Next Article in Journal
Air Pollution and Incidence of Lung Cancer by Histological Type in Korean Adults: A Korean National Health Insurance Service Health Examinee Cohort Study
Previous Article in Journal
Adsorption Characteristics of Oxytetracycline by Different Fractions of the Organic Matter from Humus Soil: Insight from Internal Structure and Composition
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Malaria Infection and Early Childhood Development Mediated by Anemia in Rural Kenya

1
Bureau for Global Health, United States Agency for International Development (USAID), Washington, DC 20024, USA
2
School of Public Health, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
3
Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering, Tufts University, Medford, MA 02155, USA
4
Department of Nutrition, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
5
WorldFish, Pulau Pinang 11960, Malaysia
6
Division of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA
7
Waterborne Disease Prevention Branch, Division of Foodborne, Waterborne, and Environmental Diseases, United States Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, GA 30329, USA
8
Innovations for Poverty Action, Nairobi 00200, Kenya
9
Social Impact, Arlington, VA 22201, USA
10
Mathematica Policy Research, Washington, DC 20002, USA
11
Community Health Sciences, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(3), 902; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17030902
Submission received: 27 December 2019 / Revised: 24 January 2020 / Accepted: 24 January 2020 / Published: 2 February 2020
(This article belongs to the Section Global Health)

Abstract

:
Malaria is a leading cause of morbidity and mortality among children under five years of age, with most cases occurring in Sub-Saharan Africa. Children in this age group in Africa are at greatest risk worldwide for developmental deficits. There are research gaps in quantifying the risks of mild malaria cases, understanding the pathways linking malaria infection and poor child development, and evaluating the impact of malaria on the development of children under five years. We analyzed the association between malaria infection and gross motor, communication, and personal social development in 592 children age 24 months in rural, western Kenya as part of the WASH Benefits environmental enteric dysfunction sub-study. Eighteen percent of children had malaria, 20% were at risk for gross motor delay, 21% were at risk for communication delay, and 23% were at risk for personal social delay. Having a positive malaria test was associated with increased risk for gross motor, communication, and personal social delay while adjusting for child characteristics, household demographics, study cluster, and intervention treatment arm. Mediation analyses suggested that anemia was a significant mediator in the pathway between malaria infection and risk for gross motor, communication, and personal social development delays. The proportion of the total effect of malaria on the risk of developmental delay that is mediated by anemia across the subscales was small (ranging from 9% of the effect on gross motor development to 16% of the effect on communication development mediated by anemia). Overall, malaria may be associated with short-term developmental delays during a vulnerable period of early life. Therefore, preventative malaria measures and immediate treatment are imperative for children’s optimal development, particularly in light of projections of continued high malaria transmission in Kenya and Africa.

1. Introduction

Malaria is a leading cause of morbidity [1] and mortality [2] globally, particularly among children under five years of age [3]. It is an acute febrile illness with an incubation period of seven days or longer. In 2018, more than two-thirds of malaria deaths were among children under five years [4]. Moreover, children under five years have a higher risk of acquiring malaria and are more likely to experience severe adverse effects compared to children over five years [5,6]. The consequences of malaria infection in children include anemia, respiratory distress, and cerebral damage. Over 90% of all malaria cases and deaths occurred in Africa in 2017 [4]. The most common parasite in Africa and the East Africa region (P. falciparum) has severe cerebral symptoms associated with it [7]. Malaria is endemic in western Kenya, which has the highest prevalence of P. falciparum in the country [7].
Forty-three percent of children under five in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) are also at risk of not meeting their developmental potential, with children in Africa being at highest risk due to many factors, including extreme poverty [8]. There is growing evidence suggesting that severe malaria is a risk factor for poor child development [9]; however, there are research gaps in quantifying the risk of milder cases of malaria, as most studies assess severe malaria. There are also gaps in understanding the pathway between malaria infection and poor child development, particularly among children under five years. Better understanding of the role of malaria in children’s development is important because developmental delays during the first five years of life can have lifelong implications related to educational attainment, adult wage earning, and health [8]. These long-term adverse effects can be prevented by identifying all benefits of prevention to implement the most cost-effective approach.

1.1. Characteristics of the Association between Malaria and Early Childhood Development

Developmental deficits related to malaria are complex and varied. In a systematic review of the relation between P. falciparum infection and cognitive function, Kihara et al. reported that specific parts of the brain may not always be impacted more than others by malaria as the effects can vary and be diffuse [10]. Impairments are driven by severity of malaria, symptomology, and pre- and post-disease exposure [11]. Malaria infection has been found to be significantly associated with a variety of impairments in areas such as language [12,13], motor [14], memory [14,15,16], executive function [13], and attention [15,16,17] in prospective case-control studies of children in Sub-Saharan Africa. Results vary, however, largely due to heterogeneity in the timing of malaria onset compared with outcome assessments, sample sizes, and measurements of risk factors, malaria infection, and child development outcomes [10,11].
The path to cognitive impairment may result from many interacting biological and social risk factors [12]. Risk factors for cognitive impairment associated with malaria include fetal exposure to malaria, repeated malaria infections, malaria symptoms (seizures, coma duration, psychosis, impaired consciousness, hypoglycemia, high fever, inflammation, lethargy, and compromised play/learning time), antimalarial medication, malnutrition, and low socioeconomic status [5,18,19]. In endemic areas where children are prone to repeated infections and other risk factors, malaria can be considered a syndrome with enhanced longer term brain and behavior effects [11].
The functions that are impaired may depend on the stage of brain growth at which the infection is acquired and impairment may not manifest until a child is older [5]. Age at malaria infection is a critical factor influencing cognitive impairment [5,6] and the pathways connecting malaria and child development change with age [20,21,22,23]. Although most research is on school-age children, malaria attacks are more common and severe in younger children and cognitive effects might be worse in children under five years of age [6,9]. For instance, in cross-sectional studies in Zanzibar, Tanzania, malaria among children under two years was associated with lower motor activity, lower language scores, and later onset of walking controlling for socioeconomic status, length-for-age Z-scores, anemia status, and season [20,24,25]. In Uganda, two longitudinal studies found that 12 months after having malaria, children under five years had low cognitive ability, attention, memory [17], gross motor, fine motor, visual perception, and language scores [14]; however, these studies only include children who had one cerebral malaria episode.

1.2. Anemia Mechanism Linking Malaria and Early Childhood Development

Malaria may impact early childhood development through a pathway of anemia/low hemoglobin concentrations. It is well-established that malaria causes anemia, especially in children under five years in Sub-Saharan Africa [26,27,28,29]. Malaria leads to anemia via two pathways: through the rapid destruction of red blood cells and as a result of virus-induced systemic inflammation that triggers a release of the iron-regulatory hormone hepcidin from the liver [26]. Hepcidin effectively blocks the absorption of iron in the intestine and leads to sequestration of iron in macrophages [30]. This hepcidin-induced hypoferremia can lead to iron-deficiency anemia, if prolonged. Recent evidence also highlights the role of hepcidin-mediated effects of inflammation on brain iron metabolism with cognitive implications [31,32,33]. Children 6 to 24 months of age with iron-deficiency anemia are at risk for poorer cognitive, motor, social-emotional, and neurophysiologic development in the short- and long-term [34]. Therefore, iron-deficiency anemia may be an important mechanism in the association between malaria and child development.
Although there is evidence that iron-deficiency anemia can lead to poor child development, there is a research gap in assessing malaria, anemia, and child development. Higher iron concentrations can increase malaria risk among children [35]. However, iron supplementation can increase hemoglobin concentrations in young children and in the WASH (water, sanitation, and hygiene) Benefits trial in Kenya, lipid-based nutrient supplements (LNS) increased hemoglobin concentrations, reduced anemia, and increased iron status [36]. In a randomized controlled trial of malaria prevention among children under three years of age in Uganda, anemia mediated the negative association between malaria infection and cognitive performance in all treatment arms [37]. In Tanzania, hemoglobin concentrations played a significant role in the pathway between malaria and motor abilities among children as they started to walk [24]. More well-designed studies longitudinally evaluating the pathways (including anemia) between mild and severe malaria infection, and a variety of child development outcomes among children under five years are needed to elucidate causal mechanisms and account for seasonal and age-dependent nuances.

1.3. Malaria and Child Health in Kenya

Malaria is widespread throughout Kenya and a leading cause of morbidity and mortality in the country [38]. About 74% of the Kenyan population is at risk for malaria and malaria accounts for 30% to 50% of all outpatient clinic or hospital visits [38]. In western Kenya, malaria is endemic and transmission is perennial with seasonal peaks in May to July and October to November [39]. Malaria is estimated to cause 20% of all deaths in children under age five years in the country [38] and the disease is common in this vulnerable population of young children [40]. Malaria and anemia are concurrently high in many parts of Kenya [41]. In 2016, the prevalence of anemia among children under five years in Kenya was 41% [42]. The prevalence of other indicators of poor child health related to nutrition and child development in Kenya is high. Information about early childhood development in Kenya is limited however, and most of the program and policy efforts in the country focus on preschool-age children despite the importance of development in younger years [43].

1.4. Objectives

This study aimed to describe the association between malaria infection and early childhood development. The objectives of the research were to: 1) investigate associations between malaria infection and three child development outcomes, and 2) assess the role of anemia as a potential mediator in the pathway between malaria infection and child development outcomes. The following two connected hypotheses were tested: 1) malaria infection is associated with increased odds of being at risk for gross motor, communication, and personal social developmental delays, and 2) anemia is a significant mediator along the pathway between malaria infection and being at risk for gross motor, communication, and personal social developmental delays.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Setting and Population

These data were collected during the WASH Benefits trial, which is a cluster-randomized controlled trial assessing the impact of WASH and nutrition interventions individually and combined on child health outcomes [44,45,46]. The study was conducted in Bungoma and Kakamega counties in western Kenya. The region is rural and the majority of the population are subsistence farmers. The WASH Benefits trial took place from November 2012 to July 2016. Households were selected to participate in the trial if they were in a rural community (defined as <25% of residents living in a rented home (most families in rural areas own homes compared with urban households in Kenya), with <2 gas stations, and <10 shops in the area) and had limited access to water and sanitation (defined as communities where >80% of residents did not have access to piped water). Female participants were eligible for enrollment into the study if they were pregnant, if they or their partner owned their house, and if they were not planning to move within the next 12 months. The infant that was in utero during the village census was the index child.

2.2. Study Design

Households were visited at enrollment and two follow-up periods, one and two years later. A sub-study assessing environmental enteric dysfunction, malaria infection, and hemoglobin concentrations from biological samples enrolled 2,304 households from four arms of the WASH Benefits trial (WASH, WASH and Nutrition, Nutrition, or Active Control) [36,44,47]. The nutrition intervention entailed nutrition education and all children between 6–24 months in the participating household receiving LNS with iron. In the second follow-up, 1,449 children were included in the sub-study [36,44,47]. This sample size was calculated based on having the power to detect differences in environmental enteropathy biomarkers [48] and this sample size was sufficient to determine the association between malaria infection and child development outcomes. An equal proportion of children were selected from each intervention arm.
Caregivers brought study children to a central location for sub-study data collection between August 2015 and April 2016. Blood samples for malaria and anemia diagnoses were taken by trained phlebotomists and health surveys were completed. Caregivers provided written informed consent. Consent for blood collection and a sufficient blood sample was obtained for 699 children and therefore malaria and hemoglobin measurements only existed for this subset of children. An average of two to three months later, caregivers brought study children to a central location for main trial data collection, including a child development assessment. Local enumerators conducted most surveys in Kiswahili, the regional language. Surveys and data collection tools were developed from validated measures [49] and locally adapted.

2.3. Malaria and Anemia Data Collection

Venous blood samples were drawn from sub-study participant children in a central location within villages for biomarker analyses and drops of blood were used to assess malaria parasites and hemoglobin concentrations. Rapid diagnostic tests (RDTs) for P. falciparum, P. vivax, P. ovale, and P. malariae were conducted using rapid diagnostic kits (SD Bioline, Malaria Ag, P.f, P.f, P.v, Alere). The kits can detect parasite concentrations up to 28 days after infection. All children with a positive test result were defined as having malaria in these analyses. Axillary temperatures were taken for all children and if the malaria parasite test was positive and the child’s temperature was 37.5 degrees Celsius or higher, he/she was referred to the nearest clinic for malaria treatment. Hemoglobin concentrations were measured using a Hemocue (Hb 301) machine (Angelholm, Sweden). Anemia was defined as having a hemoglobin concentration below 11.0 g/dL [50]. A medical history form was also completed with caregivers about the child’s recent experiences with illness, prophylaxis, and treatment related to a variety of health conditions, including malaria.

2.4. Socio-Demographics

Household demographics, including asset ownership and maternal education, were assessed using standardized questionnaires administered at enrollment. Socioeconomic status was defined with an asset index using enrollment survey results for all sub-study participants. The asset index was created using the first principal component from a principal components analysis, which has been shown to be an appropriate proxy for household wealth and is correlated with consumption expenditures [51,52,53]. Items included in the asset index were housing structure, electricity, and ownership of a radio, television, mobile phone, clock, bicycle, motorcycle, gas stove, and car. Maternal education was categorized into the following categories: incomplete primary school, completed primary school, and any secondary school.

2.5. Early Childhood Development

Three domains of child development were assessed: gross motor, communication, and personal social. The corresponding subscales of the Extended Ages and Stages Questionnaire (EASQ) were used to measure each developmental outcome among all WASH Benefits trial participants during the last data collection round (at approximately age 24 months). The EASQ is a modified version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ), which is widely used for the developmental screening of children under five years of age [54,55,56] and has been used in LMICs [57]. The questionnaires contain a series of age-specific items assessing the achievement of developmental milestones and tracking the child’s progress. The EASQ was piloted to assess respondent bias, maternal accuracy in reporting children’s abilities, cultural appropriateness, and feasibility according to standard procedures [49]. The Gross Motor subscale evaluates body and muscle movement, including tasks like standing, walking, and balancing. The Communication subscale assesses language development and the use of words or sounds to express feelings. The Personal Social subscale reflects emotional responses and social interactions. The EASQ was translated and adapted for this study by using local culturally appropriate items, examples, and tasks, but no substantial changes were made to the original questionnaires. Continuous scores for each subscale were calculated and converted to age-adjusted Z-scores based on the control group within the larger WASH Benefits trial. Children were considered at risk for delay in this population if their Z-scores were below the 25th percentile of control group Z-scores for each domain. This method of defining risk for delay was used as a conservative strategy because there was no prior research informing a culturally appropriate clinical cut-off in this study population. Binary variables were used to incorporate a quantitative definition of poor development and to be consistent with analyses used in the main WASH Benefits publications [46,58].

2.6. Statistical Analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted in Stata 14 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA, 2015). Three sets of logistic multivariate analyses were undertaken to test the hypotheses corresponding to each child development outcome (gross motor, communication, and personal social). Two regression models were built for each child development outcome to assess the pathways between malaria infection (binary) and being at risk for gross motor, communication, and personal social delays (binary). The first model included malaria infection (binary) and the child development outcomes (three separate binary outcomes). Anemia status (binary; anemia defined as hemoglobin concentration <11.0 g/dL) was added to the second model. The interaction between malaria status and participation in the nutrition intervention was also analyzed in multivariate models to assess whether being in a nutrition treatment arm differentially affected malaria infection. All models were adjusted for child characteristics (sex [binary] and age [continuous, months]), household demographics (asset index [continuous] and maternal education [categorical]), study cluster, and whether the household received the nutrition intervention (binary). Analyses controlled for arms that received nutrition interventions because they were likely confounders.
Mediation analyses were then undertaken to assess whether anemia was a mediator along the pathway between malaria infection and being at risk for gross motor, communication, and personal social delays. The binary_mediation method in Stata [59,60,61] was employed. Like the Sobel–Goodman test for continuous outcomes, the binary_mediation method shows that mediation occurred if: 1) malaria infection was significantly associated with the mediator (anemia), 2) malaria infection was significantly associated with developmental delay in the absence of the mediator (anemia), 3) the mediator (anemia) was significantly associated with developmental delay, and 4) the effect of malaria infection on developmental delay decreased upon the addition of the mediator (anemia) to the model. The proportion of the total effect of malaria infection on the risk for gross motor, communication, and personal social delay that is mediated by anemia was also calculated and reported.
Children with missing malaria data or child development assessments were removed from analyses. Nine percent of children had missing data from one of these measurements and thus were not included in the final analyses. The final sample size was therefore 592 children. Differences between those who provided consent for blood collection and had a sufficient blood sample, and those who did not were assessed due to the low response rate and no significant differences or systematic attrition were found [36].

2.7. Ethics

The WASH Benefits trial is registered as a clinical trial with the U.S. National Institutes of Health (https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01704105). Ethical approval was obtained from the University of California, Berkeley Center for the Protection of Human Subjects and the Kenya Medical Research Institute Ethical Review Committee. Adult participants provided written consent for themselves and their children prior to enrollment.

3. Results

3.1. Socio-Demographics and Child Health

Maternal education in this population was low: 53% of mothers did not complete primary school (Table 1). The mean age of the children was 24 months, with 29% of children stunted and 7% underweight. The prevalence of anemia among children included in these analyses was 31%, which is similar to the prevalence of anemia among children in the Nutrition intervention arm (36%) and WASH + Nutrition intervention arm (27%), and lower than the prevalence of anemia in the control group (49%) [36]. Eighteen percent of children had a positive malaria blood test suggesting that they were infected with a malaria parasite in the previous month. Of these, the majority (81%) were infected with P. falciparum. The percent of children that were at risk for gross motor (20%), communication (21%), and personal social (23%) delays in this population (had Z-scores in the lowest quartile based on control group domain Z-scores) were similar across domains.

3.2. Multivariate Associations

Having a positive malaria test was significantly associated with increased odds of a child being at risk for gross motor (OR 2.22; 95% CI 1.35, 3.64), communication (OR 1.92; 95% CI 1.19, 3.09), and personal social (OR 2.86; 95% CI 1.81, 4.55) delays (i.e., having domain Z-scores in the lowest quartile) adjusting for child characteristics, household demographics, study cluster, and participation in the nutrition treatment arm (Model 1, Table 2). The odds of a child being at risk for developmental delays (i.e., having domain Z-scores in the lowest quartile) across all subscales decreased with the addition of anemia (Model 2, Table 2) to models, but remained significant. The strength and significance of the associations were greater for personal social delays across Models 1 and 2. Anemia was only significant in personal social analyses. Interactions between malaria and participation in the Nutrition intervention arm were not significant as the treatment arm did not play a role in this association.

3.3. Mediation

Anemia was a significant mediator in the pathway between malaria infection and risk of gross motor, communication, and personal social development delays (i.e., having domain Z-scores in the lowest quartile) (Table 3). Across all subscales, malaria was significantly associated with being anemic and risk of developmental delay, anemia was significantly associated with risk of developmental delay, and the effect of malaria on risk of developmental delay decreased upon the addition of anemia to the models. The proportion of the total effect of malaria on risk of developmental delay (i.e., having domain Z-scores in the lowest quartile) that is mediated by anemia in this population was greatest with the communication subscale (16%), however, all proportions were small (Table 4).

4. Discussion

A positive malaria blood test was associated with increased odds of being at risk of gross motor, communication, and personal social delays (i.e., having domain Z-scores in the lowest quartile) while controlling for a range of individual- and household-level variables. The odds of personal social delays were greater than the odds of delays in the other two domains. Across subscales, the addition of the potential mediator (anemia) to models led to smaller odds of delays. Formal mediation analyses demonstrated that anemia was a mediator in the pathway between malaria infection and risk of developmental delay in all three domains. However, the proportion of the total association mediated by anemia was small.
The prevalence of malaria (18%) in this population was lower than expected given that the prevalence of malaria in the region is 42% using RDTs [62]. This could have been at least partially due to seasonality. Although studies show mixed results with varied effects based on severity of malaria and developmental domain [10,11], these findings corroborate research that demonstrates an association between malaria infection and child development, particularly among children under five years and who are not exclusively experiencing severe malaria. While few studies have undertaken formal mediation analyses, the results are comparable to two studies from Zanzibar, Tanzania, assessing malaria, hemoglobin concentrations, and motor development among children under two years [20,24]. Children with higher malaria parasite densities in Zanzibar had significantly lower hemoglobin concentrations [20]. Malaria was directly and indirectly associated with motor abilities after controlling for anemia status in these two assessments in Zanzibar [24].
This study has several strengths. The use of blood tests to indicate the presence of malaria parasites provides a robust measure of infection. Including all cases of malaria rather than only severe or cerebral incidents is beneficial for understanding associations between mild forms of malaria that are more widespread and child development. There may be a lag between infection and the manifestation of measurable effects on development, and the measurements in this study were longitudinal; however, the amount of time between the malaria blood test and the child development assessments varied. Incorporating multiple domains of child development yields a more comprehensive understanding of the implications of malaria infection. The use of a control group to calculate standardized child development scores enabled analyses to accurately assess whether a child is at risk of delay in this population. Building models by systematically adding a hypothesized mediator to analyses allowed for a thorough assessment of the potential pathways of impact. Further mediation analyses strengthened findings.
There are limitations to these analyses as the pathways between malaria and child development are complex. While there were no systematic differences found between those who provided consent for blood collection and those who did not, the low response rate could have led to selection bias. Resistance to venous blood collection was the primary reason for the low response rate, which was likely due to the high burden placed on respondents related to the collection of other biological and environmental samples, long questionnaires, and the child development assessment. The relatively small sample size could limit the generalizability and precision of the findings. Moreover, the rapid diagnostic tests detect parasites up to approximately one month after infection and do not quantify parasite load. Thus, it is difficult to determine when a child was infected and the severity of malaria, and therefore the extent to which infection could affect child development outcomes. Only one malaria test was performed and repeated infections, which may be common in this endemic area, could have larger effects on child development. Although there was time between the malaria diagnosis and child development assessment, there was only one measurement of each and therefore causal implications cannot be drawn. Further, children in the sample may have had malaria multiple times before the malaria test and child development assessment since they live in an endemic area. This could have made it more challenging to detect effects, thereby making these findings conservative. The single malaria test result is not indicative of general exposure to malaria, hence hindering interpretation of the association with the child development outcomes. Despite there being a lag between malaria and child development measurements, malaria and anemia measurements were taken at the same time, further limiting the conclusions about the pathways of impact. Finally, the mediation analysis did not yield direct and indirect effects of anemia in the complex pathway between malaria infection and the child development outcomes, simplifying the implications of the mediation findings.
Overall, this study’s findings highlight how malaria may be associated with child development outcomes. The focus on children under five years adds to the literature as exposure to risk factors like malaria during this vulnerable time period is critical for development, yet understudied. Significant associations between malaria and gross motor, communication, and personal social domains in this young population emphasize the importance of preventing and treating malaria early in life. Patterns of malaria are likely to shift throughout Africa with climate change as certain geographies may be more impacted. Climate change coupled with human population density projections for the continent suggest that malaria transmission will remain high in the coming years [63,64], additionally highlighting the need to mitigate malaria infections. Finally, these results further inform the pathway of impact with the inclusion of robust mediation analyses demonstrating the role anemia may play. The proportion of the total effect of anemia in the pathway between malaria infection and the child development outcomes was small, suggesting that other possible mechanisms such as the symptoms of malaria infection (seizures, coma duration, psychosis, impaired consciousness, hypoglycemia, high fever, inflammation, lethargy, and compromised play/learning time), repeated infection, antimalarial medication, malnutrition, and low socioeconomic status may play a substantial role in this pathway [5,11,12,18,19]. Understanding anemia and other mechanisms in the pathway can provide further impetus for addressing malaria and inform its impacts on child development.
More longitudinal studies with repeated measures of malaria and child development outcomes can inform causal relations in addition to the timing of effects. The assessment of additional domains of development (e.g., cognitive) and long-term versus short-term effects can provide a comprehensive understanding of the impacts of malaria. The role of less severe and repeated cases of malaria can also support the targeting of interventions to the largest number of children who are most susceptible to risks for developmental delay. Longitudinal research assessing other mediators is important for better understanding the nuances of mechanisms of impact and when programs can be most effective.

5. Conclusions

This study provides evidence of how malaria infection may be associated with short-term child development outcomes during a critical developmental period. These findings suggest that anemia, in this case caused by infection rather than lack of iron in the diet, may play a significant role in the pathway. The high prevalence of malaria and large percentage of children at risk for developmental delays in LMICs motivates the need for policies and programs to address the complexities of malaria infection. Preventative malaria measures and immediate treatment are imperative, particularly in light of projections of continued high malaria transmission in Africa. Overall, combating malaria may enable children to reach their full developmental potential, which can have lifelong and intergenerational benefits.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed substantially to the work reported. The following contributions were made: conceptualization, E.M.M.; P.K.; A.J.P.; C.P.S.; A.L.; H.N.D.; C.N. and L.C.H.F.; methodology, E.M.M.; P.K.; A.J.P.; C.P.S.; K.B.; A.L.; G.R.; B.A.; C.N. and L.C.H.F.; analysis, E.M.M.; writing—original draft preparation, E.M.M. and L.C.H.F.; writing—review and editing, P.K.; A.J.P.; C.P.S.; K.B.; A.L.; G.R.; B.A.; H.N.D. and C.N.; project administration, E.M.M.; P.K.; A.J.P.; C.P.S.; A.L.; G.R.; B.A. and L.C.H.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation (grant OPPGD759), Thrasher Research Fund (award 11860), and the United States Agency for International Development (USAID.; grant AID-OAA-F-13–00040).

Acknowledgments

We offer our sincere gratitude to the study participants. We are also grateful for the community health promotors, field workers, supervisors, and other Innovations for Poverty Action implementing staff.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Murray, C.J.L.; Vos, T.; Lozano, R.; Naghavi, M.; Flaxman, A.D.; Michaud, C.; Ezzati, M.; Shibuya, K.; Salomon, J.A.; Abdalla, S.; et al. Disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 291 diseases and injuries in 21 regions, 1990–2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 2012, 380, 2197–2223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Lozano, R.; Naghavi, M.; Foreman, K.; Lim, S.; Shibuya, K.; Aboyans, V.; Abraham, J.; Adair, T.; Aggarwal, R.; Ahn, S.Y.; et al. Global and regional mortality from 235 causes of death for 20 age groups in 1990 and 2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 2012, 380, 2095–2128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Wang, H.; Naghavi, M.; Allen, C.; Barber, R.M.; Bhutta, Z.A.; Carter, A.; Casey, D.C.; Charlson, F.J.; Chen, A.Z.; Coates, M.M.; et al. Global, regional, and national life expectancy, all-cause mortality, and cause-specific mortality for 249 causes of death, 1980–2015: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study. Lancet 2015, 388, 1459–1544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. WHO. Malaria Fact. Sheet; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  5. Holding, P.A.; Kitsao-Wekulo, P.K. Describing the burden of malaria on child development: What should we be measuring and how should we be measuring it? Amer. J. Trop. Med. Hyg. 2004, 71, 71–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Boivin, M.J.; Gladstone, M.J.; Vokhiwa, M.; Birbeck, G.L.; Magen, J.G.; Page, C.; Semrud-Clikeman, M.; Kauye, F.; Taylor, T.E. Developmental outcomes in Malawian children with retinopathy-positive cerebral malaria. Trop. Med. Int. Health 2011, 16, 263–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Kenya National Bureau of Statistics; Ministry of Health; National AIDS Control Council; Kenya Medical Research Institute; National Council for Population and Development; ICF International. Kenya Demographic and Health Survey 2014; Republic of Kenya Government: Nairobi, Kenya, 2015.
  8. Black, M.M.; Walker, S.P.; Fernald, L.C.H.; Andersen, C.T.; DiGirolamo, A.M.; Lu, C.; McCoy, D.C.; Fink, G.; Shawar, Y.R.; Shiffman, J.; et al. Early childhood development coming of age: Science through the life course. Lancet 2017, 389, 77–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Walker, S.P.; Wachs, T.D.; Grantham-McGregor, S.; Black, M.M.; Nelson, C.A.; Huffman, S.L.; Baker-Henningham, H.; Chang, S.M.; Hamadani, J.D.; Lozoff, B.; et al. Inequality in early childhood: Risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet 2011, 378, 1325–1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kihara, M.; Carter, J.A.; Newton, C.R.J.C. The effect of Plasmodium falciparum on cognition: A systematic review. Trop. Med. Int. Health 2006, 11, 386–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Holding, P.; Boivin, M.J. The assessment of neuropsychological outcomes in pediatric severe malaria. In Neuropsychology of Children in Africa; Boivin, M.J., Giordani, B., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  12. Carter, J.A.; Murira, G.M.; Ross, A.J.; Mung’ala-Odera, V.; Newton, C. Speech and language sequelae of severe malaria in Kenyan children. Brain Injury 2003, 17, 217–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Carter, J.A.; Ross, A.J.; Neville, B.G.R.; Obiero, E.; Katana, K.; Mung’ala-Odera, V.; Lees, J.A.; Newton, C.R. Developmental impairments following severe falciparum malaria in children. Trop. Med. Int. Health 2005, 10, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bangirana, P.; Opoka, R.O.; Boivin, M.J.; Idro, R.; Hodges, J.S.; John, C.C. Neurocognitive domains affected by cerebral malaria and severe malarial anemia in children. Learn. Ind. Differ. 2016, 46, 38–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Boivin, M.J.; Bangirana, P.; Byarugaba, J.; Opoka, R.O.; Idro, R.; Jurek, A.M.; John, C.C. Cognitive impairment after cerebral malaria in children: A prospective study. Pediatrics 2007, 119, e360–e366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. John, C.C.; Bangirana, P.; Byarugaba, J.; Opoka, R.O.; Idro, R.; Jurek, A.M.; Wu, B.; Boivin, M.J. Cerebral malaria in children is associated with long-term cognitive impairment. Pediatrics 2008, 122, e92–e99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Bangirana, P.; Opoka, R.O.; Boivin, M.J.; Idro, R.; Hodges, J.S.; Romero, R.A.; Shapiro, E.; John, C.C. Severe malarial anemia is associated with long-term neurocognitive impairment. Clin. Infect. Dis. 2014, 59, 336–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Anyanwu, E.C.; Kanu, I.; Merrick, J. Impact of malaria infectious pathways on disability and child development in endemic regions. Int. J. Dis. Hum. Dev. 2011, 6, 253–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. John, C.C.; Black, M.M.; Nelson, C.A., III. Neurodevelopment: The impact of nutrition and inflammation during early to middle childhood in low-resource settings. Pediatrics 2017, 139, S59–S71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Olney, D.K.; Kariger, P.K.; Stoltzfus, R.J.; Khalfan, S.S.; Ali, N.S.; Tielsch, J.M.; Sazawal, S.; Black, R.; Allen, L.H.; Pollitt, E. Development of nutritionally at-risk young children is predicted by malaria, anemia, and stunting in Pemba, Zanzibar. J. Nutrition. 2009, 139, 763–772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Fernando, S.D.; Rodrigo, C.; Rajapakse, S. The “hidden” burden of malaria: Cognitive impairment following infection. Malar. J. 2010, 9, 366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Fink, G.; Olgiati, A.; Hawela, M.; Miller, J.M.; Matafwali, B. Association between early childhood exposure to malaria and children’s pre-school development: Evidence from the Zambia early childhood development project. Malar. J. 2012, 12, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Thuilliez, J.; Sissoko, M.S.; Toure, O.B.; Kamate, P.; Berthélemy, J.-C.; Doumbo, O.K. Malaria and primary education in Mali: A longitudinal study in the village of Donéguébougou. Soc. Sci. Med. 2010, 71, 324–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Olney, D.K.; Pollitt, E.; Kariger, P.K.; Khalfan, S.S.; Ali, N.S.; Tielsch, J.M.; Sazawal, S.; Black, R.; Mast, D.; Allen, L.H.; et al. Young Zanzibari children with iron deficiency, iron deficiency anemia, stunting, or malaria have lower motor activity scores and spend less time in locomotion. J. Nutr. 2007, 137, 2756–2762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Olney, D.K.; Kariger, P.K.; Stoltzfus, R.J.; Khalfan, S.S.; Ali, N.S.; Tielsch, J.M.; Sazawal, S.; Black, R.; Allen, L.H.; Pollitt, E. Developmental effects of micronutrient supplementation and malaria in Zanzibari children. Early Hum. Dev. 2013, 89, 667–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Prentice, A.M.; Ghattas, H.; Doherty, C.; Cox, S.E. Iron metabolism and malaria. Food Nutr. Bull. 2007, 28, S524–S539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Ehrhardt, S.; Burchard, G.D.; Mantel, C.; Cramer, J.P.; Kaiser, S.; Kubo, M.; Otchwemah, R.N.; Bienzle, U.; Mockenhaupt, F.P. Malaria, anemia, and malnutrition in African children—Defining intervention priorities. J. Infect. Dis. 2006, 194, 108–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Verhoef, H.; West, C.E.; Veenemans, J.; Beguin, Y.; Kok, F.J. Stunting may determine the severity of malaria-associated anemia in African children. Pediatrics 2002, 110, e48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Hautvast, J.L.; Tolboom, J.J.; Kafwembe, E.M.; Musonda, R.M.; Mwanakasale, V.; van Staveren, W.A.; van’t Hof, M.A.; Sauerwein, R.W.; Willems, J.L.; Monnens, L.A. Severe linear growth retardation in rural Zambian children: The influence of biological variables. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2000, 71, 550–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Preza, G.C.; Pinon, R.; Ganz, T.; Nemeth, E. Cellular catabolism of the iron-regulatory peptide hormone hepcidin. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e58934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Vela, D. The dual role of hepcidin in brain iron load and inflammation. Front. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. McCarthy, R.C.; Sosa, J.C.; Gardeck, A.M.; Baez, A.S.; Lee, C.H.; Wessling-Resnick, M. Inflammation-induced iron transport and metabolism by brain microglia. J. Biol. Chem. 2018, 293, 7853–7863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Vela, D. Hepcidin, an emerging and important player in brain iron homeostasis. J. Trans. Med. 2018, 16, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Lozoff, B. Iron deficiency and child development. Food Nutr. Bull. 2007, 28, S560–S571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Prentice, A.M.; Verhoef, H.; Cerami, C. Iron fortification and malaria risk in children. JAMA 2013, 310, 914–915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Stewart, C.P.; Dewey, K.G.; Lin, A.; Pickering, A.J.; Byrd, K.A.; Jannat, K.; Ali, S.; Rao, G.; Dentz, H.N.; Kiprotich, M.; et al. Effects of lipid-based nutrient supplements and infant and young child feeding counseling with or without improved water, sanitation, and hygiene (WASH) on anemia and micronutrient status: Results from 2 cluster-randomized trials in Kenya and Bangladesh. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2019, 109, 148–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Boivin, M.J.; Sikorskii, A.; Familiar-Lopez, I.; Ruiseñor-Escudero, H.; Muhindo, M.; Kapisi, J.; Bigira, V.; Bass, J.K.; Opoka, R.O.; Nakasujja, N.; et al. Malaria illness mediated by anaemia lessens cognitive development in younger Ugandan children. Malar. J. 2016, 15, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Kenya Medical Research Institute. Kenya Malaria Fact Sheet; Republic of Kenya Government: Nairobi, Kenya, 2016.
  39. Artavanis-Tsakonas, K.; Tongren, J.E.; Riley, E.M. The war between the malaria parasite and the immune system: Immunity, immunoregulation and immunopathology. Clin. Exper. Immun. 2003, 133, 145–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Hamel, M.J.; Adazu, K.; Obor, D.; Sewe, M.; Vulule, J.; Williamson, J.M.; Slutsker, L.; Feikin, D.R.; Laserson, K.F. A reversal in reductions of child mortality in western Kenya, 2003–2009. Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg. 2011, 85, 597–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Foote, E.M.; Sullivan, K.M.; Ruth, L.J.; Oremo, J.; Sadumah, I.; Williams, T.N.; Suchdev, P.S. Determinants of Anemia among preschool children in rural, western Kenya. Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg. 2013, 88, 757–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. World Health Organization. Global Health Observatory Data Repository/World Health Statistics; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  43. Githinji, F.W.; Kanga, A. Early childhood development education in Kenya: A literature review on current issues. Int. J. Curr. Res. 2011, 3, 129–136. [Google Scholar]
  44. Arnold, B.F.; Null, C.; Luby, S.P.; Unicomb, L.; Stewart, C.P.; Dewey, K.G.; Ahmed, T.; Ashraf, S.; Christensen, G.; Clasen, T.; et al. Cluster-randomised controlled trials of individual and combined water, sanitation, hygiene and nutritional interventions in rural Bangladesh and Kenya: The WASH Benefits study design and rationale. BMJ Open 2013, 3, e003476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Null, C.; Stewart, C.P.; Pickering, A.J.; Dentz, H.N.; Arnold, B.F.; Arnold, C.D.; Benjamin-Chung, J.; Clasen, T.; Dewey, K.G.; Fernald, L.C.H.; et al. Effects of water quality, sanitation, handwashing, and nutritional interventions on diarrhoea and child growth in rural Kenya: A cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet Glob. Health 2016, 6, e316–e329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Stewart, C.P.; Kariger, P.; Fernald, L.C.H.; Pickering, A.J.; Arnold, C.D.; Arnold, B.F.; Hubbard, A.E.; Dentz, H.N.; Lin, A.; Meerkerk, T.J.; et al. Effects of water quality, sanitation, handwashing, and nutritional interventions on child development in rural Kenya (WASH Benefits Kenya): A cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet Child Adol. Health 2018, 2, 269–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Byrd, K.A.; Williams, T.N.; Lin, A.; Pickering, A.J.; Arnold, B.F.; Arnold, C.D.; Kiprotich, M.; Dentz, H.N.; Njenga, S.M.; Rao, G.; et al. Sickle cell and α+-thalassemia traits influence the association between ferritin and hepcidin in rural Kenyan children aged 14–26 months. J. Nutr. 2018, 148, 1903–1910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Lin, A.; Mertens, A.; Arnold, B.F. WASH Benefits Analysis of Environmental Enteric Dysfunction Outcomes. OSF. Available online: https://osf.io/qa43y (accessed on 25 July 2019).
  49. Fernald, L.C.H.; Kariger, P.; Engle, P.; Raikes, A. Examining Early Child. Development in Low-income Countries: A Toolkit for the Assessment of Children in the First Five Years of Life (No. 74771); The World Bank: Washington, DC, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  50. WHO. Haemoglobin concentrations for the diagnosis of anaemia and assessment of severity. In Vitamin and Mineral Nutrition Information System; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  51. Faulkingham, J.; Namazie, C. Measuring Health and Poverty: A Review of Approaches to Identifying the Poor; DFID: London, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  52. Filmer, D.; Pritchett, L. The effect of household wealth on educational attainment: Evidence from 35 countries. Popul. Dev. Rev. 2009, 25, 85–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Filmer, D.; Pritchett, L.H. Estimating wealth effects without expenditure data—Or tears: An application to educational enrollments in states of India. Demography 2001, 38, 115–132. [Google Scholar]
  54. Filipek, P.A.; Accardo, P.J.; Ashwal, S.; Baranek, G.T.; Cook, E.H.; Dawson, G.; Gordon, B.; Gravel, J.S.; Johnson, C.P.; Kallen, R.J.; et al. Practice parameter: Screening and diagnosis of autism. Report of the quality standards subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurology Society. Neurology 2000, 55, 468–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kerstjens, J.M.; Bos, A.F.; ten Vergert, E.M.J.; de Meer, G.; Butcher, P.R.; Reijneveld, S.A. Support for the global feasibility of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire as developmental screener. Early Hum. Dev. 2009, 85, 443–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Rydz, D.; Srour, M.; Oskoui, M.; Marget, N.; Shiller, M.; Birnbaum, R.; Majnemer, A.; Shevell, M.I. Screening for developmental delay in the setting of a community pediatric clinic: A prospective assessment of parent-report questionnaires. Pediatrics 2006, 118, e1178–e1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Fernald, L.C.H.; Kariger, P.; Hidrobo, M.; Gertler, P.J. Socioeconomic gradients in child development in very young children: Evidence from India, Indonesia, Peru, and Senegal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 17273–17280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Tofail, F.; Fernald, L.C.H.; Das, K.K.; Rahman, M.; Ahmed, T.; Jannat, K.K.; Unicomb, L.; Arnold, B.F.; Ashraf, S.; Winch, P.J.; et al. Effect of water quality, sanitation, hand washing, and nutritional interventions on child development in rural Bangladesh (WASH Benefits Bangladesh): A cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet Child Adol. Health 2018, 4, 255–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Ender, P. Binary_mediation: A new command to compute mediations with multiple mediators and binary and continuous variables in STATA. UCLA: Academic Technology Services, Statistical Consulting Group. Available online: http://www.ats.ucla.edu/stat/stata/ado/analysis.2010 (accessed on 25 March 2019).
  60. Wu, A.D.; Zumbo, B.D. Understanding and using mediators and moderators. Soc. Ind. Res. 2008, 87, 367–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Barron, R.M.; Kenny, D.A. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 1173–1182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. National Malaria Control Programme (NMCP), Kenya National Bureau of Statistics (KNBS), ICF International. Kenya Malaria Indicator Survey 2015. Available online: https://dhsprogram.com/pubs/pdf/MIS22/MIS22.pdf (accessed on 25 March 2019).
  63. Peterson, A.T. Shifting suitability for malaria vectors across Africa with warming climates. BMC Infect. Dis. 2009, 9, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  64. Ryan, S.J.; McNally, A.; Johnson, L.R.; Mordecai, E.A.; Ben-Horin, T.; Paaijmans, K.; Lafferty, K.D. Mapping physiological suitability limits for malaria in Africa under climate change. Vector Borne Zoon. Dis. 2015, 15, 718–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Table 1. Socio-demographic and child health characteristics of the study population (n = 592) a.
Table 1. Socio-demographic and child health characteristics of the study population (n = 592) a.
Mean ± SD or N (%)
Maternal education
Some primary, not completed314 (53)
Completed primary151 (26)
At least some secondary127 (21)
Child age (months)24 ± 2
Child sex (male)285 (48)
Stunted (LAZ/HAZ < −2)170 (29)
Underweight (WAZ < −2)40 (7)
Anemic (Hb < 11.0 g/dL)186 (31)
Positive malaria test109 (18)
At risk for gross motor delay b118 (20)
At risk for communication delay b126 (21)
At risk for personal social delay b139 (23)
a An asset index was also derived by use of principal components, including housing structure, electricity, and ownership of a radio, television, mobile phone, clock, bicycle, motorcycle, gas stove, and car. b Children were considered at risk for delay if their Z-scores were below the 25th percentile of control group Z-scores for each domain.
Table 2. Multivariate associations a between the odds of being at risk for developmental delays b and malaria infection (Model 1) with adjustments for anemia (Model 2) as a potential mediator (n = 592).
Table 2. Multivariate associations a between the odds of being at risk for developmental delays b and malaria infection (Model 1) with adjustments for anemia (Model 2) as a potential mediator (n = 592).
Model 1Model 2
OR (95% CI)OR (95% CI)
Gross Motor
Malaria (1 = positive RDT)2.22 (1.35, 3.64) **2.03 (1.21, 3.42) **
Child age (months)0.74 (0.65, 0.83) **0.73 (0.65, 0.83) **
Treatment arm (1 = nutrition)0.71 (0.46, 1.09)0.73 (0.47, 1.12)
Anemia (1 = anemic c) 1.30 (0.81, 2.08)
Communication
Malaria (1 = positive RDT)1.92 (1.19, 3.09) **1.73 (1.05, 2.86) *
Child age (months)1.07 (0.95, 1.20)1.07 (0.40, 0.91) *
Treatment arm (1 = nutrition)1.07 (0.71, 1.61)1.11 (0.73, 1.67)
Anemia (1 = anemic c) 1.35 (0.87, 2.10)
Personal Social
Malaria (1 = positive RDT)2.86 (1.81, 4.55) **2.46 (1.52, 3.99) **
Child age (months)0.82 (0.74, 0.92) **0.82 (0.73, 0.91) **
Treatment arm (1 = nutrition)1.15 (0.77, 1.73)1.22 (0.81, 1.83)
Anemia (1 = anemic c) 1.59 (1.03, 2.47) *
a Logistic regression models, additionally controlled for child sex, asset index, maternal education, and study cluster. b Risk for developmental delay defined as having a Z-score below the 25th percentile control group Z-score. c Hb < 11.0 g/dL. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Table 3. Mediation analyses of anemia in the pathway between malaria and risk of gross motor, communication, and personal social delay (n = 592) a.
Table 3. Mediation analyses of anemia in the pathway between malaria and risk of gross motor, communication, and personal social delay (n = 592) a.
Gross Motor b
OR (95% CI)
Communication b
OR (95% CI)
Personal Social b
OR (95% CI)
Malaria c and anemia d4.42 (2.86, 6.82) **4.42 (2.86, 6.82) **4.42 (2.86, 6.82) **
Malaria and child development (no anemia)2.15 (1.35, 3.44) **2.04 (1.28, 3.23) **2.91 (1.87, 4.53) **
Anemia and child development1.46 (0.96, 2.22) §1.60 (1.06, 2.40) *1.89 (1.28, 2.81) **
Malaria and child development (with anemia)2.02 (1.23, 3.30) **1.83 (1.13, 2.97) *2.54 (1.60, 4.05) **
a Table includes mediation test results of four regressions evaluating the association between: 1) malaria and anemia, 2) malaria and child development outcome (without anemia), 3) anemia and child development outcome, and 4) malaria and child development outcome (with anemia). b Risk for developmental delay defined as having a Z-score below the 25th percentile control group Z-score. c Binary, 1 = positive RDT. d Binary, 1 = anemic (Hb <11.0 g/dL). ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05, § p < 0.1.
Table 4. Proportion of the total association of malaria on risk of gross motor, communication, and personal social delay that is mediated by anemia (n = 592).
Table 4. Proportion of the total association of malaria on risk of gross motor, communication, and personal social delay that is mediated by anemia (n = 592).
Anemia
Gross Motor0.091
Communication 0.16
Personal Social0.14

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Milner, E.M.; Kariger, P.; Pickering, A.J.; Stewart, C.P.; Byrd, K.; Lin, A.; Rao, G.; Achando, B.; Dentz, H.N.; Null, C.; et al. Association between Malaria Infection and Early Childhood Development Mediated by Anemia in Rural Kenya. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 902. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17030902

AMA Style

Milner EM, Kariger P, Pickering AJ, Stewart CP, Byrd K, Lin A, Rao G, Achando B, Dentz HN, Null C, et al. Association between Malaria Infection and Early Childhood Development Mediated by Anemia in Rural Kenya. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(3):902. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17030902

Chicago/Turabian Style

Milner, Erin M., Patricia Kariger, Amy J. Pickering, Christine P. Stewart, Kendra Byrd, Audrie Lin, Gouthami Rao, Beryl Achando, Holly N. Dentz, Clair Null, and et al. 2020. "Association between Malaria Infection and Early Childhood Development Mediated by Anemia in Rural Kenya" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 3: 902. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17030902

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop