,
Wouter F. D. van Dongen
1,2,
Roan D. Plotz
1 and
Michael A. Weston
2,*
1
Applied Ecology and Environmental Change Research Group, Institute for Sustainable Industries and Liveable Cities, Victoria University, Ballarat Road, Footscray Victoria, Melbourne, VIC 3011, Australia
2
Centre for Marine Science, School of Life and Environmental Sciences, Faculty of Science, Engineering and Built Environment, Deakin University Melbourne Campus, 221 Burwood Hwy, Burwood, VIC 3125, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Birds 2023, 4(3), 277-283; https://doi.org/10.3390/birds4030023
Submission received: 20 June 2023 / Revised: 20 July 2023 / Accepted: 21 July 2023 / Published: 25 July 2023

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Negative interactions between humans and wildlife pose a significant challenge to their coexistence. Although dog walking brings millions of people to public open spaces, roaming unleashed dogs threaten urban birds. Leashes for dogs in public parks remain controversial, with low compliance rates. As a practical solution, leashes could reduce fear among urban birds and enhance their coexistence with humans and dogs. Park leashing laws would be more likely to be enforced if there was a better understanding of how leashing laws benefit birds and wildlife. There is, however, no evidence that birds are less fearful of dogs because they understand the function of a dog’s leash. Using a common urban bird in public parks in a major Australian city, we standardized human and dog approaches, both leashed and unleashed. The birds demonstrated an increased intensity of escape responses (for example, flight) when dogs were unleashed compared to leashed. By leashing dogs in parks, coexistence between people and birds would be enhanced.

Abstract

Controversy exists around owned dogs’ impacts in public open spaces, with concerns about dogs’ impact on wildlife, including birds. Leashing dogs in public open spaces offers a tractable way of reducing dogs’ deleterious impacts on birds. Although dogs in public spaces are often unleashed, some dogs roam freely, whilst other unleashed dogs remain close to their owners. It is currently unknown whether birds can perceive and incorporate subtle differences in the leash status of approaching, but non-roaming, dogs into their escape decisions. We compare escape responses of a common urban bird, the magpie-lark Grallina cyanoleuca, in parks in Melbourne, Australia, to standardized approaches by a walker and a dog, which was either leashed or not leashed (but with the dog at the same distance from the walker). Flight-initiation distances, the distance between the lark and dog when escape commenced, did not vary between treatments. However, the unleashed dog evoked more intense responses (mostly flying away) than the leashed dog (mostly walking away). Thus, this species appears to perceive unleashed dogs as especially threatening, independent of their roaming behavior. Our findings suggest that leashing may be an effective way to reduce dog disturbance to wildlife, even for non-roaming dogs.

1. Introduction

As urbanization and human populations increase, people encounter wildlife, including birds, more frequently [1,2]. Human encounters with birds [3] are frequent in public open spaces, which provide habitat for birds as well as places where people enjoy nature and exercise [4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. Urban birds live in a human- and dog-rich environment and, as such, must respond to people and their dogs (which can alter their normal states, i.e., cause disturbance) [12]. Urban birds may be less responsive to humans [13], but responses persist in urban parks [14,15,16].
Wildlife perceives risk in nuanced ways. Birds can distinguish between different human “stimuli” (agents capable of evoking a response) and between different attributes of the same human stimulus [3]. Examples of the former include differential responses to walkers and bicycles [17]. Examples of the latter include differential responses to joggers and walkers (i.e., different approach velocities of the same human stimulus [18]). Birds can use subtle but relevant stimulus attributes associated with humans to adjust their responses, e.g., human gaze and head orientation [19], even recognizing individual threatening humans [20]. A cognitive assessment of highly specific indicators of risk associated with different human attributes may be particularly valuable for birds where humans are common and unavoidable as well as where humans differ in the level of risk they pose. Such circumstances often occur in urban areas [20].
Dogs (Canis familiaris) are often the most abundant terrestrial carnivores in urban environments. Urban birds frequently encounter dogs accompanying humans in public open spaces. Dogs often elicit escape responses from birds [21,22]. The leash on a dog is a highly specific stimulus attribute that birds might conceivably use to determine the risk posed to them in such systems. Leashed dogs cannot chase birds, unlike some unleashed dogs [23]. Despite low compliance with leashing rules in many public areas [24], evidence suggests leashed dogs provoke less intense escape responses in birds [25]. It is likely that birds recognize unleashed dogs when they roam away from their owners [24]. Many unleashed dogs remain close to humans for long periods but are free to roam or chase whenever they wish [26]. Given birds demonstrated the cognitive capability to judge highly specific human characteristics, we propose that birds that encounter humans and dogs frequently may be able to judge the leash status of approaching but non-roaming humans and dogs, thereby altering their perception of risk. Any discrimination between risks likely minimizes unnecessary responses and their consequences (e.g., energy costs). Birds that inhabit the same strata as dogs, which are dependent on appropriate escape responses, should also show fine-scale discrimination. Any such discrimination is of interest from applied and evolutionary perspectives [27].
Two aspects of escape in birds reflect the intensity of their response: the flight-initiation distance (hereafter FID; the distance at which a response begins) and the modality of escape (i.e., walk, run, fly—an ordinal scale of energy intensity and rapidity of escape) [28,29]. We measure these in magpie-lark Grallina cyanoleuca, a ground-foraging and urban-exploiting species that frequently encounters dogs [30]. We examine whether magpie-larks respond differently to an approaching human and non-roaming dog if the dog is leashed.

2. Materials and Methods

Fieldwork was conducted in northwestern Melbourne, Victoria, Australia, in April and May 2022, 1100–1400 AEST, which is outside the breeding season in southern Australia [31]. Data were collected from five locations consisting of public parks and fields bordered by roads or residential estates where birds were common and humans and dogs were active. These were: Navan Park (−37.6696801° S, 144.5676491° E), Hannah Watts Park (−37.6835231° S, 144.5923241° E), Arthur Westlake Memorial Reserve (−37.6795405° S, 144.5485148° E), Arnolds Creek (−37.6707366° S, 144.5509778° E) and Kurunjang (−37.6621024° S, 144.5919494° E). Standardized approaches to magpie-larks were made using two different stimuli: a walker with an on-leash dog (“control”) and a walker with the same dog (Chihuahua, approximately 2 kg, silent) off leash (“treatment”, the leash was held in the investigator’s hand). We recorded:
  • Starting distance between investigator and focal bird at the beginning of the approach. FID is strongly influenced by starting distance [30,32].
  • FID, a widely used metric of indexing escape behavior in animals [33,34].
  • Response mode (intensity) was recorded as an ordinal scale, from walk (1), run (2) to fly (3). There is a gradient in response intensity between urban and rural magpie-larks [30]. This suggests that this metric may be sensitive to differing risk profiles they encounter.
The initial treatment was randomly selected at each location, and subsequent treatments were alternated so that two of each level of treatment were collected at each location. Treatments were balanced within and between locations.
During an approach, we ensured that there were no other stimuli nearby that could trigger a response. Magpie-larks were foraging on the ground, and we always approached on a grassy substrate in order to minimize noise. All approaches were made away from roads, whose proximity can influence magpie-larks FIDs [30]. An approach was made by measuring the starting distance between the approaching stimulus and the focal bird and then walking directly towards it at a slow pace (approximately 1 ms−1). The dog remained approximately 1 m ahead of the approaching researcher, regardless of whether it was on or off leash (distance between dog and researcher, 0.8–1.1 m, regardless of treatment, Mann–Whitney U = 1.695, p = 0.151). The distance from the researcher at which the magpie-lark initiated an escape response, either by walking or flying, was recorded as FID. FIDs meeting our criteria (no bird approached more than once, treatments randomized and balanced across sites, dog silent and beside the investigator, no other disturbance evident) were difficult to collect, but we collected data from 20 magpie-larks, 10 from each treatment. All distances were measured to the nearest meter using a Kogan 900 m Laser Range Finder.

Statistical Analysis

The starting distance (34.5 ± 2.2 [SE] m) did not differ between the five study locations (F4,19 = 1.576, p = 0.232, R2 = 0.108) or between the on-leash and off-leash stimuli (F1,19 = 0.098, p = 0.758, R2 = 0.005), and there was no correlation between starting distance and FID (F1,19 = 0.582, p = 0.455, R2 = 0.031). We therefore removed starting distance from all analyses to avoid overfitting the data.
We firstly logarithmically transformed the FID data to improve normality (log10). ANOVA was used to test the correlation between FID and stimulus type. A logistical regression was used to test how escape responses varied with treatment. Non-parametric tests were used where data represented ranks, i.e., the intensity scale of response. All analyses were completed using SPSS [35]. We present means ± SEs throughout.

3. Results

All magpie-larks responded to our approaches by either walking, running, or flying away (FID, 8.5 ± 0.8 m, 5.2–17.6 m). Flight initiation distances did not vary significantly between treatments (on-leash, 7.6 ± 0.6 m; off-leash, 9.4 ± 1.4 m; F1,19 = 1.072, p = 0.314). However, the escape modality varied with the stimulus. A total of 30% magpie-larks that were approached by the on-leash treatment escaped by flying, whereas 80% escaped by flying in response to the off-leash treatment (Wald = 4.53, n = 20, p = 0.033). When expressed as a response intensity, higher-intensity escapes occurred when the approaching dog was unleashed (U = −2.433, p = 0.029; Figure 1). The bird’s modal response evoked by the leashed dog was walking, while that for the unleashed dog was flying.

4. Discussion

Although FID did not differ between treatments (although additional sampling and associated increases in power may result in a difference becoming apparent), unleashed treatments induced more intense escape modes in magpie-larks. This indicates the capacity to discriminate between unleashed and leashed dogs accompanying humans (i.e., using specific attributes of a stimulus to modify escape). Compared to rural birds, urban magpie-larks typically exhibit shorter FIDs and less intense responses [30], suggesting that they can adjust both aspects of escape. The urban magpie-larks in our study exhibited similar FIDs as reported elsewhere [30] but had higher intensity escape responses (e.g., flying) previously only observed in their rural counterparts [30], thus emphasizing the perceived threat of unleashed dogs to this urban bird species.
This study reported discrimination only in terms of response intensity, not FID, which suggests that such discrimination occurred relatively late during the approach, shortly before escape began. FIDs may differ owing to delays in cognition (delay in risk decision-making) or perceptive acuity (capacity to see the leash or its absence) in magpie-larks until the owner and dog are close, at which point intense escapes are the only option. Alternatively, magpie-larks may have learned to tolerate dogs up to a certain distance and to manage encounters with dogs presenting different risks by using different escape modes. Bernard et al. [17] reported similar patterns for magpie-larks that discriminated between walkers and fast and slow bicycles. FIDs did not differ, but the escape modality was more intense with quickly approaching stimuli. Several bird species have varying escape modes under circumstances where FID also varies with stimulus (e.g., [17,18,36]), as well as independent of FID (e.g., in this study).
The nuanced differentiation described here is expected when discrimination is adaptive. Birds living in urban areas benefit from highly developed escape decisions (i.e., contributing to the “cognitive buffer” evident in urban birds [37]), which must avoid numerous deadly stimuli, such as bicycles [17] or predators [21,38]. In urban parks, magpie-larks encounter many humans and dogs, where responses require time and energy [30]. As expected, the species makes decisions about escape distances and modes depending on various factors/attributes, such as the distance to a road or pathway [30], bicycles versus walkers [17], joggers versus walkers [18], and leashed versus unleashed dogs (in this study). Magpie-larks evidently display some of the most nuanced escape behavior among Australian birds (possibly an artefact of study effort).
We describe a cost to magpie-larks of unleashed dogs that extends beyond obvious chasing and harassment [23]. Fear of a small, unleashed dog evoked more intense, energetically demanding, and spatially disruptive escape responses than fear of a leashed dog (see [14,29]). The dog itself did not bark or roam, which might trigger avian escape responses [23,39]. Leashes prevent dog-roaming behavior, which can cause problems for magpie-larks, since unleashed dogs tend to move quickly and encounter birds [23,26,40]. Leashes have another benefit; namely, they reduce fear of an approaching dog, as measured by magpie-lark responses. Dog management in urban parks is controversial and typically includes areas for dogs off leash, on leash only, or “under effective control” (also known as “voice and sight control”, where owners control dog behavior without a leash [38]). The magpie-lark response differed between on-leash and effective control prescriptions in our study. In many places, compliance with dog regulations is low, which affects avian escape responses [38,41,42]. In contrast to many dogs, our study dog was likely under a more effective control.
This study was conducted in autumn, in urban parks, and with one dog and the same handler. Although we report a higher intensity response by urban magpie-larks to this small, unleashed dog, bird responses are likely to vary spatiotemporally [30,43,44], according to the color, size and breed of dog species [26,45], and possibly in relation to the behavior of the handler. Additional studies using different dogs and handlers, and additional study species and sites, are required [46].
Evidence suggests that dogs may have a greater impact on bird abundance in rural [47] compared to urban environments [48]. There may be a greater need for dog-leashing laws or even the exclusion of dogs along the peri-urban boundaries of our cities when compared with inner-city public spaces, given that avian communities may be transitioning from recent rural histories and may be shaped by the novel human regime they face [49]. Understanding this relationship more fully will require studies to repeat our observations across more time periods (e.g., breeding season [31]) and areas (e.g., gardens [27,48,50]), using other bird species (e.g., [44]), dogs and handlers [12]. This must also include observations in rural settings with dogs off pathways, where studies have shown them to have larger impacts on bird diversity [47] and behavior (e.g., [30,36]).
Domestic dogs threaten at least 78 threatened bird species from 25 families [51]. A growing body of literature considers dogs’ impact on urban wildlife populations (e.g., [41,47,52]), yet compliance to leashing laws remains low [26,38]. If dog owners understand how leash restrictions benefit wildlife, they are more likely to support them [36,42,53]. Coexistence between magpie-larks and people in parks would be enhanced by leashing dogs.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and investigation, S.C.B., W.F.D.v.D., R.D.P. and M.A.W.; writing—original draft preparation, S.C.B.; writing—review and editing, W.F.D.v.D., R.D.P. and M.A.W.; equipment acquisition, S.C.B. and M.A.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The animal study protocol and research were approved by Deakin University Animal Ethics Committee (B08-2021) and the Department of Environment, Land, Water and Planning (No. 10010123).

Data Availability Statement

Data Availability Statement: Data used for this study may be accessed through the respective citations provided and/or by contacting the authors.

Acknowledgments

This research was conducted on the traditional lands of the Bunurong Boon Wurrung and Wurundjeri Woi Wurrung people of the Kulin nation. Editorial work occurred at Beach Ecology and Conservation Hub (Venus Bay). This communication is dedicated to the late Alan Lill, who pioneered studies of FIDs in magpie-larks.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Taylor, L.; Taylor, C.; Davis, A. The impact of urbanisation on avian species: The inextricable link between people and birds. Urban Ecosyst. 2013, 16, 481–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. White, J.G.; Fitzsimons, J.A.; Palmer, G.C.; Antos, M.J. Surviving urbanisation: Maintaining bird species diversity in urban Melbourne. Victorian Nat. 2009, 126, 73–78. [Google Scholar]
  3. Weston, M.A. Human disturbance. In The Population Ecology and Conservation of Charadrius Plovers; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2019; pp. 277–308. [Google Scholar]
  4. Pincetl, S.; Gearin, E. The reinvention of public green space. Urban Geogr. 2005, 26, 365–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Thompson, C.W. Urban open space in the 21st century. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2002, 60, 59–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Gladwell, V.F.; Brown, D.K.; Wood, C.; Sandercock, G.R.; Barton, J.L. The great outdoors: How a green exercise environment can benefit all. Extrem. Physiol. Med. 2013, 2, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Wendelboe-Nelson, C.; Kelly, S.; Kennedy, M.; Cherrie, J.W. A scoping review mapping research on green space and associated mental health benefits. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Xu, W.; Yu, J.; Huang, P.; Zheng, D.; Lin, Y.; Huang, Z.; Zhao, Y.; Dong, J.; Zhu, Z.; Fu, W. Relationship between vegetation habitats and bird communities in urban mountain parks. Animals 2022, 12, 2470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Zhang, Z.; Huang, G. How do urban parks provide bird habitats and birdwatching service? Evidence from Beijing, China. Remote Sens. 2020, 12, 3166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Gómez, E. Dog Parks: Benefits, conflicts, and suggestions. J. Park Recreat. Adm. 2013, 31, 1–24. [Google Scholar]
  11. Graham, T.M.; Glover, T.D. On the fence: Dog parks in the (un) leashing of community and social capital. Leis. Sci. 2014, 36, 217–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Weston, M.A.; McLeod, E.M.; Blumstein, D.T.; Guay, P. A review of flight initiation distances and their application to managing disturbance to Australian birds. Emu 2012, 112, 269–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Samia, D.S.; Nakagawa, S.; Nomura, F.; Rangel, T.F.; Blumstein, D.T. Increased tolerance to humans among disturbed wildlife. Nat. Commun. 2015, 6, 8877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Radvan, M.; Rendall, A.R.; Weston, M.A. The habitat connectivity hypothesis of escape in urban woodland birds. Behav. Ecol. 2023, 34, 297–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Morelli, F.; Mikula, P.; Benedetti, Y.; Bussière, R.; Jerzak, L.; Tryjanowski, P. Escape behaviour of birds in urban parks and cemeteries across Europe: Evidence of behavioural adaptation to human activity. Sci. Total Environ. 2018, 631, 803–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Morelli, F.; Leveau, L.M.; Mikula, P.; MacGregor-Fors, I.; Bocelli, M.L.; Quesada-Acuña, S.G.; González-Lagos, C.; Gutiérrez-Tapia, P.; Dri, G.F.; Delgado-V., C.A.; et al. Are birds more afraid in urban parks or cemeteries? A Latin American study contrasts with results from Europe. Sci. Total Environ. 2023, 861, 160534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Bernard, G.E.; van Dongen, W.F.; Guay, P.J.; Symonds, M.R.; Robinson, R.W.; Weston, M.A. Bicycles evoke longer flight-initiation distances and higher intensity escape behaviour of some birds in parks compared with pedestrians. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2018, 178, 276–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lethlean, H.; Van Dongen, W.F.; Kostoglou, K.; Guay, P.J.; Weston, M.A. Joggers cause greater avian disturbance than walkers. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 159, 42–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Bateman, P.W.; Fleming, P.A. Who are you looking at? Hadeda ibises use direction of gaze, head orientation and approach speed in their risk assessment of a potential predator. J. Zool. 2011, 285, 316–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lee, W.Y.; Lee, S.I.; Choe, J.C.; Jablonski, P.G. Wild birds recognize individual humans: Experiments on magpies, Pica pica. Anim. Cogn. 2011, 14, 817–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Mitchell, B.D.; Banks, P.B. Do wild dogs exclude foxes? Evidence for competition from dietary and spatial overlaps. Austral Ecol. 2005, 30, 581–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Weston, M.A.; Fitzsimons, J.A.; Wescott, G.; Miller, K.K.; Ekanayake, K.B.; Schneider, T. Bark in the park: A review of domestic dogs in parks. Environ. Manag. 2014, 54, 373–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Weston, M.A.; Stankowich, T. Dogs as agents of disturbance. In Free-Ranging Dogs and Wildlife Conservation; Gompper, M.E., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013; pp. 94–113. [Google Scholar]
  24. Gómez-Serrano, M.Á. Four-legged foes: Dogs disturb nesting plovers more than people do on tourist beaches. Ibis 2021, 163, 338–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Weston, M.A.; Elgar, M.A. Responses of incubating hooded plovers (Thinornis rubricollis) to disturbance. J. Coast. Res. 2007, 23, 569–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Schneider, T.J.; Maguire, G.S.; Whisson, D.A.; Weston, M.A. Regulations fail to constrain dog space use in threatened species beach habitats. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 2019, 63, 1022–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Mikula, P.; Tomášek, O.; Romportl, D.; Aikins, T.K.; Avendaño, J.E.; Braimoh-Azaki, B.D.A.; Chaskda, A.; Cresswell, W.; Cunningham, S.J.; Dale, S.; et al. Bird tolerance to humans in open tropical ecosystems. Nat. Commun. 2023, 14, 2146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Marsh, R.L.; Ellerby, D.J.; Carr, J.A.; Henry, H.T.; Buchanan, C.I. Partitioning the energetics of walking and running: Swinging the limbs is expensive. Science 2004, 303, 80–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ward, S.; Bishop, C.M.; Woakes, A.J.; Butler, P.J. Heart rate and the rate of oxygen consumption of flying and walking barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis) and bar-headed geese (Anser indicus). J. Exp. Biol. 2002, 205, 3347–3356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kitchen, K.I.M.; Lill, A.; Price, M. Tolerance of human disturbance by urban Magpie-larkss. Aust. Field Ornithol. 2011, 28, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  31. Gosper, D.G. Aspects of breeding of the common Koel ‘Eudynamys scolopacea’ and one of its biological hosts, the Magpie-larks ‘Grallina cyanoleuca’. Aust. Bird Watch. 1997, 17, 9–11. [Google Scholar]
  32. Blumstein, D.T. Flight-initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance. J. Wildl. Manag. 2003, 67, 852–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Guay, P.-J.; Weston, M.A.; Symonds, M.R.E.; Glover, H.K. Brains and bravery: Little evidence of a relationship between brain size and flightiness in shorebirds. Austral Ecol. 2013, 38, 516–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Guay, P.-J.; McLeod, E.M.; Cross, R.; Formby, A.J.; Maldonado, S.P.; Stafford-Bell, R.E.; St-James-Turner, Z.N.; Robinson, R.W.; Mulder, R.A.; Weston, M.A. Observer effects occur when estimating alert but not flight-initiation distances. Wildl. Res. 2013, 40, 289–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. IBM Corp. SPSS Statistics, Version 28.0.; IBM: Chicago, IL, USA, 2021.
  36. Miller, S.G.; Knight, R.L.; Miller, C.K. Wildlife responses to pedestrians and dogs. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 2001, 29, 124–132. [Google Scholar]
  37. Sayol, F.; Sol, D.; Pigot, A.L. Brain size and life history interact to predict urban tolerance in birds. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2020, 8, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Maguire, G.S.; Miller, K.K.; Weston, M.A. Only the strictest rules apply: Investigating regulation compliance of beaches to minimize invasive dog impacts on threatened shorebird populations. In Impacts of Invasive Species on Coastal Environments: Coasts in Crisis; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2019; pp. 397–412. [Google Scholar]
  39. Randler, C. Disturbances by dog barking increase vigilance in coots Fulica atra. Eur. J. Wildl. Res. 2006, 52, 265–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Rubin, H.D.; Beck, A.M. Ecological behaviour of free ranging urban dogs. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 1982, 8, 161–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Forrest, A.; St Clair Colleen, C. Effects of dog leash laws and habitat type on avian and small mammal communities in urban parks. Urban Ecosyst. 2006, 9, 51–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Guinness, S.J.; Maguire, G.S.; Miller, K.K.; Weston, M.A. My dog, my beach! Attitudes towards dog management on Victorian beaches. Australas. J. Environ. Manag. 2020, 27, 329–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Møller, A.P.; Grim, T.; Ibáñez-Álamo, J.D.; Markó, G.; Tryjanowski, P. Change in flight initiation distance between urban and rural habitats following a cold winter. Behav. Ecol. 2013, 24, 1211–1217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hall, M.J.; Burns, A.L.; Martin, J.M.; Hochuli, D.F. Flight initiation distance changes across landscapes and habitats in a successful urban coloniser. Urban Ecosyst. 2020, 23, 785–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Gutzwiller, K.J. Minimizing dog-induced biases in game bird research. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 1990, 18, 351–356. [Google Scholar]
  46. Weston, M.A.; Yarwood, M.R.; Whisson, D.A.; Symonds, M.R. Persistent spatial gaps in ornithological study in Australia, 1901–2011. Arch. Nat. Hist. 2020, 47, 264–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Banks, P.B.; Bryant, J.V. Four-legged friend or foe? Dog walking displaces native birds from natural areas. Biol. Lett. 2007, 3, 611–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Parsons, H.; Major, R.E.; French, K. Species interactions and habitat associations of birds inhabiting urban areas of Sydney, Australia. Austral Ecol. 2006, 31, 217–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Uchida, K.; Suzuki, K.K.; Shimamoto, T.; Yanagawa, H.; Koizumi, I. Decreased vigilance or habituation to humans? Mechanisms on increased boldness in urban animals. Behav. Ecol. 2019, 30, 1583–1590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Belaire, J.A.; Whelan, C.J.; Minor, E.S. Having our yards and sharing them too: The collective effects of yards on native bird species in an urban landscape. Ecol. Appl. 2014, 24, 2132–2143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Doherty, T.S.; Dickman, C.R.; Glen, A.S.; Newsome, T.M.; Nimmo, D.G.; Ritchie, E.G.; Vanak, A.T.; Wirsing, A.J. The global impacts of domestic dogs on threatened vertebrates. Biol. Conserv. 2017, 210, 56–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lenth, B.E.; Knight, R.L.; Brennan, M.E. The effects of dogs on wildlife communities. Nat. Areas J. 2008, 28, 218–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Williams, K.; Weston, M.; Henry, S.; Maguire, G.S. Birds and beaches, dogs and leashes: Dog owners sense of obligation to leash dogs on beaches in Victoria, Australia. Hum. Dimens. Wildl. 2009, 14, 89–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Response mode/intensity of magpie-larks to approaches by a walker with dog on and off leash. The right panel (red) shows responses of magpie-larks to a leashed dog; the left panel (blue) shows responses of Magpie larks to an unleashed dog at an equivalent distance from the owner.
Figure 1. Response mode/intensity of magpie-larks to approaches by a walker with dog on and off leash. The right panel (red) shows responses of magpie-larks to a leashed dog; the left panel (blue) shows responses of Magpie larks to an unleashed dog at an equivalent distance from the owner.
Birds 04 00023 g001
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Barnett, S.C.; van Dongen, W.F.D.; Plotz, R.D.; Weston, M.A. Leash Status of Approaching Dogs Mediates Escape Modality but Not Flight-Initiation Distance in a Common Urban Bird. Birds 2023, 4, 277-283. https://doi.org/10.3390/birds4030023

AMA Style

Barnett SC, van Dongen WFD, Plotz RD, Weston MA. Leash Status of Approaching Dogs Mediates Escape Modality but Not Flight-Initiation Distance in a Common Urban Bird. Birds. 2023; 4(3):277-283. https://doi.org/10.3390/birds4030023

Chicago/Turabian Style

Barnett, Skye C., Wouter F. D. van Dongen, Roan D. Plotz, and Michael A. Weston. 2023. "Leash Status of Approaching Dogs Mediates Escape Modality but Not Flight-Initiation Distance in a Common Urban Bird" Birds 4, no. 3: 277-283. https://doi.org/10.3390/birds4030023

APA Style

Barnett, S. C., van Dongen, W. F. D., Plotz, R. D., & Weston, M. A. (2023). Leash Status of Approaching Dogs Mediates Escape Modality but Not Flight-Initiation Distance in a Common Urban Bird. Birds, 4(3), 277-283. https://doi.org/10.3390/birds4030023

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop