Next Article in Journal
“If I Died Tomorrow, I’d Die Knowing That My Son Is Safe, Loved by the People in Here, Well Cared for, and Happy”: Exploring Maternal Perspectives on Community Living for Their Adult Children with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities
Previous Article in Journal
Navigating Invisible Disability Disclosure and Workplace Inclusion: Employers’ Attitudes and Workplace Policies
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Lived Experiences of Public Disability Representations: A Scoping Review

by
Aartjan ter Haar
1,*,
Sander R. Hilberink
2 and
Alice Schippers
1
1
Disability Studies, University of Humanistic Studies, 3512 HD Utrecht, The Netherlands
2
Research Centre Innovations in Care, Rotterdam University of Applied Sciences, 3015 EK Rotterdam, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Disabilities 2025, 5(2), 38; https://doi.org/10.3390/disabilities5020038
Submission received: 11 February 2025 / Revised: 27 March 2025 / Accepted: 12 April 2025 / Published: 16 April 2025

Abstract

:
Research has largely focussed on public perceptions of people with disabilities, particularly in the media. However, there is a lack of studies exploring how individuals with disabilities themselves experience public disability representation. This scoping review examines the lived experiences of individuals with disabilities regarding such representations. Eligible studies were peer-reviewed, published between January 1948 and July 2024, and presented qualitative findings on experienced public disability representation. A systematic search was conducted across PubMed, Scopus, ProQuest, OVID, EBSCO, and Web of Science using variations of the terms ’representation’ and ‘disability’, yielding 32 relevant studies. Data were analysed using both deductive and inductive methods. Deductive thematic analysis aligned the findings with two established media disability representation typologies: traditional and contemporary. Inductive thematic analysis identified three key themes—ignorance, incapability, and otherness—that illustrate how individuals with disabilities experience public representations. Overall, the review found that despite the absence of comparative studies, individuals with diverse disabilities report similar, predominantly negative experiences of public disability representation. Future studies should adopt an intersectional, cross-disability framework to better capture the experiences of people with disabilities, including those with intellectual disabilities.

1. Introduction

Public disability representation has been extensively studied. For example, this research has proceeded by employing various typologies and scales to assess elements of disability representation in the media (e.g., [1,2]) or perusing disability stigma rooted in the broader concept of disability representation (e.g., [3,4]). The extent to which actual disability representations align with the experiences of individuals with disabilities remains unclear given the paucity of research regarding lived experiences of public disability representation. This disregard for the experiences of people with disabilities is especially striking given the urgent call from the United Nations Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities (UNCRPD) for immediate, effective, and appropriate actions to raise awareness, as well as the critical need to consult and involve people with disabilities in all decision making processes concerning issues relating to persons with disabilities [5].

1.1. Public Disability Representation or Stigma?

Understanding the lived experiences of individuals with disabilities is crucial for addressing disability awareness and public disability representation. Webb [6] (p. 6) defines ‘representation’ as ‘how we experience and communicate ourselves and the world we inhabit, how we know ourselves, and how we deal with others’. Representation encompasses a range of constructs, including knowledge, beliefs, attitudes, value judgements, perceptions, stereotypes, stigmas, and labels. In each instance, an equivalence is claimed between representation and object, between signifier and signified. Shakespeare [7] identified everyday interactions, media, charitable imagery, and popular assumptions as sources of disability representations. Webb [6] contends that representation is relevant not merely as a reflection of reality but as a lens through which people perceive and interpret the world. Representation constructs and influences our understanding of the world. In this study, we use the plural ‘representations’ to denote specific images or depictions in contrast to the singular ‘representation’. ‘Public disability representations’ encompass representations of disabled people, while ‘media disability representations’ focus on how this group is depicted in the media. Furthermore, the terms ‘disabled’ and ‘with a disability’ are utilised interchangeably in this study.
In this context, it is essential to look at the relationship between stigma and representation. Stigma represents deviation from a specific ‘preferred’ characteristic. Goffman [8] (p. 3) defined stigma as an ‘attribute that is profoundly discrediting’, transforming the stigmatised individual from a ‘complete’ and ‘normal’ person into one that is tainted and devalued. It is essential to emphasise that deviance is not an intrinsic characteristic and that individuals should not be labelled as deviant [8] (p. 3). Some four decades later, Link and Phelan [9] revisited this concept, defining stigma as the concurrent presence of labelling, stereotyping, separation, status loss, and discrimination within a power structure that enables such behaviours [9] (p. 382). Their framework conceptualises labelling as identifying socially significant distinctions, with stereotypes representing the cognitive aspect, prejudices representing the affective aspect, and discrimination representing the behavioural aspect (e.g., [3]). These definitions of stigma highlight two key distinctions from representation: (1) stigma is inherently negative, and (2) stigma emerges from power differentials. Goffman [8] and Link and Phelan [9] conceptualise stigma as an undesirable deviation from societal norms, portraying the stigmatisation of people with disabilities by ‘us normals’ as a process of defining them as ‘others’ [10]. Furthermore, stigma presumes the existence of power dynamics [9] (p. 376). Conversely, representation is neither inherently normative nor negative, and it is unrelated to differences in authority. To avoid the inherent negative connotations associated with stigma, this study focusses on examining representation.

1.2. Examining Experienced Public Disability Representation

Previous methods for researching lived experiences of public disability representations include stigma scales, disability models, and media typologies.

1.2.1. Disability Stigma Scales

Researchers have employed disability stigma scales to examine representation experiences among individuals with various disabilities (e.g., [11,12,13]). Incorporating the stigma experiences of individuals with disabilities via qualitative methods was not applied consistently across all scales. Mbuba et al. [13] performed interviews and focus group discussions with individuals with epilepsy to identify relevant issues related to perceived stigma, which guided the development of their perceived stigma scale. Ali et al. [11] took a less in-depth approach by inviting six people with mild to moderate intellectual disabilities to comment on the understandability of stigma statements taken from a literature review and prioritised by professionals. Boyle [12] employed four items to evaluate stigma feelings selected after reviewing multiple disability stigma scales; however, people who stuttered were not consulted.

1.2.2. Disability Models

An alternative to applying stigma scales is obtaining insight into the social positioning of disabled individuals regarding public disability representations through disability models or paradigms. Implicit beliefs influence public disability representations, including internalised representations. Disability models offer a framework for understanding these representations by defining disability, delineating the parties responsible, and outlining strategies for resolution. Four main models, along with their variants, conceptualise disability: the religious, charity, medical, and social models (Table 1). The religious or moral model is historically considered to have preceded the medical model [14]. While the charity model is regarded as a variation or extension of the religious model [15], the social model was developed as a reaction to the medical model [16,17]. According to the religious model, which represents a blend of social, cultural, and religious beliefs [18,19], people with disabilities are either holy and innocent beings [20] or hazardous victims of supernatural forces [19,21,22]. While this model predominantly results in adverse effects for individuals with disabilities, the charity model emphasises their welfare. The specialised care they need is offered as charity [15,23]. The medical model conceptualises disability as primarily an individual medical issue [15,24]. In contrast, the social model argues that people with disabilities are not disabled by their impairments but by the disabling barriers that they face in society [25].
Additional disability models comprise the human rights model of disabilities, as reflected in the UN Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities (UNCRPD) [5], and the biopsychosocial model of disabilities, as reflected in the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) [26]. In the biopsychosocial model, disabilities are conceptualised as resulting from the interaction between internal personal factors and external environmental factors. By integrating environmental factors, this model bridges the strict separation between the medical and social models. The biopsychosocial model is reflected by the combined occurrence of representations from the medical and social models, with a modest emphasis on the representations from the medical model. In the human rights model, persons with disabilities are conceptualised as rights holders and human rights subjects. Degener [27] argues that the human rights model extends and improves upon the social model; the human rights model is reflected by representations from the social model, with a strong emphasis on disabled persons as human rights holders.
The existing disability models are broadly formulated; they coexist, interrelate, and include overlapping disability concepts [14]. A specific context may prioritise certain aspects of a model over others [14].
Table 1. Four main disability models.
Table 1. Four main disability models.
Models
A. The Religious Model
People with disabilities are either innocent and holy children of God and deserving of charity [20] or dangerous people whose disabilities are attributed to divine retribution, witchcraft, or evil spirits punishing them for immoral actions or sins committed by the individual, their mothers, or their families [19,21,22]. Remedies are sought from spiritual healers [19]. The individual with the disability displays non-human characteristics or is not considered a real human [21]. This may lead to detrimental actions, including the killing of newborns [18].
B. The Charity Model
People with disabilities are eternal children or tragic, depressed victims who need specialised and segregated care and protection because of their helplessness and dependency, offered as charity through religious organisations, concerned citizens, or the government [15,23]. This resulted in the creation of the first ‘madhouses’ and charitable organisations in the 1800s [28], alongside contemporary charity campaigns, collections, and telethons.
C. The Medical Model
People with disabilities fail to meet societal standards due to ‘long-term physical, mental, intellectual, or sensory impairments’ [5] (art. 1). They deviate from the norm. The individual bears responsibility for the solution, which may encompass recovery, rehabilitation, or coping strategies. Should this endeavour fail, the individual may be viewed as a pitiful victim of circumstance [28] or a passive one who should have exerted greater effort. If successful, the individual is inspirational.
D. The Social Model
People with disabilities are members of groups of people who are disabled by the barriers that they face in society. Inaccessible physical, social, and occupational environments, along with negative attitudes, hinder their equal participation. ‘Social model thinking mandates barrier removal, antidiscrimination legislation, independent living and other responses to social oppression’ [25] (p. 216).

1.2.3. Disability Media Typologies

To better understand how disabled people are portrayed by the media, scholars have developed various typologies of disability representations (e.g., [29,30]). Barnes approached this issue from an insider perspective. His study originated from an increasing awareness among people with disabilities that the obstacles that they encounter are a result of institutional discrimination and that the media’s misrepresentation of disability plays a substantial role in perpetuating this discrimination [31] (p. 5). Barnes’ analysis led to a typology of 11 commonly recurring media stereotypes [31]. Clogston [32] utilised the medical and social models to distinguish between traditional and progressive typologies. Haller [33,34] later refined these typologies, resulting in the Clogston–Haller framework, which identifies eight distinct media representations. Building on the Clogston–Haller approach, other scholars have developed similar typologies in their studies of media disability representation (e.g., [2,35]).

1.3. Research Aim

The Clogston–Haller media typologies have been widely used to analyse how disability is represented in the media, both as perceived by the general public (e.g., [35,36]) and by people with disabilities (e.g., [37,38]). Barnes, a disabled scholar himself, developed his typologies through collaboration with a research advisory group of disabled people who shared their insights and experiences. Nonetheless, research on the lived experiences of individuals with disabilities concerning public disability representation has remained scarce. Although stigma scales offer valuable quantitative insights into the stigma faced by individuals with disabilities, they fail to fully capture the nature of public disability representations as experienced by this group. Although the four disability models and their variations encompass a broad range of general disability representations, their contribution to understanding the lived experiences of public disability representations remains unclear. Similarly, while the Clogston–Haller typologies provide well-defined frameworks, their predetermined structures may offer only a limited perspective on how individuals with disabilities experience public representations. The extent to which these models and typologies truly capture these experiences remains ambiguous. This scoping review addresses this gap by examining qualitative, peer-reviewed research on public disability representations experienced by people with disabilities. A comprehensive understanding of the multifaceted representation experiences of individuals with disabilities requires examination from multiple perspectives. Therefore, this exploratory scoping review will analyse the studies from three distinct perspectives. We examine the alignment of these studies with the four disability models, evaluate their alignment with the Barnes and Clogston–Haller media typologies, and explore the construction of alternative themes that better reflect lived experiences.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Design and Theoretical Framework

We conducted this scoping review following the PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA ScR) checklist [39]. Scoping reviews are useful for identifying the extent of research conducted on a given topic and the current understanding of that topic [40]. The methodology employed was based on scoping review steps outlined by Arksey and O’Malley [41] and refined by Levac et al. [42] and Peters et al. [43]. The Arksey and O’Malley framework suggests five steps when performing scoping reviews: (1) identifying research questions; (2) identifying relevant studies; (3) selecting studies; (4) charting the data; and (5) collating, summarising, and reporting the results.

2.2. Identifying Research Questions

The purpose of our study was to investigate how people with disabilities experience public disability representations. To fulfil this aim, we addressed the following research questions. (1) To what extent are the experienced representations observed in the studies reflected in the four disability models (religious, charity, medical, and social) (Table 1)? (2) To what extent are the experienced public disability representations observed in the studies reflected in Barnes’s and Clogston–Haller’s traditional or contemporary media typologies (Table 2)? (3) What alternative themes could better capture experienced public disability representations?

2.3. Identifying Relevant Studies

The search terms were structured to define relevant population characteristics and core concepts [43]. The study population included individuals with disabilities, with no restrictions on disability type. Regarding core concepts, studies needed to focus on experienced public disability representation, including examinations of stereotypes and stigma.
Because a pilot literature search showed that studies referring to a specific disability sometimes lacked the terms ‘handicap’ or ‘disability’, the following search terms were also employed: ‘autism’, ‘blind’, ‘brain injury’, ‘cerebral palsy’, ‘cleft lip and palate’, ‘deaf’, ‘Down syndrome’, ‘epilepsy’, ‘muscular dystrophy’, ‘osteogenesis imperfecta’, ‘poliomyelitis’, ‘restricted growth’, ‘spina bifida’, ‘spinal cord injury’, and variations thereof. For public disability representation, the search terms included ‘representation’, ‘stereotype’, ‘stigma’, ‘attitude’, and variations thereof. The search terms ‘experienced’, ‘perceived’, and variations thereof were combined, in addition to ‘experience of disability’.
On 30 July 2024, we conducted a structured search across six databases: PubMed (including Medline), Scopus, ProQuest (Sociological Abstracts), OVID (APA and Eric), EBSCO (Academic Search Premier), and Web of Science (Core Collection). Search platforms vary in functionality, requiring slight adjustments to our search methods while maintaining consistency. Google Scholar was used as a supplementary source, with separate searches conducted for each disability type. Given the search engine’s declining accuracy beyond the initial pages, we limited our analysis to the first 75 results [44]. Additionally, we performed citation tracking and reference checking for the included studies.

2.4. Study Selection

Original qualitative research studies exploring the experienced public disability representation of people with disabilities published in English-language articles between January 1948 and July 2024 were eligible for inclusion (Table 3). Studies employing mixed methods were included if the qualitative component was reported separately from the quantitative findings. Studies that failed to analyse public disability representations or classify and label these representations were excluded.
The prolonged period from 1948 to 2024 was selected due to the apparent scarcity of research on public disability representation as experienced by people with disabilities. This prolonged search period increases the likelihood of capturing a substantial number of relevant studies. The conclusion of World War II, including the return of disabled veterans, initiated global reconstruction efforts, encompassing improvements in care and services for individuals with disabilities. The year 1948 marked the publication of the Universal Declaration of Human Rights (UDHR) [45]. Given the review’s focus on lived experiences, we analysed qualitative findings, as qualitative research provides an in-depth and contextual understanding of individuals’ experiences, lives, and social environments. To ensure a minimum level of quality, only peer-reviewed journal studies were included.
Figure 1 shows a flowchart of the process of identifying and selecting relevant studies. In the identification phase, records were identified and imported into RefWorks, where duplicates were excluded. The first author screened the titles and abstracts of the remaining records for relevance, considering the inclusion criteria. In case of uncertainty, the records were retained. The remaining full-text studies were assessed based on inclusion and exclusion criteria by the first author. In cases of ambiguity concerning inclusion or exclusion, the second author was consulted, resulting in a joint decision by both authors. The review and selection processes were discussed by the three authors.

2.5. Charting the Data

Our analysis focused on the sections where researchers analysed, classified, and labelled the public disability representation experiences of their participants. Excel was used to create data charting tables [43] containing key information about the included studies, such as country characteristics [46], methodologies, participant characteristics, and summaries of the findings.

2.6. Collating, Summarising, and Reporting the Results

We present the primary features of the included studies, including disability types, research methods, geographical locations, and core concepts, in both a tabular format and a concise narrative account. Using qualitative data analysis software (ATLAS.ti version 9.1.7.0), we also conducted both deductive and inductive thematic analyses of the sections detailing reported experiences with public disability representation. Thematic analysis was employed deductively [47] to evaluate the extent to which the four disability models (Table 1) and the Barnes and Clogston–Haller typologies (Table 2) were reflected in the studies. The models and typologies served as themes; the media representations served as subthemes.
The six-stage inductive approach outlined by Terry et al. [48] was employed to construct alternative themes. (1) After having selected the relevant passages during the deductive analysis process, the first author reread the articles and noted initial codes and themes. This resulted in 325 quotations. (2) The first author employed open coding to develop and modify the codes during the coding process. (3) Subsequently, the first author identified 26 initial themes by sorting the codes into thematic groups. The first and second authors discussed these initial themes and relevant quotations, leading to the construction of three preliminary main themes: (1) feeling misconceived due to a lack of knowledge; (2) feeling underestimated due to low expectations; and (3) feeling dehumanised due to experienced representations of not belonging. (4) A discussion among all authors was conducted to refine the final coding framework, which was based on the initial codes, their interrelationships, and how they indicated underlying public disability representation experiences. (5) The three authors refined and renamed the subsequent main themes and sub-themes. A thematic map was developed to illustrate the three interrelated primary themes: (1) experiencing ignorance, (2) experiencing assumed limited capabilities, and (3) experiencing otherness. The three themes are interrelated and iterative. (6) The last stage involved producing the review, which encompassed the final analysis, the selection of pertinent quotations to illustrate the findings, and the composition of the article. Findings and interpretations of the analysis outcomes are presented below.

3. Results

In this section, we present (1) the selection of studies, (2) the methodological characteristics of the studies, and (3) the findings of the studies regarding experienced public disability representation.

3.1. Selection of the Studies

The initial search yielded 17,626 abstracts, to which abstracts of three articles identified from citation tracking and reference checking were added; furthermore, 4101 of these were duplicates and excluded. The remaining 12,178 studies were screened for relevance based on the title and type of study, and 11,307 were excluded. After screening the abstracts of 871 studies, 764 were excluded. We then assessed the full text of the remaining 107 studies for eligibility, and 75 were excluded, primarily because they did not analyse public disability representations experienced by people with disabilities. The 32 studies that met all inclusion requirements were included in the analysis (Figure 1).

3.2. Characteristics of the Studies

The 32 selected studies covered 16 countries, including 19 studies from high-income countries, such as the United States (n = 8) and the United Kingdom (n = 5) (Table 4). The number of studies increased from 4 published between 1980 and 2000 to 4 between 2001 and 2010, 11 between 2011 and 2020, and 13 studies in the past four years. The studies included participants with various disabilities, such as physical disabilities (n = 14), sensory disabilities (n = 9), and epilepsy (n = 7). Four studies examined mixed disabilities. Studies on the experiences of people with intellectual disabilities were lacking. The ages of the sample participants ranged from 6 to 88 years. Participants varied in socioeconomic status (SES) and education level. No studies compared the experiences of disability representations among distinct groups of individuals with disabilities.

3.3. Public Disability Representation Experiences

Half of the studies examined the public disability representation experiences from a specific perspective. For example, two studies examined disability representations and coping [52,78], two examined representation experiences and assistive devices [54,72], and two examined representation experiences in the context of work [74,79].
The term ’stigma’ appeared in 22 studies. Alongside ’stigma’, 16 additional core concepts were utilised. Eleven studies used ’perception’ and ’attitudes’, while eight used ’stereotype’. A total of 28 studies used multiple core concepts.
The studies examined and clustered the disability representations as experienced by participants across various perspectives. While some studies provided detailed explanations with numerous examples, most presented relatively succinct descriptions of the clustered representations. Consequently, a direct comparison of the findings was not feasible. Therefore, as outlined in Section 2.5, we conducted both deductive and inductive thematic analyses of the relevant sections from the selected studies, in which researchers analysed, classified, and labelled participants’ experiences of public disability representation.

3.3.1. Deductive Thematic Analysis

The deductive analysis indicated that 31 out of 32 studies included one or more experienced disability representation that could be classified into the medical model (Table 5). Eleven studies included representations aligned with the religious model, while seven studies featured representations aligned with the charity model. None of the studies included representations that aligned with the social model.
Representations aligned with the charity model were predominantly found in studies from high-income countries, whereas those aligned with the religious model were more common in studies from low- and middle-income countries. No major differences were observed across studies based on disability type.
Following the frameworks of Barnes [31], Clogston [32], and Haller [33,34], two overarching typologies of media disability representations were identified: traditional and contemporary (Table 6). A deductive analysis revealed that all 32 studies included one or more representations aligned with traditional typologies, while only 6 studies featured representations aligned with contemporary typologies. Traditional typologies encompassed the following representations:
  • ‘Incapable of Participating Fully in Community Life’ (n = 17) [50,53,56,57,58,59,62,64,65,67,69,70,72,74,75,76,79], e.g., ‘The traditional view of disability in Timor Leste is that a person with disabilities cannot be independent, work, marry and have a family or travel’ [79].
  • ‘Medical’ (n = 16), which suggests illness or dysfunction resulting in dependency and passivity [51,54,56,57,60,63,64,65,67,70,72,73,74,76,77,79], e.g., ‘Epilepsy is a contagious disease: “Some didn’t even shake hands to say hello because they thought it was contagious”’ [67].
  • ‘Sinister and Evil’ (n = 15) [49,50,54,55,58,59,60,62,65,66,67,69,78,79,80], e.g., ‘According to the participants, Sierra Leoneans traditionally believe either that people with disabilities are devils in human bodies or that the disability has been caused by witchcraft’ [50].
  • ‘Pitiable and Pathetic’ (n = 11), ‘Feelings of sorry or pity are a common type of stigma surrounding hearing loss according to participants—a reaction that participants indicated strongly disliking’ [68].
  • ‘Burden’ (n = 10), indicating that individuals are presumed to be unable to cope independently [50,56,57,63,64,70,74,76,79], e.g., ‘The other half of participants experienced being rejected by family, most commonly in an early stage of disability, when they were seen as a burden and as someone who was unable to contribute to the family’s daily living’ [50].
In contrast, alongside the frequently referenced traditional representations mentioned previously, there were also less frequently observed traditional representations. These include the ‘Social Pathology’ representation (n = 4), which relates to reliance on governmental or societal economic support [50,56,75,79], the ‘Super Cripple/Supercrip’ representation (n = 2) [53,63], and the ‘Business’ representation (n = 2), which relates to economic costs, including accessibility and inclusion [74,77]. The contemporary typology included only the ‘Cultural Pluralism/Normal’ representation (n = 6) [62,64,69,75,77,78], which portrays individuals with disabilities as multifaceted persons, with disability being just one aspect of their identity. ‘When the actions of organizations and institutions enhanced participants’ access to resources, participants perceived societal attitudes as “understanding”. The sense of inclusion this created made participants feel accepted, supported, and valued as members of society’ [77].
No major differences were observed in the presence of the four models and the typologies between studies from high-income and low- to middle-income countries, nor across studies examining different disability types.

3.3.2. Inductive Thematic Analysis

The inductive thematic analysis of clustered experiences of public disability representations resulted in the construction of three interrelated themes: (1) experiencing ignorance, (2) experiencing assumed limited capabilities, and (3) experiencing otherness (Table 7). The three themes are interrelated and iterative (Figure 2).

Experiencing Ignorance

The theme of experiencing ignorance reflects experiences of ignorance about disabilities or about people with disabilities in public disability representations. Individuals with disabilities often find public representations inaccurate, as inaccuracy arises from widespread misconceptions or a lack of adequate knowledge about disabilities or about people with disabilities. This ignorance encompassed the causes, cures, and characteristics of the disability. Disability was regarded as an illness [51,55,56,63,65,76]. Ignorance about causes [50,54,55,58,60,62,65,67,69,71,77,79] included vaccinations [50,55,58], divine punishment [69,79], and curses or witchcraft [54,58,69], while some participants said they ‘experienced being rejected because non-disabled people thought the disability was contagious’ [50,54,60,65,67,71,77]. Misconceptions about cures were also addressed [54,62,67,69], as in the following: ‘A person having an epileptic seizure needs to have something hard in the mouth as bite wedge’ [67]; ‘It was common to visit traditional healers post-injury, which informants turn to mainly due to pressure by their family or friends, or to show respect to their parents’ [69]. Common misconceptions regarding people with disabilities encompassed the notion that these individuals constitute a homogeneous group [57,75,80], the assumption that disability serves as the main distinguishing feature of a person [52,54,56,57,59,63,70], and the belief that disability spreads [57,67,68,70,75,76]. One study reported: ‘A man remarked on the tendency not only to treat visually impaired people as stupid but as if they are all ‘the same’; that is, to focus only on their disability: “If you’re in a group of blind people you’re treated as if you all have the same IQ [intelligence quotient] as the most backward member of the group”’ [57]. In total, 26 out of 32 studies mentioned experiences of ignorance.

Experiencing Assumed Limited Capabilities

The theme of limited capabilities pertained to undervaluing the capabilities of individuals with disabilities, often in tandem with expressions of ignorance. Examples included dependency, academic and professional inabilities, and an inability to address ‘adult needs’. People with disabilities were assumed to be unable to cope independently [56,57,58,62,63,64,65,67,69,70,72,73,75,76,79] because, among other characteristics, they are powerless [73], helpless [65,73,76], or need protection [56,57,75]. An assumed lack of intelligence [53,57,59,61,62,65,67,68,72,75] was mentioned, as well as educational incapabilities [50,56,62,67,75,76]. Being less able or even unable to work professionally [50,52,56,58,60,62,64,67,69,70,74,75,76,77,79] became apparent in representations of people with disabilities as beggars [50,52,75], unskilled [74,75], incapable of working under pressure [67], or not being able to travel [67,75,79]. One study highlighted that ‘Participants experienced being rejected by family, most commonly in an early stage of disability, when they were considered a burden, being unable to contribute to the family’s daily living’ [50]. References to asexuality [53,56,70,75], recommendations against marriage [53,56,60,75,76,79], and perceptions of incapacity for parenting [53,56,67,75] all underscored assumed failures to meet ‘adult’ needs [53,56,58,60,67,70,75,76,79]. As reported in one study, ‘Additionally, and most upsetting for focus group participants, was the lack of acknowledgment of “adult needs and adult desires”, as John eloquently stated. The desire to date, have an intimate relationship, marry, and have children was often glossed over by practitioners, thus denying an important aspect of adulthood”’ [53]. A total of 25 out of 32 studies mentioned experiences of assumed limited capabilities.

Experiencing Otherness

The theme of experiencing otherness reflects that individuals with disabilities frequently encounter dehumanising representations that position them as ‘the other’. Representations of otherness were frequently mentioned in combination with ignorance and assumptions about limited capabilities. Instances of ‘otherness’, placing individuals with disabilities on the fringes of society, encompassed representations of pitiable individuals [52,53,54,56,57,61,63,65,68,71,75,76], imposters [55,56,57,67,68,73,80], possessing a ‘bad’ trait [49,50,55,57,58,59,60,62,65,67,73,77,78], devalued social identities [50,51,53,58,65,67,68,69,70,72,74,76,77,78], or exclusion [49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,61,62,63,64,65,67,68,69,70,71,73,75,76,77,79,80]. Babamohamadi et al. [52] characterised the pity representation in terms like ‘pathetic’ and ‘doomed’, Bagatell et al. [53] used the term ‘inspirational’, as did Ocran [75], as follows: ‘For most of the participants, an important manifestation of disability stigma lies in the show of unsolicited pity and sympathy towards them. … Their rejection of the sympathy lies in their awareness that people tend to have lowered expectations for persons with disability’. The imposter representation corresponded with perceptions of fraudulence that emerge from challenging disability stereotypes, including being blind and attending grammar school or misusing welfare [56]. One study reported ‘Some of the interviewees … had found that their competence in public was interpreted as evidence that they were “frauds”’ [57]. The possession of a ‘bad’ trait was linked to individuals with disabilities being perceived as troublemakers [58], violent [55], dangerous [49,55,67], or evil [50,59,78]. Depictions of devalued social identity included notions of inferiority [50,53,65,67,69,70,72,77], damaged goods [76], or garbage [70]. References to being perceived as abnormal [56,57,61,63,64,68,71,76,80], not regarded as human beings [50,55,67,70,75,77], viewed as something to be hidden [69,76], and other instances exemplified the representation of ‘not belonging’. Overall, 31 out of 32 studies mentioned representations of otherness. Furthermore, ten studies referenced positive representations [49,50,51,62,64,65,69,72,75,77,78], including the depiction of persons with disabilities as individual human beings deserving of respect and acceptance [49,50,69,72,77,78] and the characterisation of a disability as a condition that should not invoke embarrassment [78]. One study explained: ‘Some significant others, however, including some parents, adopt strategies of openness, honesty and neutralization. Parents who define their child’s epilepsy “just like any other medical problem” and “certainly nothing to be ashamed of” apparently encourage their children to have a much more neutral view and a more open informational control strategy. One successful businessman credited his parents with managing epilepsy so as to minimize it and prevent him from using it as a “crutch” or “excuse”: “The parents of an epileptic child are the key to the whole ball of wax, in recognizing that you have a problem in the family but not to let that control the total actions and whole livelihood and whole future of the family. Accept it and go about doing what has to be done to maintain an even keel”’ [78].

4. Discussion

4.1. Summary of Evidence

This scoping review examined lived experiences of individuals with disabilities regarding public disability representation. The findings revealed an increase in related studies since the 1990s, with 32 studies identified in both high-income and low- to middle-income countries. The analysis indicated that individuals with diverse disabilities share primarily negative experiences. A deductive thematic analysis indicated that while the four disability models—religious, charity, medical, and social—partially captured these experiences, they did not fully reflect their complexity. Notably, representations aligned with the social model were absent in the studies. Furthermore, the two typologies of media disability representations constructed by Barnes [31], Clogston [32], and Haller [33,34]—traditional and contemporary—partially reflected the main findings from the studies. The studies did not mention predominantly positive contemporary media representations.
The existing disability models and media typologies fail to fully capture all experiences of disability representations. Through inductive thematic analysis, we identified three interrelated themes that illustrate the relationships among the representations: insufficient knowledge, presumed incapacity, and the otherness of individuals with disabilities. These representations frequently depict individuals with disabilities as severely disabled and lacking in educational and occupational capabilities, contributing to dehumanisation and social exclusion.

4.2. Experienced Public Disability Representations

4.2.1. Increased Focus on Lived Experiences

Although public disability representation has received attention for an extended period (e.g., [29,30,81,82]), only recently has research on experienced public disability representations become more prevalent. As early as 1992, Barnes [31], a disabled academic and advocate, observed that there is increasing awareness among individuals with disabilities that stereotypical assumptions about them stem from superstition, myths, and beliefs from earlier, less enlightened periods (p. 6). He argued that these representations have detrimental effects on both disabled individuals and society at large (p. 19). Nevertheless, for a long time, there was a scarcity of research regarding public disability representation as experienced by individuals with disabilities. Several factors may have contributed to the recent rise in research concerning lived experiences. Public disability representations received little attention at all prior to 1990. The prevailing medical model at the time regarded disability as an individual issue (e.g., [17]). With the rise of the social model, which highlights the influence of societal structures and barriers, awareness grew concerning the detrimental effects of media representations (e.g., [17,83]). Consequently, the 1990s experienced an increase in studies focused on disability representations in the media (e.g., [29,30,32]). However, research into experienced disability representation was still largely missing. Critiques from scholars, such as Shakespeare [25] and Reeve [84], regarding the social model’s emphasis on the collective aspects of disabilities, alongside the growth of Disability Studies as a scholarly discipline championed by scholars like Barnes [85], seem to have resulted in heightened focus on the lived experiences of individuals with disabilities. Research on experienced public disability representation has increased since 2010, reflecting this trend.
The focus of the social model on social constraints affecting the collective of disabled people likely had an additional impact. The social model’s focus on social constraints affecting the collective of persons with disabilities may have resulted in the lack of studies consistent with this model, as we analysed studies focussing on the personal experiences of individuals with disabilities.

4.2.2. Predominantly Unfavourable Representations?

According to Richards et al. [86] and Schanke et al. [87], positive representations are crucial for fostering the personal development of people with disabilities and reshaping public perceptions. As a result, unlike many stigma studies (e.g., [3,11,12,88,89,90]), we did not wish to approach representation negatively. Examining public representation of disabilities through a stigma lens neglects the consideration of any potential positive dimensions. Stigma is defined by Goffman [8] and Link and Phelan [9] as being inherently negative and resulting from power disparities. Nevertheless, we found that individuals with disabilities largely associate public disability representation with negative experiences, despite acknowledging positive experiences, as well. Partly, this may be accounted for by the use of the negatively charged search term ‘stigma’. Indeed, almost 70% of the studies appeared to adhere to the stigma traditions of Goffman [8], Link and Phelan [9], and others. However, the perspective of the reviewed studies may also influence the number of negative experiences. For example, two studies examined coping mechanisms [52,78]. Coping mechanisms inherently assume the management of adverse situations. Furthermore, four studies labelled people with disabilities as ‘patients’ [65,67,69,77]. This aligns with the medical model and reinforces the notion that disability is an abnormal state requiring intervention. Consequently, it remains unclear whether individuals with disabilities predominantly experience negative public disability representations or if this finding is influenced by bias in the reviewed studies.

4.2.3. Reflecting Multifaceted Experiences

Retief [15], Shakespeare [25], and Swain [28] emphasise the significance of disability models for understanding the lives of people with disabilities. Models define disability, shape public disability representations, and affect the self-perceptions of disabled individuals. Media disability typologies facilitate a better understanding of media portrayals of people with disabilities [29,30,31,32,33,34,35]. To what extent do disability models and media typologies facilitate the understanding of experienced disability representations of individuals with disabilities? The deductive analysis indicated that the models and typologies only partially captured the experienced public disability representations.
Regarding disability models, this incomplete reflection may be partially due to the extensive and overlapping definitions of the models, as well as the prioritisation or de-emphasis of specific aspects based on context. Almost all studies identified representations that aligned with the medical model, while none aligned with the social model. It is plausible that individuals with disabilities encounter disability representations that primarily align with the medical model. The public may not perceive individuals with disabilities through a social model; people with disabilities may not recognise representations that align with it. Scholars like Shakespeare and Swain value disability models, but the models seem inadequate for understanding experienced public disability representations.
Barnes’s and Clogston–Haller’s media typologies, however, appear to reflect the lived experiences of individuals with disabilities. Nonetheless, these typologies do not to fully capture the specificity of certain experienced representations. Among other things, the typologies do not account for the ‘disability spreads’ representation, which refers to the assumption that a disability affects other aspects of one’s personality, as well; for instance, having a sensory disability implies also having a physical disability. Phillips [76] refers to the experience that people think ‘that form evokes function, that physiological damage spreads, albeit metaphorically, to the total personality and life circumstances of the disabled individual’. This instance of not capturing all experienced representations may arise from the fact that the typologies do not originate from the lived experiences of individuals with disabilities but instead from academic analyses of disability representations in the media. Moreover, media typologies possess inherent limitations, frequently referencing actions portrayed in films or television shows like ‘object of ridicule’ and ‘object of violence’. Typologies tend to be concrete, recognisable, and tangible, often accompanied by catchy titles. Media disability typologies serve as useful tools for analysing representations in the media; however, they fall short of providing a comprehensive understanding of the multifaceted and interconnected experiences of individuals with disabilities.
We were able to construct three interrelated themes—ignorance, limited capabilities, and otherness. The themes reflect the multifaceted and predominantly negative experiences of public disability representation among individuals with disabilities. They align with the fears of people with disabilities that these representations may result in the perpetuation of injustices, including restricted opportunities, exclusion, and a sense of diminished life value (e.g., [64,74,76,77]). Moreover, these representations could reinforce self-stigma (e.g., [51,58,80,88]). This scoping review underscores the necessity of investigating the lived experiences of individuals with disabilities, thereby enhancing the limited research on the effects of disability representations to date (e.g., [38,89,91]). Such experiences are crucial for developing policies concerning disability awareness and representation [5] (art. 8).

4.3. Limitations

By limiting our research to available online, qualitative, peer-reviewed, English-language studies, our findings may be biased. For instance, the experiences of individuals with disabilities from specific geographical areas and printed publications may have been omitted.
The deductive analysis examined the extent to which the experienced public disability representations in the reviewed studies were reflected in four disability models: the religious model, the charity model, the medical model, and the social model. The overview of disability models included the biopsychosocial model and the human rights model, as well. Nevertheless, the analysis did not consider these models due to their overlap with the medical and social models. The omission of these models from the deductive analysis may be considered a limitation of the scoping review.
Another limitation is the use of the search terms ‘representation’, ‘stereotype’, ‘stigma’, and ‘attitude’. Alongside these search terms, the studies employed alternative core concepts to denote public disability representation. Terms such as ‘belief’, ‘label’, ‘meaning’, and ‘prejudice’ were also found. The presence of alternative core concepts in the included studies suggests that we may have overlooked studies that exclusively employed these alternative concepts. Nevertheless, this reflects the outcome of the current search.

4.4. Recommendations for Future Studies

While most studies included in this scoping review provided detailed descriptions of participants’ disability types, there was a tendency to treat them as homogeneous groups. These studies largely overlooked how different types of disability, along with additional factors, such as public roles, gender, ethnicity, religion, educational attainment, and socioeconomic status, might influence individuals’ experiences of public disability representation. Further research is needed to explore these variations and examine the role of intersectionality in shaping these experiences. Notably, future studies should also incorporate the perspectives of individuals with intellectual disabilities. The PhotoVoice studies featured in this review [70,79] highlight the effectiveness of arts-based methods in disability representation research, particularly when verbal data collection techniques are less applicable [86]. Additionally, we encourage future studies to remain open to the potential positive aspects of public disability representations by avoiding specific perspectives, such as coping, which may bias participants’ experiences. Investigating the differences in experiences across specific representation models, such as the religious and charity models, would provide further insight.
This study focused on the overarching theme of public disability representation and found that multiple terms, including ‘stigma’, ‘stereotype’, ‘attitude’, and ‘representation’, are used to describe this concept, each highlighting different aspects. Researchers should be mindful that the choice of core terminology may influence study outcomes. Therefore, we recommend adopting a comprehensive definition of disability representation and clarifying its meaning for participants through the use of diverse terminology.

5. Conclusions

This scoping review examined the public representation experiences of individuals with disabilities. While media disability representation typologies effectively capture many of the predominantly negative experiences reported, they fail to fully reflect the nuanced lived experiences detailed in the examined studies. To address this gap, three interrelated themes were developed: ‘Ignorance’, ‘Limited Capabilities’, and ‘Otherness’.
Notably, the studies included in this review did not include the lived experiences of individuals with intellectual disabilities. Furthermore, there is a lack of comparative research examining insider perspectives across different types of disabilities. Future studies should adopt an intersectional, cross-disability approach to deepen the understanding of public disability representations from the perspective of individuals with disabilities. Such research should also explore the potential positive aspects of these representations. Evaluating the importance of public disability representations from the perspective of individuals with disabilities is crucial, particularly in light of the UNCRPD’s recommendations for increasing awareness.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, A.t.H., S.R.H. and A.S.; methodology, A.t.H. and S.R.H.; validation, A.t.H., S.R.H. and A.S.; formal analysis, A.t.H.; investigation, A.t.H.; resources, A.t.H.; data curation, A.t.H.; writing—original draft preparation, A.t.H.; writing—review and editing, A.t.H., S.R.H. and A.S.; visualisation, A.t.H.; supervision, S.R.H. and A.S.; project administration, A.t.H. Given the emphasis on experienced disability representation, we find it important to briefly acknowledge the authors’ personal positions. The first author (A.t.H.) has worked as a program manager for several disability non-governmental organisations, while the second (S.R.H.) and third (A.S.) authors teach disability studies at the university level. The first author grew up in a family with (hidden) psychiatric issues and experienced two depressive episodes as a young adult. The second author has severe functional and speech impairments due to cerebral palsy, and the third author is the parent of a disabled child. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

We appreciate the insightful comments and suggestions provided by Karen Mogendorff and Lotte Siebesma.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Quayson, A. Aesthetic Nervousness: Disability and the Crisis of Representation; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  2. Scholz, M. Presse und Behinderung—Eine Qualitative und Quantitative Untersuchung; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Pelleboer-Gunnink, H.A.; Van Oorsouw, W.M.W.J.; Van Weeghel, J.; Embregts, P.J.C.M. Mainstream health professionals’ stigmatising attitudes towards people with intellectual disabilities: A systematic review. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2017, 61, 411–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Wang, Z.; Xu, X.; Han, Q.; Chen, Y.; Jiang, J.; Ni, G.-X. Factors associated with public attitudes towards persons with disabilities: A systematic review. BMC Public Health 2021, 21, 1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. United Nations. Convention on the Rights of People with Disabilities. 2006. Available online: https://www.un.org/development/desa/disabilities/convention-on-the-rights-of-persons-with-disabilities.html (accessed on 17 April 2024).
  6. Webb, J. Understanding Representation; SAGE Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Shakespeare, T. Cultural representation of disabled people: Dustbins for disavowal? Disabil. Soc. 1994, 9, 283–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Goffman, E. Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity; Simon & Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 1963. [Google Scholar]
  9. Link, B.G.; Phelan, J.C. Conceptualizing Stigma. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2001, 27, 363–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Titchkosky, T. Disability Studies: The Old and the New. Can. J. Sociol./Cah. Can. Sociol. 2000, 25, 197–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ali, A.; King, M.; Strydom, A.; Hassiotis, A. Self-reported stigma and symptoms of anxiety and depression in people with intellectual disabilities: Findings from a cross sectional study in England. J. Affect. Disord. 2015, 187, 224–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Boyle, M.P. Enacted stigma and felt stigma experienced by adults who stutter. J. Commun. Disord. 2018, 73, 50–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Mbuba, C.K.; Abubakar, A.; Odermatt, P.; Newton, C.R.; Carter, J.A. Development and validation of the Kilifi Stigma Scale for Epilepsy in Kenya. Epilepsy Behav. 2012, 24, 81–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Devlieger, P.J. Generating a Cultural Model of Disability. In Proceedings of the 19th Congress of the European Federation of Associations of Teachers of the Deaf (FEAPDA), Geneva, Switzerland, 14–16 October 2005; Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237762101_Generating_a_cultural_model_of_disability (accessed on 27 December 2024).
  15. Retief, M.; Letšosa, R. Models of disability: A brief overview. HTS Teol. Stud./Theol. Stud. 2018, 74, a4738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Barnes, C. Understanding the social model of disability: Past, present and future. In The Routledge Handbook of Disability Studies, 2nd ed.; Watson, N., Roulstone, A., Thomas, C., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2019; pp. 14–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Oliver, M. The Politics of Disablement; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  18. Kassah, A.K.; Kassah, B.L.L.; Agbota, T.K. Abuse of disabled children in Ghana. Disabil. Soc. 2012, 27, 689–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Zuurmond, M.; Seeley, J.; Nyant, G.G.; Baltussen, M.; Abanga, J.; Polack, S.; Bernays, S.; Shakespeare, T. Exploring caregiver experiences of stigma in Ghana: They insult me because of my child. Disabil. Soc. 2020, 37, 827–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Stainton, T. Reason, Value and Persons: The Construction of Intellectual Disability in Western Thought from Antiquity to the Romantic Age. In The Routledge History of Disability; Hanes, R., Brown, I., Hansen, N.E., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2017; pp. 11–34. [Google Scholar]
  21. Denham, A.R.; Adongo, P.B.; Freydberg, N.; Hodgson, A. Chasing spirits: Clarifying the spirit child phenomenon and infanticide in Northern Ghana. Soc. Sci. Med. 2010, 71, 608–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Tekola, B.; Kinfe, M.; Girma, F.; Hanlon, C.; Hoekstra, R.A. Perceptions and experiences of stigma among parents of children with developmental disorders in Ethiopia: A qualitative study. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020, 256, 113034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Erkilic, M. Developments in Disability Issues During the Late Ottoman Period of Turkish History from 1876 to 1909. In The Routledge History of Disability; Hanes, R., Brown, I., Hansen, N.E., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2017; pp. 56–69. [Google Scholar]
  24. World Health Organization. World Report on Disability; World Health Organization (WHO): Geneva, Switzerland, 2011.
  25. Shakespeare, T. The Social Model of Disability. In The Disability Studies Reader, 2nd ed.; Davis, L.J., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 2006; pp. 197–204. [Google Scholar]
  26. World Health Organization. International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health; World Health Organization (WHO): Geneva, Switzerland, 2001.
  27. Degener, T. Disability in a Human Rights Context. Laws 2016, 5, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Swain, J.; French, S. Towards an Affirmation Model of Disability. Disabil. Soc. 2000, 15, 569–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Cumberbatch, G.; Negrine, R. Images of Disability on Television; Routledge: London, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Longmore, P.K. Screening Stereotypes: Images of Disabled People in Television and Motion Pictures. In Images of the Disabled, Disabling Images; Gartner, A., Joe, T., Eds.; Praeger: Westport, CT, USA, 1987; pp. 65–78. [Google Scholar]
  31. Barnes, C. Disabling Imagery and the Media: An Exploration of the Principles for Media Representations of Disabled People; Ryburn Publishing, Ltd.: London, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  32. Clogston, J.S. Disability Coverage in American Newspapers. In The Disabled, the Media and the Information Age; Nelson, J.A., Ed.; Greenwood Press: Westport, CT, USA, 1994; pp. 45–57. [Google Scholar]
  33. Haller, B. Rethinking Models of Media Representation of Disability. Disabil. Stud. Q. 1995, 15, 26–30. Available online: http://hdl.handle.net/1811/86488 (accessed on 5 May 2023).
  34. Haller, B. Crawling Toward Civil Rights: News Media Coverage of Disability Activism. In Cultural Diversity and the US Media; Kamalipour, Y.R., Carilli, T., Eds.; State University of New York Press: Albany, NY, USA, 1998; pp. 89–98. [Google Scholar]
  35. Burns, S.; Haller, B. The politics of representing disability: Exploring news coverage of the Americans with Disabilities Act and the National Disability Insurance Scheme. Asia Pac. Media Educ. 2015, 25, 262–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Gold, N.; Auslander, G.K. Newspaper Coverage of People with Disabilities in Canada and Israel: An international comparison. Disabil. Soc. 1999, 14, 709–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kama, A. Supercrips versus the pitiful handicapped: Reception of disabling images by disabled audience members. Communications 2004, 29, 447–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Zhang, L.; Haller, B. Consuming Image: How Mass Media Impact the Identity of People with Disabilities. Commun. Q. 2013, 61, 319–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Tricco, A.C.; Lillie, E.; Zarin, W.; O’Brien, K.K.; Colquhoun, H.; Levac, D.; Moher, D.; Peters, M.D.J.; Horsley, T.; Weeks, L.; et al. PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and Explanation. Ann. Intern. Med. 2018, 169, 467–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Munn, Z.; Peters, M.D.J.; Stern, C.; Tufanaru, C.; McArthur, A.; Aromataris, E. Systematic Review or Scoping Review? Guidance for Authors When Choosing between a Systematic or Scoping Review Approach. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2018, 18, 143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Arksey, H.; O’Malley, L. Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 2005, 8, 19–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Levac, D.; Colquhoun, H.; O’Brien, K.K. Scoping studies: Advancing the methodology. Implement. Sci. 2010, 5, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Peters, M.D.J.; Godfrey, C.; McInerney, P.; Munn, Z.; Tricco, A.C.; Khalil, H. Scoping Reviews. In JBI Manual for Evidence Synthesis; Aromataris, E., Munn, Z., Eds.; JBI: Miami, FL, USA, 2020; pp. 406–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Gusenbauer, M.; Haddaway, N.R. Which academic search systems are suitable for systematic reviews or meta-analyses? Evaluating retrieval qualities of Google Scholar, PubMed, and 26 other resources. Res. Synth. Methods 2020, 11, 181–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. United Nations. Universal Declaration of Human Rights. 2006. Available online: https://www.un.org/en/about-us/universal-declaration-of-human-rights (accessed on 13 March 2025).
  46. World Bank. World Bank Country and Lending Groups. 2024. Available online: https://datahelpdesk.worldbank.org/knowledgebase/articles/906519-world-bank-country-and-lending-groups (accessed on 26 November 2024).
  47. Braun, V.; Clarke, V. One size fits all? What counts as quality practice in (reflexive) thematic analysis? Qual. Res. Psychol. 2020, 18, 328–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Terry, G.; Hayfield, N.; Clarke, V.; Braun, V. Thematic Analysis. In The SAGE Handbook of Qualitative Research in Psychology; Willig, C., Stainton-Rogers, W., Eds.; SAGE: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2017; pp. 17–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Admi, H.; Shaham, B. Living with Epilepsy: Ordinary People Coping with Extraordinary Situations. Qual. Health Res. 2007, 19, 1178–1187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Andregård, E.; Magnusson, L. Experiences of attitudes in Sierra Leone from the perspective of people with poliomyelitis and amputations using orthotics and prosthetics. Disabil. Rehabil. 2017, 39, 2619–2625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Arias-Urueña, L.; Chandler, A.; Harden, J. Cleft Lip and/or Palate: Children’s Experiences of Stigma in Colombia. Cleft Palate-Craniofacial J. 2024, 61, 1713–1720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Babamohamadi, H.; Negarandeh, R.; Dehghan-Nayeri, N. Barriers to and facilitators of coping with spinal cord injury for Iranian patients: A qualitative study. Nurs. Health Sci. 2011, 13, 207–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Bagatell, N.; Chan, D.; Rauch, K.K.; Thorpe, D. “Thrust into adulthood”: Transition experiences of young adults with cerebral palsy. Disabil. Health J. 2017, 10, 80–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Barbareschi, G.; Carew, M.T.; Johnson, E.A.; Kopi, N.; Holloway, C. “When They See a Wheelchair, They’ve Not Even Seen Me”—Factors Shaping the Experience of Disability Stigma and Discrimination in Kenya. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Botha, M.; Dibb, B.; Frost, D.M. “Autism is me”: An investigation of how autistic individuals make sense of autism and stigma. Disabil. Soc. 2022, 37, 427–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Buljevac, M.; Majdak, M.; Leutar, Z. The stigma of disability: Croatian experiences. Disabil. Rehabil. 2012, 34, 725–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Butler, R.; Bowlby, S. Bodies and Spaces: An Exploration of Disabled People’s Experiences of Public Space. Environ. Plan. D Soc. Space 1997, 15, 411–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Carew, M.T.; Dhingra, S.; Bash-Taqi, R.; Koroma, H.K.; Fraser, T.; McLaren, M.; Groce, N. “These attitudes are a pressure”: Women with disabilities’ perceptions of how stigma shapes their sexual health choices. Cult. Health Sex. 2024, 26, 362–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Coleman, H.; Peterson, C.L.; Walker, C. Understanding the experiences of stigma in relation to social support among an Australian community cohort of people living with epilepsy. Aust. J. Soc. Issues 2022, 57, 920–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Dako-Gyeke, M.; Donkor, M.D. Experiences and perspectives of stigmatization and discrimination against people with epilepsy in Accra, Ghana. Epilepsy Behav. 2018, 87, 101–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. de Klerk, H.M.; Ampousah, L. The physically disabled woman’s experience of self. Disabil. Rehabil. 2003, 25, 1132–1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Dean, L.; Medina, A.M. Stigma and the Hispanic stuttering experience: A qualitative study. J. Commun. Disord. 2021, 89, 106056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Goodwin, D.L.; Thurmeier, R.; Gustafson, P. Reactions to the Metaphors of Disability: The Mediating Effects of Physical Activity. Adapt. Phys. Act. Q. 2004, 21, 379–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Hansen, S.; Wilton, R.D.; Newbold, K.B. ‘There is always this feeling of otherness’: Exploring the lived experiences of visually impaired immigrant women in Canada. Disabil. Soc. 2017, 32, 1121–1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hosseini, N.; Sharif, F.; Ahmadi, F.; Zare, M. Patients’ perception of epilepsy and threat to self-identity: A qualitative approach. Epilepsy Behav. 2013, 29, 228–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Kılınç, S.; Campbell, C. “It shouldn’t be something that’s evil, it should be talked about”: A phenomenological approach to epilepsy and stigma. Seizure 2009, 18, 665–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Lang, J.; Jeschke, S.; Herziger, B.; Müller, R.M.; Bertsche, T.; Neininger, M.P.; Bertsche, A. Prejudices against people with epilepsy as perceived by affected people and their families. Epilepsy Behav. 2022, 127, 108535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Lash, B.N.; Helme, D.W. Managing Hearing Loss Stigma: Experiences of and Responses to Stigmatizing Attitudes & Behaviors. South. Commun. J. 2020, 85, 302–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Löfvenmark, I.; Norrbrink, C.; Nilsson Wikmar, L.; Löfgren, M. ‘The moment I leave my home—There will be massive challenges’: Experiences of living with a spinal cord injury in Botswana. Disabil. Rehabil. 2016, 38, 1483–1492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Louw, H. ‘Half a man? Still a human’: Photo-stories to dismantle unsettling representations and re-define the self-identities of men living with paraplagia. Glob. Public Health 2022, 17, 2280–2295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Malli, M.A.; Forrester-Jones, R. Stigma and Adults with Tourette’s Syndrome: “Never Laugh at Other People’s Disabilities, Unless they have Tourette’s—Because How Can You Not?”. J. Dev. Phys. Disabil. 2022, 34, 871–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Nickbakht, M.; Ekberg, K.; Waite, M.; Scarinci, N.; Timmer, B.; Meyer, C.; Hickson, L. The experience of stigma related to hearing loss and hearing aids: Perspectives of adults with hearing loss, their families, and hearing care professionals. Int. J. Audiol. 2024, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Nochi, M. Struggling with the Labeled Self: People with Traumatic Brain Injuries in Social Settings. Qual. Health Res. 1998, 8, 675–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. O’Connell, N. “Opportunity Blocked”: Deaf People, Employment and the Sociology of Audism. Humanit. Soc. 2021, 46, 336–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Ocran, J. ‘I don’t command the equal respect as my peers’: Middle-class disability, stigma and social expectation in Ghana. Ghana J. Dev. Stud. 2022, 19, 208–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Phillips, M.J. Damaged goods: Oral narratives of the experience of disability in American culture. Soc. Sci. Med. 1990, 30, 849–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Reber, L.; Kreschmer, J.M.; James, T.G.; Junior, J.D.; DeShong, G.L.; Parker, S.; Meade, M.A. Ableism and Contours of the Attitudinal Environment as Identified by Adults with Long-Term Physical Disabilities: A Qualitative Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 7469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Schneider, J.W.; Conrad, P. In the Closet with Illness: Epilepsy, Stigma Potential and Information Control. Soc. Probl. 1980, 28, 32–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Shamrock, J.; Smith, N.; Gray, M.; Cameron, M.; Oprescu, F. People with disabilities working in the disability sector in Timor Leste: A study of ‘lived experience’ using PhotoVoice. Third World Q. 2016, 38, 117–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Treweek, C.; Wood, C.; Martin, J.; Freeth, M. Autistic people’s perspectives on stereotypes: An interpretative phenomenological analysis. Autism 2019, 23, 759–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Hollinger, C.S.; Jones, R.L. Community attitudes toward slow Learners and mental retardates: What’s in a name? Ment. Retard. 1970, 8, 19–23. [Google Scholar]
  82. Schroedel, J.G.; Schiff, W. Attitudes towards deafness among several deaf and hearing populations. Rehabil. Psychol. 1972, 19, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Hahn, H.D.; Belt, T.L. Disability Identity and Attitudes Toward Cure in a Sample of Disabled Activists. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2004, 45, 453–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Reeve, D. Towards a Psychology of Disability: The Emotional Effects of Living in a Disabling Society. In Disability and Psychology: Critical Introductions and Reflections; Goodley, D., Lawthom, R., Eds.; Palgrave: London, UK, 2006; pp. 94–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Barnes, C. Disability, Disability Studies and the Academy. In Disabling Barriers–Enabling Environments, 3rd ed.; Swain, J., French, S., Barnes, C., Thomas, C., Eds.; SAGE: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2014; pp. 17–23. [Google Scholar]
  86. Richards, M.; Lawthom, R.; Runswick-Cole, K. Community-based arts research for people with learning disabilities: Challenging misconceptions about learning disabilities. Disabil. Soc. 2018, 34, 204–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Schanke, A.-K.; Thorsen, K. A life-course perspective on stigma-handling: Resilience in persons of restricted growth narrated in life histories. Disabil. Rehabil. 2014, 36, 1464–1473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Milacic-Vidojevic, I.; Colic, M.; Toskovic, O.; Dragojevic, N. People with physical disability in Serbia: Relationship between internalized stigma, experienced and anticipated discrimination, and empowerment. Scand. J. Psychol. 2020, 61, 290–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Hammer, S.T.; Stutts, L.A. The impact of disability representation on disability stigma in a general population. Rehabil. Psychol. 2025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Benson, A.; O’toole, S.; Lambert, V.; Gallagher, P.; Shahwan, A.; Austin, J.K. The stigma experiences and perceptions of families living with epilepsy: Implications for epilepsy-related communication within and external to the family unit. Patient Educ. Couns. 2016, 99, 1473–1481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Shelton, S.S.; Waddell, T.F. Does ‘Inspiration Porn’ Inspire? How Disability and Challenge Impact Attitudinal Evaluations of Advertising. J. Curr. Issues Res. Advert. 2020, 42, 258–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flowchart of the study selection process.
Figure 1. Flowchart of the study selection process.
Disabilities 05 00038 g001
Figure 2. Experienced public disability representations.
Figure 2. Experienced public disability representations.
Disabilities 05 00038 g002
Table 2. Overview of revised major media disability typologies.
Table 2. Overview of revised major media disability typologies.
Barnes StereotypesClogston–Haller Models
Traditional Typologies
People with disabilities are viewed as dysfunctional, and their disability-related limitations are attributed to them as individuals.
1. Burden
People with disabilities are helpless and must be ‘cared’ for by non-disabled people.
1. Medical
People with disabilities have an illness or malfunction that results in a dependent and passive state.
2. Incapable of Participating Fully in Community Life
People with disabilities are dispensable and unproductive members of the community who should be segregated.
2. Social Pathology
People with disabilities are disadvantaged clients who rely on the government or society for economic support.
3. Business
People with disabilities and their accessibility are costly to both society and businesses.
3. Super Cripple
People with disabilities have ‘super’ qualities and, therefore, should be respected.
4. Supercrip
People with disabilities are superhuman or amazing, suggesting that the individual’s achievements are remarkable for someone who is incomplete.
4. Atmosphere or Curio
People with disabilities are dehumanised objects of curiosity.
5. Object of Ridicule
People with disabilities are hapless fools.
6. Object of Violence
People with disabilities are subject to violent abuse by non-disabled people.
7. Pitiable and Pathetic
People with disabilities are endearing and elicit feelings of sentimentality.
8. Sexually Abnormal
People with disabilities are sexually dead, and, therefore, their lives are not worth living.
9. Sinister and Evil
People with disabilities are violent, wicked, sinners, or villains.
10. Their Own Worst and Only Enemy
People with disabilities are self-pitiers who might overcome their difficulties if they stop feeling sorry for themselves, think positively, and rise to ‘the challenge’.
Contemporary Typologies
The most limiting aspect of a person’s disability is society’s inability to adapt its physical,
social, and occupational environments, as well as its attitudes.
11. Normal
People with disabilities are individuals who just happen to have impairments.
5. Cultural Pluralism
People with disabilities are multifaceted individuals whose disability is only one of many facets.
6. Minority/Civil Rights
People with disabilities are members of a minority group with political grievances.
7. Legal
People with disabilities are individuals with legal rights who may need to file a lawsuit to stop discrimination.
8. Consumer
People with disabilities are an untapped consumer group.
Note: adopted from Barnes [31], Clogston [32], and Haller [33,34]. In this study, we use the more neutral term ‘contemporary’ instead of Clogston–Haller’s ‘progressive’.
Table 3. The inclusion and exclusion criteria.
Table 3. The inclusion and exclusion criteria.
Inclusion Criteria
  • Population: People with disabilities.
  • Experience: Public disability representation (e.g., attitudes, metaphors, perceptions, prejudices, stereotypes, and stigmas).
  • Type of study: Original, qualitative research.
  • English-language article.
  • Peer-reviewed.
  • Published between January 1948 and July 2024.
Exclusion Criteria
  • Studies not analysing experienced public disability representations.
  • Studies not classifying experienced public disability representations.
Table 4. The characteristics of the studies.
Table 4. The characteristics of the studies.
Author (Year)CountryWorld Bank Country
Classification
Disability TypeFemale/MaleAge RangeEducation/SESBackgroundData
Instruments
Method of AnalysisPerspectiveCore Concepts
(Terms to
Denote Public Disability
Representation)
Admi and Shaham (2007) [49]IsraelHigh IncomeEpilepsy11/315–24Varying
Education and SES
Non-Directed and Directed
Interviews
Constant Comparison Method Perception, Stigma
Andregård and Magnusson (2016) [50]Sierra LeoneMiddle–Low IncomePhysical
Disability
3/918 and aboveVarying
Education and SES
Varying Ethnic Groups; Muslim (n = 4), Christian (n = 8)Semi-Structured InterviewQualitative Content Analysis Attitude, Belief
Arias-Urueña et al. (2024) [51]ColombiaMiddle–Low
Income
Cleft Lip a/o Palate7/56–12 (m = 9)Low–Middle Class Semi-Structured Interview, Field NotesThematic Analysis
Following Theoretical Frameworks
Label, Stereotype, Stigma
Babamohamadi et al. (2011) [52]IranMiddle–Low
Income
Spinal Cord Injury10/819–63
(m = 37)
MuslimSemi-Structured InterviewQualitative Content AnalysisCopingAttitude, Belief, Knowledge
Bagatell et al. (2017) [53]USAHigh IncomeCerebral Palsy9/419–33
(m = 26)
Varying
Education and SES
Caucasian (n = 8), Multiracial (n = 1)Focus GroupThematic Inductive AnalysisTransition to AdulthoodPerception, Prejudice, Stereotype
Barbareschi et al. (2021) [54]KenyaMiddle–Low
Income
Mixed (1)3/3 Structured
Interview
Thematic Inductive AnalysisAssistive TechnologyConception, Stereotype, Stigma
Botha et al. (2022) [55]United
Kingdom
High IncomeAutism9/9 + Non-Binary (n = 2)21–62
(m = 37)
Varying Backgrounds (2)InterviewThematic Inductive Analysis Meaning, Stereotype, Stigma, Understanding
Buljevac et al. (2012) [56]CroatiaHigh IncomeMixed (3)3/222–48Varying
Education and SES
Focus GroupQualitative Content Analysis Attitude, Label,
Perception, Prejudice, Stereotype, Stigma,
Understanding
Butler and Bowlby (1997) [57]United
Kingdom
High IncomeVisual
Disability
16/416 to over 80Varying
Education and SES
White (n = 20)Interview Public SpaceAttitude, Conception
Carew et al. (2024) [58]Sierra LeoneMiddle–Low
Income
Mixed (4)32/0 Semi-Structured InterviewThematic Analysis (Top–Down)Sexual HealthAttitude, Belief, Stigma
Coleman et al. (2022) [59]AustraliaHigh IncomeEpilepsy194/149 (5)2–86 (m = 43; sd = 18.84; missing (n = 5)Varying Education and SES Survey (Analysis of Free-Text Responses)Mixed Methods (Thematic Content Analysis)Social SupportStigma, Understanding
Dako-Geyeke and Donkor (2018) [60]GhanaMiddle–Low IncomeEpilepsy7/720–55High School or LowerChristian (n = 13), Muslim (n = 1)InterviewThematic Inductive Analysis Belief, Stigma, View
de Klerk and Ampousah (2003) [61]South AfricaMiddle–Low IncomePhysical Disability40/021–60Predominantly Middle-IncomeWhite (n = 28), Black (n = 12)Semi-Structured InterviewInterpretative Phenomenological AnalysisPersonal AppearancePerception
Dean and Medina (2021) [62]USAHigh IncomeStuttering4/3AdultsVarying Education and SESHispanic or LatinoSemi-Structured InterviewThematic Inductive Analysis Stigma
Goodwin et al. (2004) [63]USAHigh IncomePhysical Disability7/714–24 (m = 19)Varying Education and SESCaucasianSemi-Structured Interview, Artefacts, Field NotesThematic AnalysisPhysical ActivitiesMeaning, Metaphor, Perception, Stigma
Hansen et al. (2017) [64]CanadaHigh IncomeVisual Disability7/0Early twenties to over sixty Varying Regions of Origin (6)Semi-Structured InterviewThematic AnalysisImmigrationAttitude, Meaning, Understanding
Hosseini et al. (2013) [65]IranMiddle–Low IncomeEpilepsy Unstructured and Semi-Structured InterviewsQualitative Content Analysis Perception, Perspective, Stigma
Kılınç and Campbell (2009) [66]United KingdomHigh IncomeEpilepsy52 (undisclosed) Structured Interview (Mis)conception, Knowledge, Stigma
Lang et al. (2022) [67]GermanyHigh IncomeEpilepsy152/019–83 (median = 40) Online Survey (Analysis of Open Questions)Mixed Methods (Data Clustering) Attitude, Prejudice, Stigma
Lash and Helme (2020) [68]USAHigh IncomeHearing Disability12/1818–79 (m = 53; sd = 17.6)Student (n = 2), Other (n = 28)Caucasian/White (n = 28), Hispanic (n = 1), African American (n = 1)Interactive Qualitative InterviewThematic Inductive Analysis Attitude, Perception, Stigma
Löfvenmark et al. (2016) [69]BotswanaMiddle–Low IncomeSpinal Cord Injury5/816–60Varying Education and SES Semi-Structured Interview, Participant ObservationThematic Inductive Analysis Attitude, Perspective, Stigma
Louw (2022) [70]South AfricaMiddle–Low IncomeParaplegia0/1527–50Varying Education and SESColoured (n = 15)PhotovoiceThematic Inductive Analysis Perception, Representation
Malli and Forrester-Jones (2022) [71]United KingdomHigh IncomeTourette’s Syndrome6/1420–71 (m = 33.5) White British (n = 19)Survey, InterviewMixed Methods (Interview: Thematic Inductive Analysis) Stigma
Nickbakht et al. (2024) [72]AustraliaHigh IncomeHearing Disability7/1353–88 Semi-Structured Interview Hearing Loss and Hearing AidsPerception. Perspective, Stereotype, Stigma
Nochi (2008) [73]USAHigh IncomeTraumatic Brain InjuryInterview: 2/8; Informant: 8/5Interview: 24–54 (m = 37.9; sd = 11); Informant: 26–61 years Semi-Structured Interview, Email, Participant ObservationThematic Inductive AnalysisTerminologyLabel, Meaning
O’Connell (2021) [74]IrelandHigh IncomeHearing Disability3/535–57 (m = 49.6; sd = 6.7)Varying Education and SES Unstructured and Semi-Structured InterviewsThematic Inductive AnalysisTransition to EmploymentPrejudice, Stereotype, Stigma
Ocran (2022) [75]GhanaMiddle–Low IncomeMixed (7)5/11 Middle-Class In-Depth InterviewInterpretative Phenomenological Analysis Social Expectation, Stigma
Phillips (1990) [76]USAHigh IncomePhysical Disability20/1321 to early 60sVarying Education and SESCaucasian (n = 27), Racially Mixed (n = 2), Missing (n = 4) (8)Informal InterviewCultural Analysis Notion, Perception
Reber et al. (2022) [77]USAHigh IncomePhysical Disability24/2623–75 (m = 52)Varying Education and SESBlack (n = 31), White (n = 16), Hispanic or Biracial (n = 3)Focus Group, InterviewThematic Inductive AnalysisHealthy AgeingAttitude, Stigma
Schneider and Conrad (1980) [78]USAHigh IncomeEpilepsy’Roughly equally’14–54 Interview CopingPerception, Stigma
Shamrock et al. (2016) [79]Timor LesteMiddle–Low IncomePhysical Disability2/620–52 (m = 32)Varying Education and SES PhotovoiceThematic Inductive AnalysisEmployment in Disability SectorAttitude, Stigma
Treweek et al. (2019) [80]United KingdomHigh IncomeAutism5/820–63 (M = 37.3; SD = 15.0) Semi-Structured InterviewInterpretative Phenomenological Analysis Stereotype
1. Mixed: hearing disability (n = 2), physical disability (n = 2), and visual disability (n = 2). 2 Varying backgrounds: White British (n = 13); Black British (n = 1); White other from Europe, America, Israel, or New Zealand (n = 5); mixed-race South American (n = 1). 3 Mixed: visual disability (n = 3) and physical disability (n = 2). 4 Mixed: physical disability (n = 30) and visual disability (n = 2). 5 Respondents: persons with epilepsy (n = 247) and on behalf of a person with epilepsy (n = 52). 6 Varying regions of origin: Middle East, including South Asia (n = 4); Africa (n = 1); Europe/Caribbean (n = 1); Europe/South America (n = 1). 7 Mixed: hearing disability (n = 3), visual disability (n = 2), partial speech impairment (n = 1), physical disability (n = 8), and both physical and visual disability (n = 2). 8 Racially mixed; reared in a predominantly Caucasian environment.
Table 5. Experienced public disability representations classified by disability models.
Table 5. Experienced public disability representations classified by disability models.
Disability Models
ReligiousCharityMedicalSocial
Admi and Shaham (2007) [49] x
Andregård and Magnusson (2016) [50]x x
Arias-Urueña et al. (2024) [51] x
Babamohamadi et al. (2011) [52] x
Bagatell et al. (2017) [53] x
Barbareschi et al. (2021) [54]x x
Botha et al. (2022) [55] x
Buljevac et al. (2012) [56] xx
Butler and Bowlby (1997) [57] xx
Carew et al. (2024) [58]x x
Coleman et al. (2022) [59]x x
Dako-Geyeke and Donkor (2018) [60]x x
de Klerk and Ampousah (2003) [61] x
Dean and Medina (2021) [62]x x
Goodwin et al. (2004) [63] xx
Hansen et al. (2017) [64] x
Hosseini et al. (2013) [65]x x
Kılınç and Campbell (2009) [66] x
Lang et al. (2022) [67] xx
Lash and Helme (2020) [68] x
Löfvenmark et al. (2016) [69]x x
Louw (2022) [70] x
Malli and Forrester-Jones (2022) [71] xx
Nickbakht et al. (2024) [72] x
Nochi (2008) [73] xx
O’Connell (2021) [74] x
Ocran (2022) [75]x x
Phillips (1990) [76] xx
Reber et al. (2022) [77] x
Schneider and Conrad (1980) [78]x x
Shamrock et al. (2016) [79]x
Treweek et al. (2019) [80] x
Note: The symbol x denotes that representations in the study corresponded with a particular disability model.
Table 6. Experienced public disability representations classified by media disability representation typologies.
Table 6. Experienced public disability representations classified by media disability representation typologies.
Media Disability Typologies
TraditionalContemporary
MedicalSocial PathologySupercrip/Super CrippleBusinessObject of RidiculeObject of ViolenceAtmosphere or CurioBurdenIncapable of Participating Fully in Community LifePitiable and PatheticSexually AbnormalSinister and EvilTheir Own Worst and Only EnemyMinority/Civil RightsLegalCultural Pluralism/NormalConsumer
Admi and Shaham (2007) [49] x
Andregård and Magnusson (2016) [50] x xx x
Arias-Urueña et al. (2024) [51]x
Babamohamadi et al. (2011) [52] x
Bagatell et al. (2017) [53] x x x
Barbareschi et al. (2021) [54]x x x
Botha et al. (2022) [55] x
Buljevac et al. (2012) [56]xx xxxx
Butler and Bowlby (1997) [57]x xxx
Carew et al. (2024) [58] x xx
Coleman et al. (2022) [59] x x
Dako-Geyeke and Donkor (2018) [60]x x
de Klerk and Ampousah (2003) [61] x
Dean and Medina (2021) [62] x x x x
Goodwin et al. (2004) [63]x x x x
Hansen et al. (2017) [64]x xx x
Hosseini et al. (2013) [65]x xx x
Kılınç and Campbell (2009) [66] x
Lang et al. (2022) [67]x x x
Lash and Helme (2020) [68] x
Löfvenmark et al. (2016) [69] x x x
Louw (2022) [70]x xx x
Malli and Forrester-Jones (2022) [71] xxx x
Nickbakht et al. (2024) [72]x x
Nochi (2008) [73]x x
O’Connell (2021) [74]x x xx
Ocran (2022) [75] x xxx x
Phillips (1990) [76]x xxxx x
Reber et al. (2022) [77] x x x
Schneider and Conrad (1980) [78] x x
Shamrock et al. (2016) [79]xx xx x
Treweek et al. (2019) [80] x
Note: The symbol x denotes that representations in the study corresponded with a particular media typology.
Table 7. Thematic classification of experienced public disability representations.
Table 7. Thematic classification of experienced public disability representations.
Experienced Public Disability Representations
IgnoranceLimited CapabilitiesOtherness
Ignorance About the Disability (General)CausesCuresIllnessContagiousnessIgnorance About People with disabilities (General)Disability ‘Spreads’Main Distinguishing Feature/Distinguishing CharacteristicHomogeneous GroupAssuming the Most Severe DisabilityIncapable (General)DependentPhysically Less CapableNon-VerbalIntellectually Less CapableEducationally Less CapableOccupationally Less CapableUnable to TravelNo ‘Adult’ NeedsPitifulInspirationalImpostersPossessing a ‘Bad’ TraitDevalued Social IdentityExclusion (General)An Individual Human Being
Admi and Shaham (2007) [49]X X XX
Andregård and Magnusson (2016) [50]Xo o XX XXXX
Arias-Urueña et al. (2024) [51]X o XXX
Babamohamadi et al. (2011) [52]X X o X X X X
Bagatell et al. (2017) [53] X o XXo XX
Barbareschi et al. (2021) [54]Xoo oX o X X
Botha et al. (2022) [55]Xo o X o XX X
Buljevac et al. (2012) [56]X o X o Xo XX XX X X
Butler and Bowlby (1997) [57] Xooo Xo oo X XX X
Carew et al. (2024) [58]Xo Xo X X XXX
Coleman et al. (2022) [59]X X o X o X X
Dako-Geyeke and Donkor (2018) [60]Xo o X X X
de Klerk and Ampousah (2003) [61] X o X X
Dean and Medina (2021) [62]Xoo Xo oXX X XX
Goodwin et al. (2004) [63]X o X o Xo X X
Hansen et al. (2017) [64] Xo X XX
Hosseini et al. (2013) [65]Xo oo Xo o X XXXX
Kılınç and Campbell (2009) [66]X
Lang et al. (2022) [67]Xoo oXo oXo oXXoX XXX
Lash and Helme (2020) [68]X Xo X o o X X XX
Löfvenmark et al. (2016) [69]Xoo o X XXX
Louw (2022) [70] Xoo Xo X X XX
Malli and Forrester-Jones (2022) [71]Xo o X X
Nickbakht et al. (2024) [72] Xo o X X
Nochi (2008) [73] Xo o XX X
O’Connell (2021) [74] X X
Ocran (2022) [75] Xo o Xoo oXXoXX XX
Phillips (1990) [76]X o Xo Xo XX XX XX
Reber et al. (2022) [77] Xo o X XXXX
Schneider and Conrad (1980) [78]X XX X
Shamrock et al. (2016) [79]Xo Xo XoX X
Treweek et al. (2019) [80] X o X X
Note: The main subthemes are represented by X. Examples of minor subthemes, which constitute elements of the main sub-themes, are represented by o.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

ter Haar, A.; Hilberink, S.R.; Schippers, A. Lived Experiences of Public Disability Representations: A Scoping Review. Disabilities 2025, 5, 38. https://doi.org/10.3390/disabilities5020038

AMA Style

ter Haar A, Hilberink SR, Schippers A. Lived Experiences of Public Disability Representations: A Scoping Review. Disabilities. 2025; 5(2):38. https://doi.org/10.3390/disabilities5020038

Chicago/Turabian Style

ter Haar, Aartjan, Sander R. Hilberink, and Alice Schippers. 2025. "Lived Experiences of Public Disability Representations: A Scoping Review" Disabilities 5, no. 2: 38. https://doi.org/10.3390/disabilities5020038

APA Style

ter Haar, A., Hilberink, S. R., & Schippers, A. (2025). Lived Experiences of Public Disability Representations: A Scoping Review. Disabilities, 5(2), 38. https://doi.org/10.3390/disabilities5020038

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop