Next Article in Journal
Non-Dairy Fermented Beverages as Potential Carriers to Ensure Probiotics, Prebiotics, and Bioactive Compounds Arrival to the Gut and Their Health Benefits
Next Article in Special Issue
Moderate Folic Acid Supplementation in Pregnant Mice Results in Behavioral Alterations in Offspring with Sex-Specific Changes in Methyl Metabolism
Previous Article in Journal
Dietary Macronutrient Intakes and Mortality among Patients with Type 2 Diabetes
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Vitamin D Status Is Not Associated with Cognitive or Motor Function in Pre-School Ugandan Children

by
Agnes M. Mutua
1,*,
Margaret Nampijja
2,3,
Alison M. Elliott
3,4,
John M. Pettifor
5,
Thomas N. Williams
1,6,7,
Amina Abubakar
1,8,9,10,
Emily L. Webb
11 and
Sarah H. Atkinson
1,7,12,*
1
Kenya Medical Research Institute (KEMRI), Centre for Geographic Medicine Research-Coast, KEMRI Wellcome Trust Research Programme, P.O. BOX 230-80108 Kilifi, Kenya
2
Maternal and Child Wellbeing (MCW) Unit, African Population and Health Research Center, P.O. Box 10787-00100 Nairobi, Kenya
3
Medical Research Council/Uganda Virus Research Institute and London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine Uganda Research Unit, P.O. Box 49, Entebbe, Uganda
4
Department of Clinical Research, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London WC1E 7HT, UK
5
South African Medical Research Council and Wits Developmental Pathways for Health Research Unit, Department of Paediatrics, University of the Witwatersrand, 26 Chris Hani Road, Soweto 6201, Johannesburg, South Africa
6
Department of Medicine, Imperial College of Science Technology and Medicine, St Mary’s Hospital, London W2 1NY, UK
7
Centre for Tropical Medicine and Global Health, Nuffield Department of Medicine, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 7FZ, UK
8
Department of Public Health, School of Human and Health Sciences, Pwani University, P.O. BOX 195-80108 Kilifi, Kenya
9
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK
10
Institute for Human Development, Aga Khan University, 2nd Parklands Avenue, P.O. BOX 30270-00100 Nairobi, Kenya
11
MRC Tropical Epidemiology Group, Department of Infectious Disease Epidemiology, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London WC1E 7HT, UK
12
Department of Paediatrics, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 9DU, UK
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2020, 12(6), 1662; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12061662
Submission received: 21 April 2020 / Revised: 26 May 2020 / Accepted: 27 May 2020 / Published: 3 June 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Nutrition and the Brain)

Abstract

:
Vitamin D deficiency is common worldwide and young children are among the most affected groups. Animal studies suggest a key role for vitamin D in brain development. However, studies investigating the effects of vitamin D on neurobehavioural outcomes in children are inconclusive and evidence is limited in sub-Saharan Africa. We evaluated the effect of vitamin D status on cognitive and motor outcomes using prospective data from the Entebbe Mother and Baby Study birth cohort. We analysed data from 302 Ugandan children with 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D) measurements below five years and developmental measures at five years of age. We used multivariable linear regression, adjusted for potential confounders, to estimate the effect of 25(OH)D on cognitive and motor outcomes. Of 302 children, eight (2.7%) had 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L, 105 (35.8%) had levels 50–75 nmol/L and 189 (62.6%) had levels >75 nmol/L. There was no evidence that earlier vitamin D status was associated with cognitive and motor outcomes in five-year-old Ugandan children. This study adds to the sparse literature and highlights the need for further longitudinal studies on vitamin D and neurobehavioural outcomes in children living in sub-Saharan Africa.

1. Introduction

The first five years in a child’s life are a period of rapid brain development [1]. About 81 million children aged three to four years have poor cognitive and socioemotional development in low and middle-income countries worldwide and 29.4 million of these children live in sub-Saharan Africa [2]. Therefore, mitigation of recognized risk factors for impaired development, including infections, inadequately stimulating home environments and malnutrition, is necessary [3]. Evidence indicates that micronutrient deficiencies may also be an important risk factor for impaired neurobehavioural outcomes [4]. Vitamin D deficiency and/or insufficiency is prevalent among populations worldwide [5,6] and children are among the most at-risk groups for sub-optimal 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D) levels [7,8]. A recent systematic review and meta-analysis of vitamin D deficiency in Africa reported a pooled prevalence of vitamin D deficiency (25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L) of 49.1% among newborns and 23.0% among children below 18 years of age [9].
Vitamin D may influence neurobehavioural outcomes through its role in brain development. The presence of vitamin D receptors and metabolites in cerebrospinal fluid and parts of the brain including the hippocampus and the cortex may indicate a key role for vitamin D in brain development [10]. Vitamin D deficiency is also associated with structural and functional alterations in the developing brains of rodents, which are associated with impaired behaviour, learning, motor and cognitive processes [11,12,13]. Furthermore, vitamin D appears to plays a role in neuroprotection through its anti-inflammatory properties in the brain [14]. There are few epidemiological studies of the effect of vitamin D status on neurobehavioural outcomes in children and their findings are inconsistent [15]. One small randomized controlled trial, without a placebo arm, reported a beneficial effect of supplementation with lower doses of vitamin D compared to higher doses [16] while another found no beneficial effect of vitamin D supplementation on cognitive development in extremely preterm infants compared to no supplementation [17].
Only two studies have investigated the relationship between the vitamin D status of children and their neurobehavioural outcomes in Africa, and these were in school-aged children. A prospective study of 254 Ugandan children reported associations between 25(OH)D levels and socio-emotional adjustment outcomes among school-children who were stratified according to perinatal HIV exposure/infection and in utero/peripartum antiretroviral exposure [18], while a cross-sectional study of 45 Egyptian school-children reported a positive association between higher 25(OH)D levels and improved cognition and school performance compared to lower levels [19]. Our study aimed to evaluate the association between vitamin D status and cognitive and motor outcomes in pre-school Ugandan children using data from the Entebbe Mother and Baby Study (EMaBS) prospective birth cohort. We analysed data from 302 community-based children with 25(OH)D measurements below five years of age and cognitive and motor development assessed at five years of age.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Ethical Approval

Ethical approval was given by the Medical Research Council (MRC)/Uganda Virus Research Institute Research Ethics Committee (reference numbers: GC/127 and GC/127/15/04/35), Uganda National Council for Science and Technology and the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine Ethics Committee (reference numbers: 790 and 17261). Parents or guardians provided informed consent in writing, or with a thumbprint if not literate with a signature from a literate witness.

2.2. Participants and Study Design

The Entebbe Mother and Baby Study (EMaBS) prospective birth cohort was initially designed as a double-blind randomized controlled trial of the effects of anthelmintic treatment during pregnancy and early childhood on immunological and disease outcomes in childhood (ISRCTN32849447). Details of the study are described elsewhere [20]. Briefly, between April 2003 and November 2005, 2507 pregnant women were recruited into the study during their first antenatal care clinic visit at Entebbe hospital and randomized to receive either albendazole or placebo and praziquantel or placebo in a 2 × 2 factorial design. Their children formed the EMaBS birth cohort that was followed up from birth and seen during scheduled annual visits or when they were sick. The children were randomized to receive albendazole or placebo from the age of 15 months to five years and children with helminth infection detected at annual visits were treated according to clinical guidelines. Data on sex, birthweight, gestational age and anthropometric measurements were recorded and blood samples were taken at birth and during annual visits. Children were brought to the study clinic for diagnosis and treatment when they were unwell. Information on maternal age, socioeconomic status, location, education, parity and medical conditions was collected at enrolment. Household socioeconomic status was derived as a composite of building materials of the home, number of rooms and items owned. A total asset score was calculated and categorized into 6 ordered categorical levels with 1 indicating lowest and 6 indicating highest household socioeconomic status.

2.3. Sample Size

In the larger EMaBS study, the cohort size of 2500 was determined based on the primary immunological objectives. For the present sub-study, assuming a prevalence of 36.6% of 25(OH)D levels ≤75 nmol/L among Ugandan children [21] and a standard deviation of two in cognitive and motor scores [22], our sample size of 302 has 80% power at α = 0.05 to detect a difference of 0.7 when comparing mean cognitive/motor scores between the 25(OH)D levels ≤75 nmol/L and >75 nmol/L groups.

2.4. Laboratory Procedures and Definitions

Stored blood samples were analysed based on availability with most samples being from two years of age. The biomarkers assayed were plasma 25(OH)D (Chemiluminescent Microparticle Immunoassay (CMI), Abbott Architect), ferritin (CMI, Abbott Architect), haemoglobin (Medonic CA 530) and C-reactive protein (CRP, MULTIGENT CRP Vario assay, Abbott Architect) [23]. For this study, we classified 25(OH)D levels into three categories; levels <50 nmol/L, 50–75 nmol/L and >75 nmol/L, according to the Endocrine Society Practice Guidelines [24]. We defined iron deficiency as plasma ferritin <12 µg/L and inflammation as CRP levels >5 mg/L. Anaemia was defined as haemoglobin levels <11 g/dL, after subtracting 0.2 g/dL from all haemoglobin values to adjust for altitude (1000 m above sea level). Malaria parasitaemia was measured using Giemsa-stained thick and thin blood smears and a malaria episode was defined as parasitaemia and temperature >37.5 °C. All children were tested annually for asymptomatic malaria and the study included longitudinal active surveillance for malaria and other infections during fortnightly home visits and quarterly clinic visits. Children of HIV-positive mothers were tested for HIV at six weeks and 18 months using a filter-paper blood spot for DNA-PCR, a measure for HIV-antibodies and viral RNA.

2.5. Cognitive and Motor Assessments

At age five years, cognitive and motor tests were performed at the EMaBS clinic by trained nurses and doctors in 60–90-min sessions including breaks. Of the 13 measures used to assess cognitive and motor abilities at five years, four (counting span, running memory, shapes task and tower of London) were excluded in this analysis as they were administered to a small sub-set of children (n < 113). We combined the remaining nine individual measures using principal components analysis to generate scores on three components of child development; verbal and non-verbal intelligence quotient (IQ), executive function (EF) and motor function. We excluded two measures (tap one tap twice task and sentence repetition) that did not load well on the IQ, EF or motor function components. We excluded one recorded observation in the coinbox test and another in the balancing on one leg test as they were beyond the maximum attainable scores for the respective measures. Details of the translation and validation of all of the neurobehavioural measures in the Ugandan setting are described elsewhere [25]. In a small pilot study, involving children not in the current study, the measures had good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.65 − 0.82) and test-retest reliability (correlation coefficient (r) = 0.45 − 0.88), except the Wisconsin card sort test (r = 0.22) which was administered to only 19 children in the pilot study [25]. The measures used for the current study are briefly described in Table 1.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted using STATA version 15.0 (StataCorp, College Station, TX 77845, United States of America). We assessed the distribution of 25(OH)D levels and developmental scores using histograms and scatterplots. We examined the characteristics of the participants using descriptive statistics including proportions and means with standard deviations and used ANOVA to compare mean scores between children with 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L, 50–75 nmol/L and >75 nmol/L. We compared baseline characteristics of children included in these analyses (n = 302) and those who were not included (n = 2043). Of the 2043 children, 594 were lost to follow-up, 116 died, 312 did not attend their fifth annual visit, 540 did not have cognitive and motor outcome data and 481 children did not have data on 25(OH)D levels prior to five years of age.
We used univariable and multivariable linear regression models to estimate the effect of vitamin D in early childhood on cognitive and motor outcomes at five years. We identified potential confounders from literature including age at 25(OH)D measurement, sex, iron status and anaemia (haemoglobin levels <11 g/dL), inflammation (CRP levels >5 mg/L), height-for-age Z-scores, randomized treatment of children with albendazole, socioeconomic status, maternal education and asymptomatic malaria parasitaemia. We empirically tested associations of the potential confounders identified from literature in addition to weight-for-age and weight-for-height Z-scores, location, maternal age, parity and randomized maternal treatment with albendazole and praziquantel with 25(OH)D levels and cognitive and motor outcomes using multivariable linear regression models. Then, using a forward modelling approach, we fitted three separate linear regression models for each outcome starting with separate crude models with 25(OH)D levels as the exposure variable (included as a continuous variable, per 10 nmol/L increase) for each of the three outcomes. Then we generated minimally adjusted models with age at vitamin D measurement, sex, iron and anaemia status included as potential confounders. Finally, we added other potential confounders one by one to the minimally adjusted models observing the change in regression coefficients until final multivariable models for each of the three outcomes were generated. We re-ran these final models, fitting vitamin D as a categorical variable (25(OH)D levels ≤75 nmol/L compared to levels >75 nmol/L). We were unable to further investigate associations with 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L using multivariable analysis since few children (n = 8) had 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L, so children with 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L were grouped with children that had levels 50–75 nmol/L. We also compared children with 25(OH)D levels 50–75 nmol/L with children that had levels >75 nmol/L.
Due to missing observations in haemoglobin (16 missing values) and ferritin levels (14 missing values), we assessed whether excluding these variables in final models impacted results. We examined multicollinearity using the variance inflation factor (VIF) which measures change in the regression coefficient if predictor variables are correlated, with a VIF > 10 suggesting severe multicollinearity. None of the variables had a VIF > 2 in any model although clinical malaria episodes and asymptomatic malaria parasitaemia appeared to be moderately correlated and we therefore, only adjusted for asymptomatic malaria parasitaemia.

3. Results

3.1. Description of Study Participants

A total of 302 children, 48.3% boys and 51.7% girls were included in this study. None of the included children or their mothers received vitamin D supplementation. Characteristics of the study participants are presented in Table 2. Most children (68.2%) had 25(OH)D levels measured at the second annual visit (mean age = 2.3 years, standard deviation (SD) = 0.7) and mean age at cognitive and motor assessment was 5.0 (SD = 0.02) years. Mean (SD) and median (interquartile range) 25(OH)D levels were 83.2 (22.2) nmol/L and 81.0 (67.9 to 95.5) nmol/L, respectively. Eight (2.7%) children had 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L, 105 (34.8%) had levels 50–75 nmol/L and 189 (62.6%) had levels >75 nmol/L.
In this population, stunting at age 5 years was more common (34.4%) than underweight (11.1%) or wasting (4.7%). Malaria was common, with 55.3% children having had at least one episode of clinical malaria while 11.3% had asymptomatic malaria parasitaemia at an annual visit between birth and five years. Over a third of the children were anaemic, while 27.1% were iron deficient and 23.1% had inflammation at the time of 25(OH)D measurement. The majority of participants resided in urban (39.1%) or peri-urban (26.2%) areas (Table 2). Of 302 children, four (1.3%) were HIV positive at 18 months and of 300 children with available data, four (1.7%) were delivered preterm (<37 gestational weeks). Baseline characteristics were comparable between participants included in the analyses and those not included, with the exception that stunting was more common while asymptomatic malaria parasitaemia was less common among participants than those not included. Participants were more likely to have higher socioeconomic status while mothers of participants were older and had higher parity compared to children who were not included (Supplementary Table S1).

3.2. Associations Between 25(OH)D and Participant Characteristics

In the multivariable analyses we observed evidence of a decreasing trend of 25(OH)D levels with increasing child age and asymptomatic malaria was associated with high 25(OH)D levels. Inflammation was associated with higher mean 25(OH)D levels and 25(OH)D levels >75 nmol/L were more common among those with inflammation than for those with lower CRP levels (Table 2).

3.3. Cognitive and Motor Outcomes at Five Years and Associations with Participant Characteristics

The IQ, EF and motor function scores were normally distributed. Mean scores for the individual tests that were used to assess cognitive and motor outcomes are presented in Supplementary Table S2. Girls had higher EF and motor scores compared to boys after adjusting for potential confounders, but there was no difference by sex for IQ scores (Table 3). Children who were randomly allocated placebo in the original EMaBS trial had lower motor scores compared to those who received albendazole, but little difference was observed in IQ and EF scores (Table 3). Stunting, low socioeconomic status and low levels of maternal education were associated with lower IQ scores in univariable analyses but not after adjusting for potential confounders. No associations were observed with other participant characteristics in univariable analyses (Supplementary Table S3).

3.4. Associations between 25(OH)D and Cognitive and Motor Outcomes

Table 4 shows the univariable and multivariable linear regression results for the effect of 25(OH)D levels on cognitive and motor outcomes. We observed little association between 25(OH)D levels and IQ, EF or motor function either in crude analysis or after adjusting for potential confounders. Considering 25(OH)D as a categorical variable, we found no evidence of differences in the mean scores among children with 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L, 50–75 nmol/L and >75 nmol/L for IQ (p = 0.89), EF (p = 0.44) or motor function (p = 0.44) outcomes. We found little evidence of association between IQ, EF or motor function and 25(OH)D levels ≤75 nmol/L compared to levels >75 nmol/L or for levels 50–75 nmol/L compared to levels >75 nmol/L after adjusting for potential confounders (Table 4).

4. Discussion

We evaluated the association between 25(OH)D levels measured in young Ugandan children below five years and their cognitive and motor outcomes at five years of age. In this study population, we found that few children had 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L (2.7%), and the majority of children (62.6%) had 25(OH)D levels >75 nmol/L. We found no evidence of association between vitamin D status and IQ, EF or motor function. This finding may be explained by the small proportion of children with 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L limiting the ability to detect an effect, especially if 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L, rather than 25(OH)D levels ≤75 nmol/L, drives impaired neurobehavioural outcomes [33]. Girls had higher EF and motor scores compared to boys but there was no difference by sex for IQ scores. Children who were randomly allocated placebo in the original trial had marginally lower motor scores compared to those who received albendazole, but we did not observe differences in IQ and EF scores.
Our findings of no association between vitamin D status and cognitive or motor function, agree with nine previous observational studies [34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42]. On the contrary, a small dose-response randomized controlled trial (n = 55) of vitamin D supplementation in Canadian infants reported that a lower dose of 400 international units (IU) of vitamin D was more beneficial for gross motor development than higher doses of 800 IU or 1200 IU [16]. This finding may indicate a threshold effect for vitamin D but may be limited by the small sample size and lack of a control group that did not receive vitamin D supplements. Additionally, a cross-sectional study of one-year-old Iranian children (n = 186) reported a positive association between high 25(OH)D levels and improved motor function [43]. To our knowledge, ours is the first prospective study to investigate the relationship between vitamin D status and cognitive and motor development in pre-school children in sub-Saharan Africa. To date, only three observational studies have evaluated the effect of child or maternal vitamin D status on neurobehavioural outcomes in African children. One prospective study (n = 254) observed an association between increasing 25(OH)D levels and improved socioemotional adjustment in Ugandan school-aged children who were HIV negative and those perinatally exposed to HIV but uninfected and unexposed to early antiretroviral therapy (ART) [18]. They also observed an association between increasing 25(OH)D levels and poor socioemotional adjustment in HIV-infected children and in HIV-uninfected children exposed to early ART, which may be attributed to dysregulation of 25(OH)D metabolism by ART. Another small study (n = 45) reported that high 25(OH)D levels were positively associated with improved cognition in Egyptian school-aged children [19]. Additionally, an observational study of 202 mother-child pairs in Seychelles reported no association between maternal 25(OH)D levels during pregnancy and neurobehavioural outcomes at five years [44].
Contrary to our findings, animal and in vitro studies provide consistent evidence for the adverse effects of vitamin D deficiency on neurodevelopment and possible mechanisms for this effect. The presence of vitamin D receptors and metabolism enzymes in parts of the brain involved in cognition suggests that vitamin D may be important for cognition [45]. Similarly, the presence of vitamin D receptors in skeletal muscles suggests that vitamin D is important for muscle development and subsequent motor function. Experimental evidence shows reduced type II muscle fibre diameter in mice whose skeletal muscle vitamin D receptors are knocked out [46]. Furthermore, vitamin D deficiency is implicated in alterations in neurotransmitter synthesis and synaptic plasticity which may affect learning processes, cognition [11,47] and motor processes [13,48]. However, the application of these findings to humans may be limited as experimental studies are able to generate conditions of adverse vitamin D deficiency to an extent unlikely to be observed in humans. Additionally, it is unclear when 25(OH)D levels might influence neurobehavioural outcomes. Some observational studies in mother-child pairs suggest that levels of 25(OH)D influence neurobehavioural outcomes exclusively during the prenatal period [49,50,51]. However, other similar studies have reported little association between maternal 25(OH)D levels and neurobehavioural outcomes in children [52,53].
We observed a decreasing trend of 25(OH)D levels with increasing child age consistent with previous evidence [54]. Younger children may acquire 25(OH)D from their mothers through placental transfer of 25(OH)D in utero or breastfeeding during infancy. Asymptomatic malaria parasitaemia was associated with higher 25(OH)D levels. Evidence on the relationship between malaria and 25(OH)D levels is limited and inconclusive [55,56]. High 25(OH)D levels were associated with increased inflammation contrary to other studies that have shown possible anti-inflammatory properties of vitamin D [57]. We found that two other factors influenced neurobehavioural outcomes. Girls performed better in the EF and motor tests compared to boys, consistent with previous studies [58,59]. There is no clear evidence of the causes for the observed differences though they may be attributed to differences in environmental and socio-cultural factors between boys and girls [60]. It is also possible that the differences were a developmental pattern that changed after the pre-school age [61]. Furthermore, randomized treatment of children with albendazole in the EMaBS trial was marginally associated with higher motor scores compared to placebo treatment. This may be a chance finding since in the larger EMaBS birth cohort no associations were observed between either child or maternal treatment with albendazole and cognitive or motor scores [62,63].
Strengths of our study include the extensive battery of tools used to measure cognitive and motor outcomes that were specifically validated and adapted to the Ugandan population [25]. Moreover, prospectively collected data on multiple covariates were available allowing adjustment for many potential confounders. Furthermore, we analysed 25(OH)D both as continuous and categorical variables which was important considering the lack of global consensus on vitamin D status cut-offs. Several limitations must be considered when interpreting our results. We included about 13% of participants in the original EMaBS birth cohort in our analyses as 25(OH)D levels and cognitive and motor outcomes were assessed in smaller subsets of participants and over a quarter of the original participants were lost to follow up. However, our study was adequately powered to detect the reported effect sizes, and selection bias was unlikely as most baseline characteristics were comparable between children who were included in these analyses and those who were not included. We were unable to assess the association between 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L and cognitive and motor outcomes as only 2.7% children had 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L in this study population. Additionally, plasma 25(OH)D levels were measured at one time-point prior to developmental assessment and may not reflect the vitamin D status of the children throughout early childhood or at the time of cognitive and motor assessment, which could lead to underestimation of the association between 25(OH)D and cognitive or motor outcomes. Further, the possibility of other unmeasured variables that may affect 25(OH)D levels cannot be ruled out. These include habits that might influence exposure to sunlight, for example, outdoor physical activity and clothing and dietary vitamin D intake. However, diet may not be an important risk factor since vitamin D fortification and supplementation is not routine in Uganda. There may have also been other unmeasured factors that might have influenced cognitive and motor outcomes, such as pre-schooling, inadequately stimulating home environment or maternal IQ. Additionally, other variables recoded at enrolment such as location and socioeconomic status of some participants may have changed over the study period which may possibly result in residual confounding for the associations between 25(OH)D and cognitive and motor outcomes.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, our findings add to the evidence from similar studies that report no association between vitamin D status and cognitive and motor outcomes in children. This study adds to the very limited literature on the effect of vitamin D status on neurobehavioural outcomes in children from sub-Saharan Africa. Further longitudinal studies in populations with 25(OH)D levels <50 nmol/L and well-conducted randomized controlled trials of vitamin D supplementation would be valuable in clarifying the causal effect of vitamin D status in early childhood for cognitive and motor development.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2072-6643/12/6/1662/s1, Table S1: Comparison of baseline characteristics between children who were included in this analysis and children who were excluded due to loss to follow up or death or missing 25(OH)D and/or cognitive and motor outcome data. Table S2: Summary of cognitive and motor development scores, Table S3: Univariable linear regression results for the association between exposure variables and outcomes.

Author Contributions

A.M.M., S.H.A., A.A., E.L.W., M.N. and A.M.E. conceptualized the study and obtained the relevant data transfer authorization. A.M.E., S.H.A, E.L.W. and M.N. provided the datasets. A.M.M. and E.L.W. conducted the formal analysis. M.N. and A.A. advised and validated the combination of developmental outcomes using principal components analyses. A.M.M. and S.H.A. wrote the first draft and A.M.M., S.H.A., A.A., M.N., J.M.P., T.N.W., E.L.W. and A.M.E. reviewed and edited successive drafts of the paper and approved the final version. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by Wellcome [Grant numbers 209640 to A.M.M., 110255 to S.H.A., 202800 to TNW, 064693, 079110, 095778 to A.M.E.] and and with core awards to the KEMRI-Wellcome Trust Research Programme (203077) and by the Medical Research Council [Grant number MR/M025454/1 to A.A.]. The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study, manuscript preparation or interpretation of the findings.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the permission of the Director of the Kenya Medical Research Institute (KEMRI) for publication.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Shonkoff, J.P.; Phillips, D.A. From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2000; pp. 182–185. [Google Scholar]
  2. McCoy, D.C.; Peet, E.D.; Ezzati, M.; Danaei, G.; Black, M.M.; Sudfeld, C.R.; Fawzi, W.; Fink, G. Early childhood developmental status in low-and middle-income countries: National, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modeling. PLoS Med. 2016, 13, e1002034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Black, M.M.; Walker, S.P.; Fernald, L.C.; Andersen, C.T.; DiGirolamo, A.M.; Lu, C.; McCoy, D.C.; Fink, G.; Shawar, Y.R.; Shiffman, J. Early childhood development coming of age: Science through the life course. The Lancet 2017, 389, 77–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Black, M.M. Micronutrient deficiencies and cognitive functioning. J. Nutr. 2003, 133, 3927S–3931S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Lips, P. Vitamin D physiology. Prog. Biophys. Mol. Biol. 2006, 92, 4–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wahl, D.; Cooper, C.; Ebeling, P.; Eggersdorfer, M.; Hilger, J.; Hoffmann, K.; Josse, R.; Kanis, J.; Mithal, A.; Pierroz, D. A global representation of vitamin D status in healthy populations. Arch. Osteoporos. 2012, 7, 155–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Hilger, J.; Friedel, A.; Herr, R.; Rausch, T.; Roos, F.; Wahl, D.A.; Pierroz, D.D.; Weber, P.; Hoffmann, K. A systematic review of vitamin D status in populations worldwide. Br. J. Nutr. 2014, 111, 23–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Roth, D.E.; Abrams, S.A.; Aloia, J.; Bergeron, G.; Bourassa, M.W.; Brown, K.H.; Calvo, M.S.; Cashman, K.D.; Combs, G.; De-Regil, L.M. Global prevalence and disease burden of vitamin D deficiency: A roadmap for action in low-and middle-income countries. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2018, 1430, 44–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Mogire, R.M.; Mutua, A.; Kimita, W.; Kamau, A.; Bejon, P.; Pettifor, J.M.; Adeyemo, A.; Williams, T.N.; Atkinson, S.H. Prevalence of vitamin D deficiency in Africa: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Glob. Health 2019, 8, e134–e142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Harms, L.R.; Burne, T.H.; Eyles, D.W.; McGrath, J.J. Vitamin D and the brain. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2011, 25, 657–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Al-Harbi, A.N.; Khan, K.M.; Rahman, A. Developmental Vitamin D Deficiency Affects Spatial Learning in Wistar Rats. J. Nutr. 2017, 147, 1795–1805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. De Abreu, D.A.F.; Nivet, E.; Baril, N.; Khrestchatisky, M.; Roman, F.; Féron, F. Developmental vitamin D deficiency alters learning in C57Bl/6J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2010, 208, 603–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Kesby, J.P.; Cui, X.; Ko, P.; McGrath, J.J.; Burne, T.H.; Eyles, D.W. Developmental vitamin D deficiency alters dopamine turnover in neonatal rat forebrain. Neurosci. Lett. 2009, 461, 155–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. AlJohri, R.; AlOkail, M.; Haq, S.H. Neuroprotective role of vitamin D in primary neuronal cortical culture. eNeurologicalSci 2019, 14, 43–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mutua, A.M.; Mogire, R.M.; Elliott, A.M.; Williams, T.N.; Webb, E.L.; Abubakar, A.; Atkinson, S.H. Effects of vitamin D deficiency on neurobehavioural outcomes in children: A systematic review. Wellcome Op. Res. 2020, 5, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Wicklow, B.; Gallo, S.; Majnemer, A.; Vanstone, C.; Comeau, K.; Jones, G.; L’Abbe, M.; Khamessan, A.; Sharma, A.; Weiler, H. Impact of vitamin D supplementation on gross motor development of healthy term infants: A randomized dose-response trial. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2016, 36, 330–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Salas, A.A.; Woodfin, T.; Phillips, V.; Peralta-Carcelen, M.; Carlo, W.A.; Ambalavanan, N. Dose-response effects of early vitamin D supplementation on neurodevelopmental and respiratory outcomes of extremely preterm infants at 2 years of age: A randomized trial. Neonatology 2018, 113, 256–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Yakah, W.; Fenton, J.I.; Sikorskii, A.; Zalwango, S.K.; Tuke, R.; Musoke, P.; Boivin, M.J.; Giordani, B.; Ezeamama, A.E. Serum Vitamin D is Differentially Associated with Socioemotional Adjustment in Early School-Aged Ugandan Children According to Perinatal HIV Status and In Utero/Peripartum Antiretroviral Exposure History. Nutrients 2019, 11, 1570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Nassar, M.F.; Amin, D.A.; Hamed, A.I.; Nassar, J.F.; Abou-Zeid, A.-E.K.; Attaby, M.A. Vitamin D status and scholastic achievement in middle age childhood. J. Egypt. Soc. Parasitol. 2012, 240, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Elliott, A.M.; Kizza, M.; Quigley, M.A.; Ndibazza, J.; Nampijja, M.; Muhangi, L.; Morison, L.; Namujju, P.B.; Muwanga, M.; Kabatereine, N. The impact of helminths on the response to immunization and on the incidence of infection and disease in childhood in Uganda: Design of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, factorial trial of deworming interventions delivered in pregnancy and early childhood [ISRCTN32849447]. Clin. Trials. 2007, 4, 42–57. [Google Scholar]
  21. Nabeta, H.W.; Kasolo, J.; Kiggundu, R.K.; Kiragga, A.N.; Kiguli, S. Serum vitamin D status in children with protein-energy malnutrition admitted to a national referral hospital in Uganda. BMC Res. Notes. 2015, 8, 418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Nampijja, M. The Impact of Helminth Infections on Developmental and Educational Outcomes. In Handbook of Applied Developmental Science in Sub-Saharan Africa; Abubakar, A., Van de Vijver, F., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 133–156. [Google Scholar]
  23. Muriuki, J.M.; Mentzer, A.J.; Kimita, W.; Ndungu, F.M.; Macharia, A.W.; Webb, E.L.; Lule, S.A.; Morovat, A.; Hill, A.V.; Bejon, P. Iron status and associated malaria risk among African children. Clin. Infect. Dis. 2018, 68, 1807–1814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Endocrine Society: clinical practice guidelines for vitamin D deficiency. Available online: https://www.healio.com/endocrinology/practice-management/news/online/%7Bf7557f26-0d85-4ec8-a33a-ef05f41e0e94%7D/endocrine-society-publishes-clinical-practice-guidelines-for-vitamin-d-deficiency (accessed on 24 July 2019).
  25. Nampijja, M.; Apule, B.; Lule, S.; Akurut, H.; Muhangi, L.; Elliott, A.; Alcock, K.J. Adaptation of Western measures of cognition for assessing 5-year-old semi-urban Ugandan children. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 80, 15–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Elliott, C.S.; McCulloch, K. The British Ability Scales, 3rd ed.; National Foundation for Psychological Research: Windsor, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  27. Holding, P.A.; Taylor, H.G.; Kazungu, S.D.; Mkala, T.; Gona, J.; Mwamuye, B.; Mbonani, L.; Stevenson, J. Assessing cognitive outcomes in a rural African population: Development of a neuropsychological battery in Kilifi District, Kenya. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2004, 10, 246–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Korkman, M.; Kirk, U.; Kemps, A. A developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  29. Manly, T.; Anderson, V.; Nimmo-Smith, I.; Turner, A.; Watson, P.; Robertson, I.H. The differential assessment of children’s attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2001, 42, 1065–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Berg, E.A. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. J. Gen. Psychol. 1948, 39, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Abubakar, A.; Holding, P.; Van Baar, A.; Newton, C.; van de Vijver, F.J. Monitoring psychomotor development in a resourcelimited setting: An evaluation of the Kilifi Developmental Inventory. Ann. Trop. Paediatr. 2008, 28, 217–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Henderson, S.E.; Sugden, D. Movement assessment battery for children, 1st ed.; The Psychological Corporation: London, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  33. Baksi, S.N.; Hughes, M.J. Chronic vitamin D deficiency in the weanling rat alters catecholamine metabolism in the cortex. Brain Res. 1982, 242, 387–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Chowdhury, R.; Taneja, S.; Bhandari, N.; Kvestad, I.; Strand, T.A.; Bhan, M.K. Vitamin-D status and neurodevelopment and growth in young north Indian children: A secondary data analysis. Nutr. J. 2017, 16, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Chowdhury, R.; Taneja, S.; Kvestad, I.; Hysing, M.; Bhandari, N.; Strand, T.A. Vitamin D status in early childhood is not associated with cognitive development and linear growth at 6–9 years of age in North Indian children: a cohort study. Nutr. J. 2020, 19, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Filteau, S.; Rehman, A.M.; Yousafzai, A.; Chugh, R.; Kaur, M.; Sachdev, H.; Trilok-Kumar, G. Associations of vitamin D status, bone health and anthropometry, with gross motor development and performance of school-aged Indian children who were born at term with low birth weight. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e009268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Rahman, A.; Al-Taiar, A.; Shaban, L.; Al-Sabah, R.; Al-Harbi, A.; Mojiminiyi, O. Plasma 25-hydroxy vitamin D is not associated with either cognitive function or academic performance in adolescents. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Schmidt, R.J.; Niu, Q.; Eyles, D.W.; Hansen, R.L.; Iosif, A.M. Neonatal vitamin D status in relation to autism spectrum disorder and developmental delay in the CHARGE case–control study. Autism Res. 2019, 12, 976–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Tolppanen, A.M.; Williams, D.; Lawlor, D.A. The association of circulating 25-hydroxyvitamin D and calcium with cognitive performance in adolescents: Cross-sectional study using data from the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2011, 25, 67–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Trilok-Kumar, G.; Kaur, M.; Rehman, A.M.; Arora, H.; Rajput, M.M.; Chugh, R.; Kurpad, A.; Sachdev, H.S.; Filteau, S. Effects of vitamin D supplementation in infancy on growth, bone parameters, body composition and gross motor development at age 3–6 years: Follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Epidemiol 2015, 44, 894–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Windham, G.C.; Pearl, M.; Anderson, M.C.; Poon, V.; Eyles, D.; Jones, K.L.; Lyall, K.; Kharrazi, M.; Croen, L.A. Newborn vitamin D levels in relation to autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability: A case–control study in California. Autism Res. 2019, 12, 989–998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Tofail, F.; Islam, M.M.; Mahfuz, M.; Ashraful Alam, M.; Aktar, S.; Haque, R.; Hossain, M.I.; Mondal, D.; Petri, W.A.; Ahmed, T. Association of vitamin D nutrition with neuro-developmental outcome of infants of slums in Bangladesh. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0221805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Tavakolizadeh, R.; Ardalani, M.; Shariatpanahi, G.; Mojtahedi, S.Y.; Sayarifard, A. Is There Any Relationship between Vitamin D Deficiency and Gross Motor Development in12-Month-Old Children? Iran. J. Child Neurol. 2019, 13, 55–60. [Google Scholar]
  44. Laird, E.; Thurston, S.W.; van Wijngaarden, E.; Shamlaye, C.F.; Myers, G.J.; Davidson, P.W.; Watson, G.E.; McSorley, E.M.; Mulhern, M.S.; Yeates, A.J.; et al. Maternal vitamin D status and the relationship with neonatal anthropometric and childhood neurodevelopmental outcomes: Results from the Seychelles child development nutrition study. Nutrients 2017, 9, 1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Marini, F.; Bartoccini, E.; Cascianelli, G.; Voccoli, V.; Baviglia, M.G.; Magni, M.V.; Garcia-Gil, M.; Albi, E. Effect of 1α, 25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 in embryonic hippocampal cells. Hippocampus 2010, 20, 696–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Chen, S.; Villalta, S.A.; Agrawal, D.K. FOXO1 Mediates Vitamin D Deficiency–Induced Insulin Resistance in Skeletal Muscle. J. Bone Miner. Res. 2016, 31, 585–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Taghizadeh, M.; Talaei, S.A.; Salami, M. Vitamin D deficiency impairs spatial learning in adult rats. Iran. Biomed. J. 2013, 17, 42–48. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  48. Hawes, J.E.; Tesic, D.; Whitehouse, A.J.; Zosky, G.R.; Smith, J.T.; Wyrwoll, C.S. Maternal vitamin D deficiency alters fetal brain development in the BALB/c mouse. Behav. Brain Res. 2015, 286, 192–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Darling, A.L.; Rayman, M.P.; Steer, C.D.; Golding, J.; Lanham-New, S.A.; Bath, S.C. Association between maternal vitamin D status in pregnancy and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood: Results from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Br. J. Nutr. 2017, 117, 1682–1692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Keim, S.A.; Bodnar, L.M.; Klebanoff, M.A. Maternal and cord blood 25 (OH)-vitamin D concentrations in relation to child development and behaviour. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2014, 28, 434–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Zhu, P.; Tong, S.-L.; Hao, J.-H.; Tao, R.-X.; Huang, K.; Hu, W.-B.; Zhou, Q.-F.; Jiang, X.-M.; Tao, F.-B. Cord Blood Vitamin D and Neurocognitive Development Are Nonlinearly Related in Toddlers. J. Nutr. 2015, 145, 1232–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Gale, C.R.; Robinson, S.M.; Harvey, N.C.; Javaid, M.K.; Jiang, B.; Martyn, C.N.; Godfrey, K.M.; Cooper, C. Maternal vitamin D status during pregnancy and child outcomes. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 62, 68–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Veena, S.R.; Krishnaveni, G.V.; Srinivasan, K.; Thajna, K.P.; Hegde, B.G.; Gale, C.R.; Fall, C.H. Association between maternal vitamin D status during pregnancy and offspring cognitive function during childhood and adolescence. Asia Pac. J. Clin. Nutr. 2017, 26, 438–449. [Google Scholar]
  54. Zhu, Z.; Zhan, J.; Shao, J.; Chen, W.; Chen, L.; Li, W.; Ji, C.; Zhao, Z. High prevalence of vitamin D deficiency among children aged 1 month to 16 years in Hangzhou, China. BMC Public Health 2012, 12, 126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Cusick, S.E.; Opoka, R.O.; Lund, T.C.; John, C.C.; Polgreen, L.E. Vitamin D insufficiency is common in Ugandan children and is associated with severe malaria. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e113185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Sudfeld, C.R.; Manji, K.P.; Smith, E.R.; Aboud, S.; Kisenge, R.; Fawzi, W.W.; Duggan, C.P. Vitamin D deficiency is not associated with growth or the incidence of common morbidities among Tanzanian infants. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 2017, 65, 467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Wöbke, T.K.; Sorg, B.L.; Steinhilber, D. Vitamin D in inflammatory diseases. Front. Physiol. 2014, 5, 244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Junaid, K.A.; Fellowes, S. Gender differences in the attainment of motor skills on the movement assessment battery for children. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2006, 26, 5–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Flatters, I.; Hill, L.J.; Williams, J.H.; Barber, S.E.; Mon-Williams, M. Manual control age and sex differences in 4 to 11 year old children. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e88692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Peyre, H.; Hoertel, N.; Bernard, J.Y.; Rouffignac, C.; Forhan, A.; Taine, M.; Heude, B.; Ramus, F.; EDEN Mother–Child Cohort Study Group. Sex differences in psychomotor development during the preschool period: A longitudinal study of the effects of environmental factors and of emotional, behavioral, and social functioning. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2019, 178, 369–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Malina, R.M.; Bouchard, C.; Bar-Or, O. Growth, maturation, and physical activity; Human kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  62. Ndibazza, J.; Mpairwe, H.; Webb, E.L.; Mawa, P.A.; Nampijja, M.; Muhangi, L.; Kihembo, M.; Lule, S.A.; Rutebarika, D.; Apule, B. Impact of anthelminthic treatment in pregnancy and childhood on immunisations, infections and eczema in childhood: a randomised controlled trial. PLoS ONE 2012, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Nampijja, M.; Apule, B.; Lule, S.; Akurut, H.; Muhangi, L.; Webb, E.L.; Lewis, C.; Elliott, A.M.; Alcock, K.J. Effects of maternal worm infections and anthelminthic treatment during pregnancy on infant motor and neurocognitive functioning. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2012, 18, 1019–1030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Table 1. Description of the cognitive and motor measures used in the study.
Table 1. Description of the cognitive and motor measures used in the study.
PCA ComponentsName of TestDomain Description of MeasureAbsolute Scores (Min, Max)
Verbal and non-verbal IQBlock designNon-verbal IQThe measure is adapted from the British Ability Scales-third edition [26]. The child is asked to copy and construct items with wooden blocks following a demonstration by the assessor.0, 16
Picture vocabulary scaleVerbal IQThe measure is adapted from the Kilifi Vocabulary Test [27]. The child is asked to point out and identify items from 24 black and white picture items familiar to them. 0, 24
Executive functionVerbal fluencyWorking memoryThe measure is adapted from the Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment [28]. The child is asked to name items including foods and animals as fast as possible in a minute.*
Picture searchSelective attentionThe measure is adapted from the Sky Search in Tests of Everyday Attention for Children [29]. The child is presented with three A3 sheets each with a target picture on top and about 100 others at the bottom including copies of the target picture. The child is asked to locate as many copies of the target pictures as possible within 10 s.**
Wisconsin card sort testCognitive flexibilityThe measure is adapted from Berg’s card sort test [30]. The child is given four playing cards of different suits and a pack of 12 cards and asked to sort the cards by number (block 1) and suit (block 2).0, 12
Motor functionCoin box Fine motor functionThe measure is adapted from the Kilifi Developmental Inventory [31]. The child is asked to slot coins through a small opening on a coinbox within 20 s in two trials.0, 20 †
Balancing on one legGross motor functionThe measure is adapted from the Movement Assessment Battery for Children [32]. It entails timed attempts (two per leg) of balancing on one leg for one minute.0, 60 †
PCA, principal components analysis; IQ, intelligence quotient; Min, minimum score; Max, maximum score. *One point is awarded for each correct name and a total score is calculated from the total correct names in a minute. **A total score is calculated from the number of target pictures identified within 10 s. † An average score is calculated after timed attempts of the tests. Block design and picture vocabulary tests loaded heavily on verbal and non-verbal IQ while verbal fluency, picture search and Wisconsin card sort tests loaded heavily on executive function. Coinbox and balancing on one leg tests loaded heavily on motor function. The resulting scores were centred on zero with higher scores representing better and lower scores worse development.
Table 2. Baseline characteristics stratified by 25(OH)D levels of 302 Ugandan children included in the analysis and multivariable linear regression results for associations between 25(OH)D and participant characteristics.
Table 2. Baseline characteristics stratified by 25(OH)D levels of 302 Ugandan children included in the analysis and multivariable linear regression results for associations between 25(OH)D and participant characteristics.
CharacteristicsAll Participants 25(OH)D Levels ≤75 nmol/L)
(n = 113)
25(OH)D Levels >75 nmol/L
(n = 189)
25(OH)D Levels
(Per 10 nmol/L)
(n = 295) β
(95% CI) ††
p Value
Age at 25(OH)D measurement in years, (n, %)
110 (3.3)3 (30.0)7 (70.0)0.91 (−0.79, 2.62)0.02 *
2206 (68.2)75 (36.4)131 (63.6)Reference
363 (20.9)24 (38.1)39 (61.9)−0.49 (−1.19, 0.19)
423 (7.6)11 (47.8)12 (52.2)−1.31 (−2.72, 0.11)
Sex (n, %)
Male146 (48.3)56 (38.4)90 (61.6)Reference
Female156 (51.7)57 (36.5)99 (63.5)−0.29 (−0.82, 0.24)0.29
Height-for-age Z-scores (mv = 8)
Normal (>2 SD)193 (65.7)79 (40.9)114 (59.1)Reference
Stunted (<2 SD)101 (34.4)32 (31.7)69 (68.3)0.21 (−0.36, 0.78)0.47
Weight-for-age Z-scores
(mv = 5)
Normal (>2 SD)264 (88.9)103 (39.0)161 (61.0)Reference
Underweight (<2 SD)33 (11.1)8 (24.2)25 (75.8)0.33 (−0.57,1.23)0.47
Weight-for-height Z-scores
(mv = 5)
Normal (>2 SD)283 (95.3)105 (37.1)178 (62.9)Reference
Wasted (<2 SD)14 (4.7)6 (42.9)8 (57.1)−0.50 (−1.84, 0.84)0.46
Helminthic infections between birth and 5 years (n, %)
Negative248 (82.1)96 (38.7)152 (61.3)Reference
Positive54 (17.9)17 (31.5)37 (68.5)0.01 (−0.74, 0.76)0.98
Asymptomatic malaria between birth and 5 years (n, %)
Negative268 (88.7)103 (38.4)165 (61.6)Reference
Positive34 (11.3)10 (29.4)24 (70.6)1.14 (0.27, 2.02)0.01
Malaria episodes between birth and 5 years (n, %)
None 135 (44.7)56 (41.8)79 (58.5)Reference
165 (21.5)24 (36.9)41 (63.1)−0.38 (−1.12, 0.36)
≥2102 (33.8)33 (32.3)69 (67.7)0.25 (−0.38, 0.89)0.48 *
Haemoglobin levels at time of 25(OH)D measurement a (n, %)
(mv = 16)
Normal172 (60.1)62 (36.1)110 (63.9)Reference
Anaemia114 (39.9)44 (38.6)70 (61.4)−0.17 (−0.74, 0.40)0.56
Iron deficiency at time of 25(OH)D measurement b (n, %) (mv = 14)
Normal210 (72.9)76 (36.2)134 (63.8)Reference
Iron deficiency78 (27.1)31 (39.7)47 (60.3)0.03 (−0.60, 0.66)0.93
CRP levels at time of 25(OH)D measurement c (n, %) (mv = 3)
Normal230 (76.9)98 (42.6)132 (57.4)Reference
Inflammation 69 (23.1)15 (21.7)54 (78.3)0.79 (0.14, 1.46)0.02
Randomized treatment of children with albendazole (ABZ) in EMaBS trial
Placebo143 (47.4)53 (37.1)90 (62.9)Reference
ABZ159 (52.7)60 (37.7)99 (62.3)0.13 (−0.41, 0.67)0.63
Maternal age in years at enrolment to EMaBS (n, %)
14–24165 (54.6)63 (38.2)102 (61.8)Reference
25–34110 (36.4)40 (36.4)70 (63.6)−0.09 (−0.77, 0.59)
35+27 (8.9)10 (37.0)17 (63.0)0.69 (−0.54, 1.91)0.55 *
Maternal education at enrolment to EMaBS (n, %) (mv = 1)
Primary/none165 (54.8)66 (40.0)99 (60.0)0.19 (−0.91, 1.29)
Secondary114 (37.9)37 (32.5)77 (67.5)0.46 (−0.64, 1.56)
Tertiary22 (7.3)9 (40.9)13 (59.1)Reference0.77 *
Parity (n, %)
154 (17.9)27 (50.0)27 (50.0)Reference
2–4179 (59.3)61 (34.1)118 (65.9)0.19 (−0.56, 0.94)
5+69 (22.9)25 (36.2)44 (63.8)−0.09 (−1.23, 1.05)0.97 *
Randomized treatment of mothers with ABZ during pregnancy in EMaBS trial (n, %)
Placebo144 (47.7)51 (35.4)93 (64.6)Reference
ABZ158 (552.3)62 (39.2)96 (60.8)−0.02 (−0.56, 0.52)0.94
Randomized treatment of mothers with praziquantel during pregnancy in EMaBS trial (n, %)
Placebo167 (55.3)65 (38.9)102 (61.1)Reference
Praziquantel135 (44.7)48 (35.6)87 (64.4)0.47 (−0.07, 1.00)0.09
Household socioeconomic status recorded at EMaBS enrolment d (n, %) (mv = 5)
1 (lowest)16 (5.4)13 (81.3)3(18.7)−1.49 (−2.92, −0.07)
216 (5.4)6 (37.5)10 (62.5)−1.26 (−2.77, 0.24)
382 (27.6)22 (26.8)60 (73.2)0.33 (−0.71, 1.37)
489 (29.9)30 (33.7)59 (66.3)−0.14 (−1.18, 0.89)
569 (23.2)29 (42.0)40 (58.0)−0.33 (−1.39, 0.72)
6 (highest)25 (8.4)11 (44.0)14 (56.0)Reference0.25 *
Location recorded at EMaBS enrolment (n, %)
Urban118 (39.1)49 (41.5)69 (58.5)Reference
Peri-urban79 (26.2)29 (36.7)50 (63.3)0.36 (−0.33, 1.05)
Rural105 (34.8)35 (33.3)70 (66.7)0.24 (−0.41, 0.89)0.45 *
SD, standard deviation; mv, missing values. a Anaemia as haemoglobin <11 g/dL, adjusted for change in altitude (1000 m above sea level); b iron deficiency as ferritin levels <12 µg/L; and c inflammation as C-reactive protein levels >5 mg/L. d Household socioeconomic status was derived as a composite of building materials of the home, number of rooms and items owned. ††All multivariable models were adjusted for all the other participant characteristics (age at 25(OH)D measurement, sex, iron status, anaemia, inflammation, weight-for-height Z-scores, helminthic infections between birth and 5 years, randomized treatment of children and mothers with albendazole and praziquantel, household socioeconomic status, location, maternal age and education, parity and asymptomatic malaria parasitaemia. Sample size reduced from 302 to 295 children in the final multivariable analyses due to missing values in some of the adjusted variables such as anaemia and iron status. * p value for trend.
Table 3. Multivariable linear regression results for associations between participant characteristics and outcomes.
Table 3. Multivariable linear regression results for associations between participant characteristics and outcomes.
VariablesVerbal and Non-Verbal IQ
(n = 259) β (95% CI)
p ValueExecutive Function
(n = 259) β (95% CI)
p ValueMotor Function
(n = 259) β (95% CI)
p Value
Sex
Male Reference Reference Reference
Female−0.19 (−0.50, 0.13)0.250.42 (0.11, 0.72)0.010.33 (0.05, 0.60)0.02
HAZ
NormalReference Reference Reference
Stunted−0.30 (−0.64, 0.04)0.080.02 (−0.31, 0.35)0.91−0.05 (−0.34, 0.25)0.75
Maternal education
Primary/none−0.46 (−1.08, 0.16) −0.43 (−1.04, 0.17) 0.27 (−0.27, 0.81)
Secondary−0.21 (−0.84, 0.41) −0.18 (−0.79, 0.43) 0.34 (−0.21, 0.88)
TertiaryReference0.07 *Reference0.07 *Reference0.66 *
Household socioeconomic status
1 (Lowest)−0.33 (−1.19, 0.53) −0.04 (−0.88, 0.79) 0.36 (−0.38, 1.11)
20.09 (−0.80, 0.99) −0.04 (−0.91, 0.31) 0.17 (−0.61, 0.94)
3−0.06 (−0.68, 0.57) −0.29 (−0.90, 0.32) 0.37 (−0.17, 0.92)
40.06 (−0.56, 0.69) −0.23 (−0.84, 0.38) 0.42 (−0.12, 0.97)
50.44 (−0.19, 1.08) 0.10 (−0.52, 0.72) 0.39 (−0.16, 0.94)
6 (Highest)Reference0.08 *Reference0.35 *Reference0.66 *
Location
UrbanReference Reference Reference
Peri-urban0.03 (−0.37, 0.43) −0.37 (−0.76, 0.02) −0.08 (−0.42, 0.27)
Rural−0.11 (−0.49, 0.27)0.57 *−0.20 (−0.57, 0.17)0.27 *0.12 (−0.21, 0.45)0.50 *
Randomized treatment of children with albendazole (ABZ) in EMaBS trial
ABZReference Reference Reference
Placebo−0.07 (−0.39, 0.25)0.69−0.13 (−0.45, 0.18)0.40−0.31 (−0.59, −0.04)0.03
CI, confidence interval; IQ, intelligence quotient; HAZ, height-for-age Z-scores. All models are adjusted for age at 25(OH)D measurement, 25(OH)D levels, anaemia, iron status, inflammation and asymptomatic malaria. Sample size reduced from 302 to 259 children in the final multivariable analyses due to missing values in some of the adjusted variables such as anaemia and iron status. * p value for trend.
Table 4. Univariable and multivariable linear regression results for the association between 25(OH)D levels and cognitive and motor outcomes.
Table 4. Univariable and multivariable linear regression results for the association between 25(OH)D levels and cognitive and motor outcomes.
Univariable Model β (95% CI) n= 302p ValueModel 1 * β (95% CI) n = 272p ValueModel 2 ** β (95% CI) n = 259p Value
Verbal and non-verbal IQ
25(OH)D levels (per 10 nmol/L) −0.01 (−0.07, 0.06)0.760.01 (−0.07, 0.08)0.860.01(−0.07, 0.08)0.82
25(OH)D levels >75 nmol/LReference Reference Reference
25(OH)D levels 50–75 nmol/L−0.08 (−0.40, 0.24)0.630.02 (−0.31, 0.35)0.890.04 (−0.30, 0.39)0.80
25(OH)D levels ≤75 nmol/L0.08 (−0.23, 0.39)0.63−0.01 (−0.33, 0.32)0.96−0.05 (−0.38, 0.29)0.78
Executive function
25(OH)D levels (per 10 nmol/L)0.01 (−0.06, 0.07)0.810.03 (−0.04, 0.10)0.370.04 (−0.03, 0.12)0.25
25(OH)D levels >75 nmol/LReference Reference Reference
25(OH)D levels 50–75 nmol/L−0.07 (−0.38, 0.23)0.64−0.06 (−0.38, 0.27)0.73−0.04 (−37, 0.29)0.81
25(OH)D levels ≤75 nmol/L0.08 (−0.22, 0.39)0.580.04 (−0.27, 0.35)0.800.01 (−0.32, 0.34)0.94
Motor function
25(OH)D levels (per 10 nmol/L)0.02 (−0.04, 0.07)0.540.02 (−0.04, 0.08)0.550.02 (−0.04, 0.09)0.52
25(OH)D levels >75 nmol/LReference Reference Reference
25(OH)D levels 50–75 nmol/L0.00 (−0.27, 0.27)0.990.01 (−0.27, 0.29)0.960.01 (−0.29, 0.30)0.97
25(OH)D levels ≤75 nmol/L−0.04 (−0.29, 0.22)0.79−0.06 (−0.33, 0.22)0.68−0.06 (−0.34, 0.23)0.70
CI, confidence interval. * Model 1 was minimally adjusted for age at 25(OH)D measurement, sex, iron status and anaemia. ** Model 2 was adjusted for factors in model 1 plus stunting, inflammation, randomized treatment of children with albendazole, socioeconomic status, maternal education and asymptomatic malaria parasitaemia. Sample size reduced from 302 to 259 children in the final multivariable analyses due to missing values in some of the adjusted variables such as anaemia and iron status. All p values are from the Wald test.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mutua, A.M.; Nampijja, M.; Elliott, A.M.; Pettifor, J.M.; Williams, T.N.; Abubakar, A.; Webb, E.L.; Atkinson, S.H. Vitamin D Status Is Not Associated with Cognitive or Motor Function in Pre-School Ugandan Children. Nutrients 2020, 12, 1662. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12061662

AMA Style

Mutua AM, Nampijja M, Elliott AM, Pettifor JM, Williams TN, Abubakar A, Webb EL, Atkinson SH. Vitamin D Status Is Not Associated with Cognitive or Motor Function in Pre-School Ugandan Children. Nutrients. 2020; 12(6):1662. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12061662

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mutua, Agnes M., Margaret Nampijja, Alison M. Elliott, John M. Pettifor, Thomas N. Williams, Amina Abubakar, Emily L. Webb, and Sarah H. Atkinson. 2020. "Vitamin D Status Is Not Associated with Cognitive or Motor Function in Pre-School Ugandan Children" Nutrients 12, no. 6: 1662. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12061662

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop