Next Article in Journal
The Impact of Transformational Leadership on Service Employees in the Hotel Industry
Next Article in Special Issue
Sleep and Well-Being during the COVID-19 Remote and In-Person Periods: Experiences of College Faculty and Staff with and without Disabilities
Previous Article in Journal
Generating a Moderated Mediation Model of Positive Outcome Expectancy and Aggression
Previous Article in Special Issue
Do Illegitimate Tasks Lead to Work Withdrawal Behavior among Generation Z Employees in China? The Role of Perceived Insider Status and Overqualification
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Does Abusive Supervision Affect Organisational Gossip? Understanding the Mediating Role of the Dark Triad †

by
Fatih Uçan
1,2,* and
Salih Börteçine Avci
1
1
Faculty of Economics and Administrative Sciences, Atatürk University, Erzurum 25050, Turkey
2
Master Araştırma Eğitim ve Danışmanlık Hizmetleri Ltd., Şti., Ata Teknokent, Erzurum 25050, Turkey
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
This paper derived from the doctoral dissertation performed by Fatih Uçan under the supervision of Salih Börteçine Avci.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(9), 730; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13090730
Submission received: 25 June 2023 / Revised: 20 August 2023 / Accepted: 30 August 2023 / Published: 31 August 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Important Perspectives on Workplace Relationships)

Abstract

:
According to the trait activation theory (TAT), personality characteristics are dormant until contextual elements stir them into action. Personality traits are expected to be activated in the context of abusive supervision. From this perspective, our paper examines whether abusive supervision affects organisational gossiping behaviour through the dark triad. To this end, this study examines the mediating effects of the dark triad on the relationship between abusive supervision and organisational gossip based on cross-sectional data gathered from two separate samples. Using the results from structural equation modelling, it is evident that abusive supervision activates the dark triad, and its context influences organisational gossip in line with the TAT. In addition, our results show that abusive supervision positively affects gossip for information gathering and relationship building, with the dark triad proving to be completely mediating. This finding implies that abusive supervision is a contextual factor, and as such, behaviours such as consistent ill treatment and non-violent, verbal or non-verbal hostile acts will have long-term and lasting effects on organisational communication in many organisations. This study offers significant policy implications concerning behavioural issues within education-centred organisations.

1. Introduction

Gossip has received more attention as of late as an inevitable fact and phenomenon within organisational communication networks [1,2,3]. Gossip is often used by employees in the workplace, either as a strategic and political tool or as a means of seeking relief and responding to social injustice [4]. Gossip can affect policies with contradictory roles at the organisational level. In particular, employees gossip during their official duties due to the instrumental ties between them that arise [5] and within private relationships when they are with friends or giving advice to one another [6]. Regarding its negative role, gossip is associated with organisational issues, such as lost reputations, social undermining, decreased productivity, and wasted time [7,8,9].
On the other hand, gossip has positive roles in the organisation, such as information acquisition, relationship building, social enjoyment and group protection, and organisational citizenship behaviours [10,11,12]. Organisations are affected by the positive and negative aspects of gossip, and this effect can play a significant role in determining organisational development and human resource policies. The underlying motives for gossiping behaviour involve many different motives, such as information gathering, relationship building, social enjoyment, and having a negative influence on others [13]. This study focuses on three types of gossip: information gathering, relationship building, and negative influence. Information gathering gossip exchanges are evaluative (positive or negative) information about third parties [12]. Relationship building gossip builds relationships or strengthen relationships between gossipers via the subject of the person who is gossiped about [14]. Negative influence gossip is the influence of others and manipulation of their opinions, typically negatively [15].
Gossip in an organisational setting is determined and sustained by organisational possibilities that regulate and govern the survival of the individual and the group [16]. While most of the current studies in the literature have focused on determining the managerial-level consequences of gossip in organisations [17,18,19], there are few studies on how gossip starts [2]. Most studies that explain the structural antecedents of gossip in organisations and determine the reasons for participating in gossip at the individual level point to the organisational context [7,20]. Thus, the contextual nature of gossip [9] makes it an intriguing phenomenon worth investigating in live situational settings [2]. In particular, an organisational context involving abusive supervision as a stressor [21] provides an opportunity for an in-depth examination of behaviours.
Abusive supervision refers to a perceptual representation of the psychological state induced by a manager in his or her subordinates [22]. Previous research has reported differing views on the effects of abusive supervision on employees’ well-being, attitudes, and behaviour. Some have expressed the adverse effects of abusive supervision on subordinates and the negative reactions of subordinates [23,24,25], while others believe that subordinates are not always negatively affected and will not react negatively [26,27,28]. For this reason, a call for a more balanced perspective through studies examining the differing effects of abusive supervision on subordinates is expressed in the literature [28]. Despite opposing views, studies confirm that subordinates’ reactions to deliberately unexpected behaviour by a supervisor will vary depending on individual and situational characteristics [23,25].
The connection between contextual factors and individual traits is debated through the trait activation theory (TAT). The choice of this theory as the framework for this study stems from its potential to enhance our understanding of how internal psychological mechanisms are triggered when an abusive supervisor creates a hostile environment and puts subordinates in a vulnerable position [29,30]. Tett and Burnett [31] argue that personality traits are only behaviourally active once contextual factors activate them. These factors include daily tasks, social demands from working with supervisors, and organisational culture [32].
Trait activation may occur due to social demands from peers, subordinates, customers, and supervisors [31]. TAT is broad enough to be applied to various personalities, which could fuel more systematic research on person–situation interactionism in predicting behaviours such as organisational gossip [30]. Considering the current debates, personality is seen as a phenomenon that interacts with many other factors that affect human behaviour [33]. Considering that gossip is, to some extent, personality dependent [9,13], personality may explain the psychological mechanism of the relationship between abusive supervision and these types of gossip. Various models for understanding personalities include HEXACO, the Myers–Briggs Type Indicator, the 16 personality factors, and the Big Five personality traits [34]. Moreover, there are three personality traits, Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy, on the dark side of relating, which is an inevitable part of interacting with others [35]. It is reported that individuals with a dark triad are more ready to gossip for their own good, engage in selfish actions, and ignore norms [13]. Furthermore, dark triads (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy) may be activated due to subordinates’ negative emotions. They may seek supportive activities (such as information gathering and relationship building) under the direction of these traits, and gossip can help this process.
Researchers distinguish between intense situations with normative expectations and clear roles that limit behaviour and relaxed situations that allow for more freedom. It has been argued that one’s behaviour in intense situations is based on situational conditions rather than individual personality [13,36]. Therefore, this study included two educational institutions, a public high school and a public university, to explore the relationships between the variables for a deeper understanding. Educators working in public high schools, where quite intense situations are assumed to occur, must adjust their behaviour to comply with the rules and achieve organisational goals. On the other hand, educators at public universities are subject to fewer regulations and guidelines and regulate their conduct based on their own internalized standards [37].
Based on these explanations and to the best of our knowledge, this study investigates the influence of abusive supervision on organisational gossip for the first time and examines the potential mediating role of the dark triad from the perspective of TAT for educational organisations. This study addresses the existing research gap by examining three different types of organisational gossip and making several contributions to the literature. First, this study re-examines the internal mechanism that leads subordinates to gossip when faced with abusive supervision and offers a new perspective for understanding the relationship between the two. Second, this paper employs Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy as mediator variables. When integrated into the model separately, the dark triad supports a deeper understanding of the internal mechanism by mediating the relationship between abusive supervision and organisational gossip. Thirdly, based on trait activation theory, this study provides an in-depth understanding of how subordinates, who use personality traits triggered by negative situations as a tool, resort to organisational gossip to cope with abusive supervision. Thus, it helps to understand how the dark triad of employees acts as a buffer when facing negative situations. Consequently, this research has contributed significantly to the existing literature by thoroughly examining examples in educational organisations.
This paper is structured as follows: Section 2 provides an overview of the conceptual framework and hypothesis relationships. The modelling, data sources, and methods are explained in Section 3. In Section 4, the empirical outcomes and discussion are indicated. The last section evaluates the empirical results and proposes organisational policies for educational organisations.

2. Conceptual Framework and Hypothesized Relationships

2.1. The Relationship between Abusive Supervision and Organisational Gossip

Today’s workplace often faces organisational problems created by corporate officials who exploit and abuse employees or employees who engage in deceptive and sneaky business behaviour [23]. Although different labels such as petty tyranny [38], supervisor aggression [39], and supervisor undermining [40] have been used to explain these behaviours of supervisors, most of the studies conducted today use the term ‘abusive supervision’. It is known that abusive supervisors make derogatory comments towards their subordinates, treat them as if they do not exist, do not share important information, and humiliate them in front of others [40,41]. Tepper [22] defines abusive supervision as “subordinates’ subjective perceptions of the continuity of managers’ non-violent, verbal or non-verbal hostile behavior towards their subordinates”. Ashforth [38], on the other hand, describes abusive supervisors as people who use their power and authority callously and arbitrarily to mistreat employees. Empirical research often examines abuse from the subordinate’s point of view [22,24,38,42]. The current study follows this tradition. Many studies reveal that employees exposed to abusive supervision suffer from this situation which imposes negative costs on organisations [43,44]. For example, employees who display withdrawal behaviours resulting from abusive supervision [27], who decide to leave [45], and who have low job satisfaction [46] have been proven to lead to adverse business outcomes, such as poor job performance [47]. Moreover, managerial hostility that does not include physical violence also reduces the quality of one’s work life [39].
Although the idea that abusive supervision has many destructive effects is generally prevalent, some studies indicate its possible positive effects. It is claimed that especially when employees encounter abusive supervision, they show constructive reactions, such as problem solving [23]. On the other hand, the study of Zhang and Liu [28] claimed that abusive supervision in the cultural context of the Asia-Pacific region, unlike the West, may be positively related to subordinates’ experiences of anger and promotion-oriented work efforts. In other words, subordinates can protect themselves from the potential effects of abusive supervision by adopting positive rather than negative coping strategies [28]. In line with the Yerkes–Dodson law, studies have shown that individuals can increase their problem-solving and risk-taking behaviour and adapt their decision making when faced with a stressful context [48,49]. While the idea of there being many adverse effects is generally prevalent, a more balanced view of the impact associated with abusive supervision is needed when considering possible positive effects [28]. The best employee behaviour for which the positive and negative effects of abusive supervision can be observed is gossip [17,18,19].
Gossip can be described as sharing information that includes judgments about absent third parties [12]. According to Rosnow [50], gossip is an instrumental process used by individuals to talk with others for various purposes, such as gaining status, amusement, financial gain, acquiring information, or fulfilling specific preconceived desires and anticipations. When we look at why employees gossip, we come across reasons such as facilitating the information-sharing process, creating a social environment, gaining coercive power, creating power dynamics within organisations, and controlling people in the workplace [17]. It is argued that motivations such as verifying information, gathering information, building relationships, and negatively influencing others underlie gossiping [13]. These motives are also validated in terms of their functions at the organisational level [51]. The first two are information gathering [5,16,52,53] and relationship building [11,53,54,55], which are also considered positive gossip types. The third is having a negative influence on others, which we can consider as a negative gossip dimension [9,12,55].
In our literature review, only some studies have examined the relationship between abusive management and organisational gossip. Decoster, Camps, Stouten, Vandevyvere, and Tripp [26], who found that abusive management predicts organisational gossip, emphasize in their study that employees exposed to abusive management tend to gossip about their perceived harmony with their colleagues and their supervisors. Interestingly, employees confronted with an abusive supervisor exhibit a stronger rapport and less gossip when they identify more strongly with their organisation [26]. Naeem, Weng, Ali, and Hameed [17] researched the effect of perceived subordinates, negative workplace gossip, and supervisors’ negative emotions on abusive supervision in China. Their findings corroborated the association between negative workplace gossip and abusive supervision. Ye, He, and Sun [18] examined the impact of subordinates’ negative workplace gossip on abusive supervision in China. Their hierarchical regression analysis results confirmed that subordinates’ perceived negative workplace gossip is positively related to abusive supervision. In two studies, Ahmad, et al. [56] investigated subordinates’ gossiping behaviour, thoughts of revenge, and abusive supervision. Their outcomes showed that subordinates’ gossiping leads to increased abusive supervision. The existing literature primarily comprises studies on abusive supervision, and its reported effects on subordinates’ gossip have been both positive and negative.
On the other hand, Beersma, Van Kleef, and Dijkstra [10] reported that when people are in danger from norm violations and have the opportunity to discuss them with a member of the same group, they use gossip to gather and verify information and to protect the group against norm violations [10]. The absence of studies examining the potential implications of abusive supervision for types of organisational gossip has created a research opportunity. Given the utilitarian purpose of gossip, we suggest that when faced with aversive actions from a supervisor, subordinates may react with either positive or negative types of gossip to deal with abusive supervision. As a result, we expect the following:
Hypothesis 1a:
Abusive supervision positively relates to information gathering gossip.
Hypothesis 1b:
Abusive supervision positively relates to relationship building gossip.
Hypothesis 1c:
Abusive supervision negatively relates to negative influence gossip.

2.2. The Role of Subordinates’ Dark Triad Personality Traits Based on Trait Activation Theory

Understanding what triggers personality traits, described as the latent potential found in an individual, is crucial to understanding the role of personality in the workplace [31]. Janowski and Szczepańska-Przekota [34] suggested that the personality issue extends throughout all organisational operations involving human resources, representing a distinct form of individual drive within a company. Personality traits, defined as dominant constructs in psychology, require certain situations to become activated and direct behavioural responses [57]. Trait activation theory (TAT) formalizes the trait–state relationship by recognizing that the behavioural expression of a trait requires the constant stimulation of that trait by relevant situational cues [57]. This study argues that personality traits, such as the dark triad, can best explain how employees exposed to maladministration perceive this situation and the degree of its reflection on their behaviour [13] and suggests the conceptual model in Figure 1. Although it shares a common core with callous manipulation [58] and is considered socially undesirable, the dark triad differs and has distinct defining features [58,59]. Although these traits deserve to be clustered, their correlations are typically relatively modest, so each can be viewed as a particular aspect of socially aversive behaviour [60]. Narcissists believe they have the right to exploit others for their own benefit [61,62]. Machiavellians are characterized by apathy, the strive for Argentine goals (i.e., money, power, and status), calculating, cunning tactics of manipulation, and a cold lack of emotion and ethical concern [63]. Machiavellians maximize their self-interest through deception and disregard for others [64]. At the subclinical level, psychopaths are impulsive, seek excitement, lack empathy, lead an irregular lifestyle, and are likely to exhibit anti-social behaviours [65]. In summary, ego identity goals drive narcissistic behaviour, instrumental goals drive Machiavellian and psychopathic behaviour, and all three have an emotionless core that encourages interpersonal manipulation [66].
Traits constitute behavioural probabilities that can be explained according to situational demands [67,68]. The principle of trait activation theory also holds that personality traits are expressed as responses to trait-relevant situational cues [31]. Trait activation becomes the process of individuals expressing their traits when particularly relevant situational cues are presented. For example, take a situation in which someone shouts for help when they see that someone else needs help. This situation indicates that the person has the trait of compassion. Otherwise, unresponsiveness means low compassion [31]. Considering that abusive supervisors make derogatory comments towards their subordinates, pretend they do not exist, do not share important information, and humiliate them in front of others [40,41], not every subordinate exposed to this situation will give the same reaction. For example, an employee with high honesty (i.e., low on Machiavellianism and psychopathy) will not perceive embezzlement as an option, regardless of need or opportunity [68]. Lyons and Hughes [69] explored the possible effects of narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism on the multiple functions of gossip. In their study, psychopathy and narcissism were associated with social pleasure, group protection, and negative influence gossip; Machiavellianism was found to be associated with negative influence gossip. The assertive and belligerent nature of an abusive supervisor can hinder employee status/career development by making employees feel stupid and humiliating them [22]. Abusive supervisors can mobilize narcissists’ distrust of others. One study found that narcissists who felt threatened (compared to those who were not) responded by expressing a significantly negative view of the personality of others [61]. Whereas charismatic and superior narcissists believe other people should be interested in their work [70].
Due to their position in the hierarchy, managers can create situations that will affect employees positively or negatively [71]. Therefore, some factors may cause the abuse of employees who encounter abusive supervision, while others may suppress this behaviour. Gossip is significantly linked to an individual’s personality and the context in which this information exchange occurs [9]. The literature remains silent on whether the characteristics of subordinates who encounter abusive supervision are predisposed to similar gossip behaviours. However, Tepper [25] claims in his conceptual model that the characteristics and behaviours of employees have a regulatory effect on the adverse effects of abusive supervision on employees. In other words, Tepper’s conceptual model reflects that abusive supervision does not affect all subordinates similarly. Based on these explanations, the employees’ personality traits may mediate the relationship between abusive supervision and organisational gossip since not every employee encountering abusive supervision will react similarly in the study. In that sense, this study explores the following hypotheses.
Hypothesis 2a:
Narcissism positively mediates the relationship between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip.
Hypothesis 2b:
Narcissism positively mediates the relationship between abusive supervision and relationship building gossip.
Hypothesis 2c:
Narcissism negatively mediates the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip.
The unethical nature of Machiavellianism is revealed when there are clues about traits [30]. According to the TAT, the expression of Machiavellian behaviour depends on contextual cues from the psychological work environment. Specifically, it is claimed to be activated by situational cues that are activated or align with the exploitative nature of Machiavellians [72]. Therefore, the impulse control of the Machiavellian is triggered when malicious leadership, such as abusive supervision, creates a risky situation [68,70]. Greenbaum, Hill, Mawritz, and Quade [30] found that abusive supervisors strengthen the relationship between Machiavellianism and unethical behaviours by giving clues that activate the Machiavellian employee’s distrust of others, immoral manipulation, and the desire for control and status. As a result, we would expect the following:
Hypothesis 3a:
Machiavellianism positively mediates the relationship between abusive supervision information gathering gossip.
Hypothesis 3b:
Machiavellianism positively mediates the relationship between abusive supervision and gossip relationship building gossip.
Hypothesis 3c:
Machiavellianism negatively mediates the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip.
Like other dark triad traits, psychopathy is critical in explaining how traits are activated against situations [73]. It is claimed that psychopathy is not taxonomic (i.e., present or absent) but dimensional (i.e., it varies in degree among individuals) [29]. In contrast to narcissists, individuals with high psychopathic tendencies are less sensitive to risk and related losses [70]. Abusive supervisors can create external, powerful threats that psychopaths cannot directly control. In particular, managers who threaten the interests/expectations of their subordinates will activate the psychopathic features of their employees [29]. When dissatisfied with the situation created by the leader, followers with higher psychopathic traits may have the opportunity to break the rules and question the status quo [74]. As a result, we would expect the following:
Hypothesis 4a:
Psychopathy positively mediates the relationship between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip.
Hypothesis 4b:
Psychopathy positively mediates the relationship between abusive supervision and relationship building gossip.
Hypothesis 4c:
Psychopathy negatively mediates the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Study Design, Participants, and Procedures

This study, which we conducted based on the exploratory research approach, is deductive research in which primary data are used with a quantitative research design. A structured questionnaire covering 68 variables (79 with demographic variables) was used to analyse cross-sectional quantitative data collected simultaneously. Questionnaire can be seen in the Appendix A. Pen-and-paper questionnaires were distributed face-to-face to the individuals during working hours by a member of the research team between February and May 2021. The ethics committee report was shown to all participants in advance, and it was assured that their answers would be evaluated anonymously. The researcher returned to collect the questionnaires two days after the day he distributed them.
Data were collected through stratified random sampling (SRS), a probability sampling method. SRS is a modification of random sampling in which the target population is divided into two or more relevant strata based on one or more attributes [75]. To achieve greater generalizability and to present a constructive replication of the findings [76], the hypotheses were tested with two different samples: (a) teachers working in public high schools and (b) academic staff working at a public university. We focused on educators since a single director generally governs educational organisations, plans with a distinct goal in mind, is limited to a certain period, has a clearly outlined and organised syllabus which is given by specially qualified staff, and follows rigorous disciplinary standards, unlike other organisations. Thus, it provides us with the opportunity to examine a context created by abusive administration more accurately. To avoid having multiple respondents from the same school/faculty, we asked for an equal number of respondents from each to participate in the survey.
Moreover, collecting data from multiple sources at multiple points in the same period reduces concerns that our findings are affected by common method bias. This multigroup SEM aims to determine whether the study is acceptable for the two proposed cultural samples. To clarify whether the type of gossip is related to the personality of the gossiper, a sample from public schools and a second sample from a public university were used. Respondents did not receive any incentives or compensation for their participation in the survey. The research procedure was applied equally for both samples.
(S1) Sample 1. S1 is composed of 470 teachers working in high schools operating under the Ministry of National Education affiliated with the central government in Turkey. The sample included 261 women and 209 men with an average age of 34.9 (SD = 0.81) years. Three hundred forty-two of the participants (72.8%) were married. The participants had been working for their current organisation for 5.5 (SD = 1.37) years. In regards to the participants’ workloads according to their statements, eighty-five (18.1%) were very busy, 136 (28.9%) were busy, 222 (47.2%) were moderately busy, 22 (4.7%) were not very busy, and 5 (1.1%) were not at all busy.
(S2) Sample 2. The participants in S2 were 990 academic staff from a public university operating in the same region. The respondents were primarily male (n = 668, 67.5%) with an average age of 43.2 years (SD = 0.99). Seven hundred and five of the participants (71.2%) were married. The participants had been working in the department for an average of 9.2 (SD = 1.58) years. In regards to the participants’ workloads according to their statements, two hundred seventy-eight (28.1%) were very busy, 325 (32.8%) were busy, 351 (35.5%) were moderately busy, 29 (2.9%) were not very busy, and 7 (0.7%) were not at all busy. As a result, a total of 1460 responses were obtained from two samples for this study.

3.2. Measures

Constructs were measured using multi-item scales from existing studies whenever possible. However, some items have been slightly modified to fit the research context. All items were on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree, and the scales were used similarly in both samples.
Abusive Supervision: A 15-item version of the scale of abusive supervision perception by Tepper [22] was used for the present study. The sample item for the scale is “Supervisor ridicules me”. The reliabilities assessed by Cronbach’s alpha values indicated good and acceptable reliabilities, respectively (Sample 1 α = 0.93 and Sample 2 α = 0.95).
Dark Triad: The scale of Jonason and Webster [77] was used to measure the dark triad. The scale consists of three sub-dimensions, namely narcissism (sample item: I tend to want others to pay attention to me), Machiavellianism (sample item: I tend to manipulate others to get my way), and psychopathy (sample item: I tend to lack remorse). The Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients for the overall scale were α = 0.86 (Sample 1) and α = 0.84 (Sample 2).
Organisational Gossip: A 24-item revised version of the organisational gossip scale developed by Han and Dağlı [51] was used. The scale consists of three sub-dimensions. They are distributed in the following manner: information gathering (sample item: I learn many things about my colleagues at my organisation through gossip), relationship building (sample item: Gossiping with my colleagues at my organisation increases our sincerity), and negative influence (sample item: The gossip in my organisation causes disagreements among us). The reliability of the overall scale was good: α = 0.89 (Sample 1); and α = 0.84 (Sample 2).

3.3. Control Variables

Individual differences are reported to predict attitudes towards gossip and the tendency to gossip [78]. In particular, age and gender could predict organisational gossip [9,79,80], and were controlled in the tests conducted with both samples. The correlation matrix of means, standard deviations, and scale reliability for all variables is given in Table 1 and Table 2.

3.4. Data Analysis

We tested our hypotheses using a structural equation modelling (SEM) framework with a mediation model. The same analyses were conducted for both samples [81,82]. Following the two-stage approach of Anderson and Gerbing [83], first, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to confirm the fit of the measurement model to the data. Then regression analysis and path analysis were performed on the structural model. Descriptive analyses were performed to examine the preliminary correlations between the main variables. The study also measured Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega. The mediating effects of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy were tested by the bootstrapping procedure [84] with 5000 sampling to generate 95% confidence intervals. Mediation effect analysis is proposed to determine whether a mediation effect exists and the type of mediation’s corresponding parameters which refer to influence affect value. The mediating effect is considered statistically significant when the bootstrap 95% confidence interval does not include 0. The goodness of fit of the measurement and structural model was evaluated using the following indicators: chi-square/df, goodness-of-fit index (GFI), normed fit index (NFI), incremental fit index (IFI), comparative fit index (CFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) [85]. Additionally, we checked for two types of error: incorrect sign (Type S) and incorrect magnitude (Type M) for all hypotheses. Gelman and Carlin [86] provide a simple function to estimate these errors in their paper called retro design. The Type S error rate is “the probability that the replicated estimate has the incorrect sign if it is statistically significantly different from zero”. The exaggeration ratio (expected Type-M error) is “the expectation of the absolute value of the estimate divided by the effect size, if statistically significantly different from zero” [86]. The noisier or more variable the data, the less one should be confident about any inferences based on statistical significance, as illustrated with the Type-S (size) and Type-M (magnitude) errors [86,87,88].

3.5. Construct Diagnostics

In both samples, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) showed an acceptable measurement pattern (χ2/df = 2.13 (S1); 3.11 (S2), GFI = 0.86 (S1); 0.85 (S2), NFI = 0.89 (S1); 0.88 (S2):, IFI = 0.91 (S1); 0.91 (S2), CFI = 0.90 (S1); 0.91 (S2), RMSEA = 0.049 (S1); 0.046 (S2). The χ2 statistics were significant, and all fit indices were within the recommended acceptable ranges [89]. All standardized first-order loadings were positive and statistically significant, suggesting that the factors were well-defined in both samples.
The measurement model generally has two validity and two reliability measures. Convergent validity and discriminant validity are used for validity. In contrast, composite reliability, Cronbach’s alpha [90], and McDonald’s omega assess internal consistency, for which values equal to or higher than 0.700 were considered good [91]. Convergent validity refers to the degree to which multiple measures of a construct that should theoretically be related are related [92]. Convergent validity is assessed using average variance extracted (AVE), which shows how much of the variance of the indicators can be explained by the latent unobserved variable. All factor loading values are in the range of 0.625–0.878 (S1) and 0.605:0.872 (S2); therefore, factor loadings for both samples exceed the threshold limit of 0.6. AVE values are 0.511–0.665 (S1) and 0.568:0.689 (S2), which are higher than the recommended cut-off value of 0.5 [93]. These results show that the model has good convergent validity for both samples. Discriminant or divergent validity indicates how much a particular construct differs from others. It refers to how different the metrics, which should not be highly correlated, actually are [83].
The discriminant validity is measured by the Fornell and Larcker ratio criterion (Table 3) and the Heterotrait–Monotrait (HTMT) (Table 4) correlation ratio. The Fornell and Larcker ratio criterion measures discriminant validity in which all diagonal values in the table must be greater than the underlying values. HTMT technique examines the ratio of between-trait correlations to within-trait correlations to the correlations of indicators within a construct [94]. Table 2 shows that all values written in bold on the diagonal are more significant than those below. This result makes the Fornell and Larcker ratio criteria results appropriate. Additionally, the second measure to estimate discriminant validity is the HTMT ratio, for which all values must be less than 0.90. The table shows that all values are less than the threshold point. Thus, the discriminant validity condition was also met.
When the reliability results were evaluated, the composite reliability (CR) values calculated according to the factor loads were 0.79–0.95 (S1); they varied between 0.76 and 0.96 (S2). The Cronbach’s alpha values and McDonald’s omega values were greater than 0.7, which is similarly acceptable [91,95]. According to these results, reliability was confirmed.

3.6. Common Method Variance

Common method variance, defined as artificial correlation [96] among constructs attributable to the measurement method, can distort survey-based results (bias). We investigated common method bias using Harman’s single-factor test and used an unrotated principal component analysis with varimax rotation [96]. It was seen that each item was loaded on its theoretical structure and hidden common method factor. Additionally, the mean variance extracted by the common method factor was 0.27, well below the 0.50 threshold that [97] associated with an independent construct. Therefore, common method bias is not overly problematic for this study.

4. Results

The descriptive analysis results and correlations of abusive supervision, narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy, information gathering gossip, relationship building gossip, and negative influence gossip are presented in Table 1 and Table 2. Abusive supervision was found to be significantly positively related to narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy, information gathering gossip, and relationship building gossip but was significantly negatively related to negative influence gossip. Meanwhile, narcissism was positively and significantly related to Machiavellianism, psychopathy, information gathering gossip, relationship building gossip, and negative influence gossip. Machiavellianism was significantly and positively related to psychopathy, information gathering gossip, and relationship building gossip but negatively related to negative influence gossip. Psychopathy was positively related to information gathering gossip and relationship building gossip but negatively related to negative influence gossip.
The complete structural model shown in Figure 1 was evaluated separately for both samples. A mediation analysis explored how abusive supervision is related to information gathering, relationship building, and negative influence gossip. The bootstrapping method was used to test the existence of mediating effects, as recommended by Hayes [98]. Bootstrapping is used in mediational analyses to generate an empirically derived representation of the sampling distribution of the indirect effect and then bootstrapped confidence intervals. The results of the 5000-bootstrap sample at the 95% confidence interval are shown in Table 5, Table 6 and Table 7. If the 95% confidence limits include zero, the indirect effect test is insignificant at the 95% confidence interval. Table 5, Table 6 and Table 7 present the results of the mediation models used to test the hypotheses. These three tables examine the direct and indirect effects of information gathering, relationship building, and negative influence gossip, which are the types of organisational gossip. As they represent different types of gossip, the hypothesis tests for each type of organisational gossip were analysed separately. Using the obtained effect size estimate and the standard error for all hypothesis relationships, we calculate the Type-S and Type-M errors with the help of Rstudio (alpha = 0.05 is the default significance level, and n.sims = 10,000 is the default number of simulations). As Gelman and Carlin [86] point out, we ensured the Type-S (size) error rate was less than 0.5, and the Type-M (magnitude) error rate was greater than 1 in each of our analyses.

4.1. Mediated Regression Results for Information Gathering Gossip

A mediation analysis explored how abusive supervision is related to information gathering gossip. First, the results showed that the model adequately fit the data for both samples (S1: χ2/df = 2.16 GFI = 0.880; NFI = 0.940; CFI = 0.950; RMSEA = 0.50)–(S2: χ2/df = 3.38; GFI = 0.885; NFI = 0.901; CFI = 0.951 RMSEA = 0.049). Second, this study employed p-values, beta values, confidence intervals, and Type-S and Type-M errors to confirm the statistical significance and the direction were positive, and the results for the structural model assessment are illustrated in Table 5. H1a was tested and confirmed as there was a positive and significant association between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip. As shown in Table 5, the direct effect of abusive supervision on information gathering gossip was not statistically significant for sample 1 (β = 0.085, p = 0.086, 95% CI [−0.013, 0.187] S/M = 0.144/6.264) and there was a significant effect for sample 2 (β = 0.111, p= 0.001, 95% CI [0.041, 0.180]; S/M = 0.042/3.625).
Three indirect effects (mediating variables) were introduced and are proposed in Table 5. All of these three mediating roles were accepted for both samples. Hypotheses H2a, H3a, and H4a confirmed that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy positively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip. Narcissism significantly and positively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip for both samples (S1: β = 0.076, p = 0.014, 95% CI [0.014, 0.180] S/M = 0.102/5.192; S2: β = 0.047, p = 0.001, 95% CI [0.021, 0.082] S/M = 0.001/1.919). Machiavellianism significantly and positively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip for both samples (S1: β = 0.074, p = 0.050, 95% CI [0.002, 0.204] S/M = 0.048/3.783; S2: β = 0.036, p = 0.001, 95% CI [0.014, 0.071] S/M = 0.006/2.348) Psychopathy significantly and positively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip for both samples (S1: β = 0.186, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.070, 0.300] S/M = 0.139/6.077; S2: β = 0.052, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.019, 0.110] S/M = 0.299/12.899). As a result, the higher the level of abusive supervision, the higher the level of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy, leading to better information gathering gossip. Therefore, an indirect effect test indicated that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy (fully mediated in sample 1 and partially mediated in sample 2) mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip. The effects of the control variables are shown in Table 5. The direct effect of the gender and age of the subordinates on information gathering gossip was not statistically significant for both samples. Furthermore, the Type-S rates, which indicate that, based on our sample size, the risk that the sign indicates the effect we observe is incorrect, is low, and the Type-M rates, which indicate that, based on our sample size, there is not any risk that we are overestimating the magnitude of the significant effect uncovered for all significant hypotheses.

4.2. Mediated Regression Results for Relationship Building Gossip

The model fit values showed an acceptable fit in Table 6, which showed the situation in which relationship building gossip is the dependent variable (S1: χ2/df = 2.18; GFI = 0.858; NFI= 0.941; CFI = 0.953 RMSEA = 0.050), (S2: χ2/df = 3.41; GFI = 0.887; NFI= 0.912; CFI = 0.967 RMSEA = 0.049). The H1b is tested and confirmed that a positive and significant association between abusive supervision and relationship building gossip exists. The results are presented in Table 6. The direct effect of abusive supervision on relationship building gossip was not statistically significant for both samples (S1:β = 0.055, p = 0.255, 95% CI [−0.050, 0.167] S/M = 0.186/7.418; S2:β = 0.039, p = 0.241, 95% CI [−0.031, 0.112] S/M = 0.039/3.567).
Hypotheses H2b, H3b, and H4b confirmed that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy positively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and relationship building gossip. Narcissism significantly and positively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and relationship building gossip for both samples (S1: β = 0.069, p = 0.011, 95% CI [0.015, 0.161] S/M = 0.069/4.341; S2: β = 0.027, p = 0.001, 95% CI [0.011, 0.051] S/M = 0.003/1.969). Machiavellianism significantly and positively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and relationship building gossip for both samples (S1: β = 0.101, p = 0.012, 95% CI [0.012, 0.249] S/M = 0.034/3.416; S2: β = 0.065, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.036, 0.108] S/M = 0.001/1.068). Psychopathy significantly and positively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and relationship building gossip for both samples (S1: β = 0.149, p = 0.001, 95% CI [0.052, 0.313] S/M = 0.151/6.440; S2: β = 0.088, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.044, 0.154] S/M = 0.010/1.797). The higher the level of abusive supervision, the higher the level of the dark triad, leading to better relationship building gossip. Therefore, an indirect effect test indicated that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy fully mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and relationship building gossip for both samples. The effects of the control variables are shown in Table 6. The direct effect of the gender and age of the subordinates on relationship building gossip was not statistically significant for both samples. The Type-S rates are low, indicating there is a low risk that the sign of the effect we observed is incorrect, and the Type-M rates indicate that there is no risk that we are overestimating the magnitude of the significant effect uncovered for all significant hypotheses.

4.3. Mediated Regression Model for Negative Influence Gossip

A mediation analysis was conducted to explore how abusive supervision is related to negative influence gossip. First, the results showed that the model adequately fit the data for both samples (S1: χ2/df = 2.06; GFI = 0.857; NFI= 0.942; CFI = 0.952 RMSEA = 0.048)–(S2: χ2/df = 3.14; GFI = 0.887; NFI= 0.890; CFI = 0.922 RMSEA = 0.047). Our second hypothesis, Hypothesis 1c, predicted that abusive supervision is negatively related to negative influence gossip. An examination of Table 7 reveals that we did not find statistically significant support for Hypothesis 1c; abusive supervision was not positively related to negative influence gossip in either of the samples (S1: β = −0.051, p = 0.339, 95% CI [−0.150, 0.055] S/M = 0.474/109.339; S2: β = 0.001, p= 0.985, 95% CI [−0.073, 0.072] S/M= 0.373/21.564).
Hypotheses H2c, H3c, and H4c confirmed that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy negatively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip. Contrary to our expectations, narcissism significantly and positively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip for sample 1 (S1: β = 0.053, p = 0.011, 95% CI [0.009, 0.146] S/M = 0.004/2.157). However, there was no significant indirect effect for sample 2 (S2: β = 0.009, p = 0.230, 95% CI [−0.006, 0.031] S/M = 0.015/2.764). Machiavellianism significantly and negatively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip for both samples (S1: β = −0.106, p = 0.016, 95% CI [−0.018, −0.261] S/M = 0.479/132.381; S2: β = −0.036, p = 0.003, 95% CI [−0.072, −0.011] S/M = 0.281/11.756). The higher the level of abusive supervision, the higher the level of Machiavellianism, leading to more negative influence gossip. Psychopathy significantly and negatively mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip for sample 2 (β = −0.035, p = 0.003, 95% CI [−0.081, −0.011] S/M = 0.328/15.352). However, there was no significant indirect effect for sample 1 (β = −0.054, p = 0.263, 95% CI [−0.233, 0.045] S/M = 0.328/15.352). Therefore, an indirect effect test indicated that the dark triad fully mediated the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip for sample 1. The effects of the control variables are shown in Table 7. The direct effect of the gender of the subordinates on the negative influence of gossip was not statistically significant for either of the samples. The direct effect of age on the negative influence of gossip was statistically significant for sample 2 but not for sample 1. Furthermore, the risk that the sign of the effect we observed is incorrect is low, and there is not any risk that we are overestimating the magnitude of the significant effect uncovered for all significant hypotheses.

5. Discussion

Based on trait activation theory, the current study deepened our understanding of the role of trait factors (i.e., the dark triad) in organisational gossip by clarifying how abusive supervision is associated with information gathering, developing relations, and negative influence gossip. While many studies have investigated the effect of gossip on abusive supervision [17,18,28,56], the current study is the first to examine whether abusive supervision affects organisational gossip types and the mediating effect of dark triad characteristics on this relationship. A total of 470 participants from sample 1 and 990 from sample 2 within educational organisations were investigated for our empirical analysis of the proposed hypotheses, and our results demonstrate the following. (1a) Abusive supervision favourably influences information gathering gossip for both studies. (2a, 3a, 4a) Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy play a mediating role in the relationship between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip for both studies. (1b) The direct effect of abusive supervision on relationship building gossip is not statistically significant for both studies. (2b, 3b, 4b) Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy play a mediating role in the relationship between abusive supervision and relationship building gossip. (3a) The direct effect of abusive supervision on negative influence gossip is not statistically significant for either of the studies. (2c) Narcissism is a mediating factor in the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip for sample 1. However, it is not significant for sample 2. (3c) Machiavellianism is a mediating factor in the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip for both studies. (4c) Psychopathy plays a mediating role in the relationship between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip for sample 2 but is not significant for sample 1.
To begin with, the findings of this study indicated a low occurrence of abusive supervision in both samples. One potential explanation for this trend is the study’s location in Turkey, a country with a high power distance score (66). In such an environment, hierarchical superiors, often perceived as paternal figures, tend to be distant and inaccessible [99]. High power distance, one of the distinctive cultural values in Turkey, indicates that employees there might respond differently to malicious supervision compared to those in countries with low power distance. Thus, the present study allowed for a richer assessment of abusive supervision that takes cultural contexts into account [28].
The results indicate that abusive supervision has a significant positive effect on information gathering gossip. This finding contradicts the assumption that “reactions to interpersonal threats are always negative” [100]. Given that gossip often serves as an information gathering tool, subordinates may engage in gossip to gain insight into navigating and mitigating exposure to a supervisor’s abusive behaviour or how to respond to such attitudes [50]. Similarly, studies that posit employees who encounter abusive supervision will gossip to seek information [55,101] align with this finding. However, it is worth noting that the direct effects of abusive supervision on relationship building gossip and negative influence gossip are not statistically significant. These findings contrast with those of previous studies [17,18,26].
Our study contributes to the development of trait activation theory (TAT). TAT posits that specific work-related factors influence an individual’s psychological tendencies and that the influence of personality traits may vary depending on the context [57]. These factors include aspects such as the task itself (e.g., daily tasks, responsibilities, and procedures), social dynamics (e.g., social demands arising from interactions with peers, supervisors, subordinates, and customers) and organisational factors (e.g., organisational culture, climate, and structure) [32]. This theory furthers our knowledge of how abusive supervision affects the types of organisational gossip and the conditions that activate employees’ dark triad features. This study also demonstrates the significance of the dark triad in the relationship between supervisors and employees. The results indicate that abusive supervision influences the tendencies of organisational gossip, mediated by the presence of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. This result is consistent with Chaby, Sheriff, Hirrlinger, and Braithwaite [49]’s findings and the Yerkes–Dodson law. Chaby et al. [49] highlight the importance of context in behaviour and performance, stating that in the case of fear conditioning, behaviour changes according to the Yerkes–Dodson law. Considering that an abusive supervisor creates a threatening environment and risky situation for subordinates [25], there are social conditions (such as abusive supervision) in which the dark triad can be activated. Exposure to workplace mismanagement predisposes subordinates to seek solutions to get them out of the situation and to develop complicated and hostile personalities that reflect a faster coping strategy (such as gossip). The study also examines the factors that activate employees’ personality traits, starting with the conditions created by abusive management, and makes a significant contribution to research on employees’ gossiping behaviour.
Michelson and Mouly [9] claim that few published studies reveal personality-related factors that affect the frequency or nature of gossip. There are different reasons why people may be motivated to engage in information gathering, relationship building, and negative influence gossip [12,17,69,102]. Firstly, narcissists, who often have an extreme love of self, a grandiose sense of self-importance, and a powerful sense of entitlement, are motivated to maintain their identity and legitimacy [103]. Consistent with Pertuz-Peralta et al. [104]’s findings, this study is quite interesting as it shows that narcissistic subordinates activated by abusive supervision, characterized by a sense of greatness and need for admiration, tend to gossip, which will increase information sharing with other subordinates. Characterized by amoral manipulation, a distrust of others, the desire for control, and the desire for status, Machiavellians may perceive they are threatened by others in their pursuit of maximizing their interests. A strong position would be a good option [105]. Our findings suggest that, unlike the findings of Liu [106], Machiavellians turn to gossip to gather information when faced with abusive supervision. Although there is a consensus that psychopathy is an underlying factor in deviant interpersonal behaviours that cause distress for co-workers [107], results show that psychopathy explains the relationship between abusive supervision and information gathering gossip. People high in psychopathy generally resist stress, including interpersonal abuse, and seem to need fewer positive relationships than others [108].
Our findings suggest that subordinates tend to be more impulsive, callous, and manipulative when exposed to a supervisor’s behaviour. Therefore, our findings are consistent with those of Jones and Paulhus [109], who stated that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy traits are associated with exploitative interpersonal behaviour. Insensitive to the concerns and social restrictions of others, narcissists carry out most of their identity-targeting efforts in the social sphere [61]. Our mediation analysis supports those with the narcissistic personality trait facing abusive supervision will strive to maintain a positive self-image at all costs. The findings of Smalley and Stake [110] show that they suppress hostility by showing positive gossip tendencies (relationship building), as opposed to the finding that narcissism is associated with hostility versus humiliation and hostility. Consequently, our study demonstrates that narcissists can utilize the impact of abusive control to legitimize organisational gossip mechanisms.
Inconsistent previous studies [30,32] showed that Machiavellians are more willing to behave socially when they experience abusive supervisor behaviour. Machiavellians, who are more manipulative towards others, may view stressful situations as potentially harmful, believing they can exploit them if they cannot manipulate others [111]. Our study shows that abusive supervision can easily provoke Machiavellianism. This finding may be associated with Machiavellians being more willing to provide support and learn. As a result, when faced with a difficult situation, Machiavellians are willing to gather information to manipulate others or provide social support to meet their interests. The finding that psychopathy mediates the relationship between abusive supervision and developing relations gossip confirms that people with psychopathic tendencies can better cope with stressful situations [112]. Moreover, findings suggest that individuals with high levels of psychopathy remain unaffected by abusive behaviours.
We should note the unexpected results produced for narcissism and psychopathy’s mediating role. Interestingly, our results showed a significant difference in the mediated role of narcissism and psychopathy between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip for both studies. Narcissism was an intermediary between abusive supervision and negative influence gossip for sample 1, but not for sample 2. Psychopathy was an intermediary between abusive supervision and negative influence for sample 2, not for sample 1. The differences in our findings across sample 1 and sample 2 may be due to differences in organisational cultures and differences in the operationalization of gossip behaviours. This finding supported that the logic that aversive behaviour, narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy are related to bad things. The finding that Machiavellianism has a negative mediating effect for negative influence gossip supports the explanation that Machiavellians exhibit corporate citizenship behaviours that can lead to positive social impressions [113]. Another possible reason is that as they identify with their organisation; employees with a stronger sense of harmony with their workgroup buffer their tendency to gossip about their leaders negatively to protect the organisation’s image [26]. That is, even though employees who value their organisation may be inclined to participate in gossip for negative influence when confronted with an abusive supervisor, they will be less inclined to gossip to protect the organisation’s image from potentially harmful consequences. These findings support studies claiming that employees who identify with their organisations experience more favourable outcomes regarding well-being and behaviour, even in challenging and persistent personal contexts [114,115].
Furthermore, it was found that gender was not associated with information gathering, developing relations, and negative influence gossip. The analysis yielded consistent results for both samples. The high school teachers in sample 1 were not accessible during their school hours. However, they were free before or after their teaching hours, whereas the academics in sample 2 were more independent than the teachers [116]. The nature of these occupations meant that these two samples were not biased in their gender composition. These unique sample characteristics may explain why, for example, gender was not a significant control variable in this study. Finally, the results indicated that age did not associate with developing relationships. However, our results indicated a significant difference in the association between age and information gathering and the negative influence of gossip for both samples. The probable reason for this result is the difference in the average age of the samples (Sample 1 had an average age of 34.9 and Sample 2 had an average of 43.2). Those in Sample 2 were almost a decade older with lots more experience than those in Sample 1. Despite the limited literature, our findings differ from Massar, Buunk, and Rempt [80]’s study. Massar et al. [80] found a significant negative effect between a participant’s age and their tendency to gossip. The difference in experience between the two samples may be a possible reason for this result.

6. Limitations and Future Direction

This study provides suggestions for future research while recognizing its inherent limitations. The first limitation stems from the cross-sectional nature of this study, which prevents us from drawing conclusions on the nature of causality [117]. Further research using experimental or longitudinal designs may help clarify these issues. Second, the sample of this study was limited to those working in the education sector, which may raise concerns about the generalizability of our findings. While this is a concern, it is also important to note that two samples from the education sector provided a reasonably suitable testing ground for our hypotheses, suggesting that the effects found were powerful. With further research, these findings must be replicated in groups of subjects from various industries. Examining the effect of positive personality traits to support TAT in future gossip studies is recommended. Exploring gossip from different perspectives has the potential to provide practitioners with a better understanding of this critical phenomenon. The findings of this study can be evaluated more broadly in studies conducted in different cultures.

7. Conclusions and Policy Implications

The dark side of management and unregulated organisational communication systems endangers the sustainability of organisations and the peace climate within organisations. This research examines the types of organisational gossip from the perspective of trait activation theory in educational organisations by applying structural equation modelling. In this context, the current research sheds light on the literature on organisational behaviour by integrating the effects of abusive supervision on organisational gossip, considering the mediating role of the dark triad. The results show that abusive supervision affects information gathering gossip but not relationship building gossip or negatively influence gossip. Moreover, the empirical evidence further confirms the mediating role of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy on the relationship between abusive supervision and the three categories of organisational gossip. Furthermore, the results of this study indicate that abusive supervision may serve as a situational factor that activates narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy and predicts information.
Based on these findings, this study offers policy suggestions for educational organisations in particular and for all organisations in general. Even if educators have some autonomy in their school/faculty, their share of participatory activities in management should be improved. Last, the findings demonstrated that despite the ramifications of abusive supervision, a sense of trust, organisational identification, and perceptions of organisational justice can significantly affect organisational gossip. Consequently, human resource policies that enhance employees’ personal characteristics and organisational identity should be adopted, management–employee relationships strengthened, and perceptions of organisational justice increased.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: F.U. and S.B.A. Methodology: F.U. and S.B.A. Software, visualization, surveillance, and note: F.U. and S.B.A. Verification: F.U. Formal analysis and data curation: F.U. and S.B.A. References: F.U. Writing original drafting, writing—review, editing, and project administration: F.U. and S.B.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Atatürk University Institute of Social Sciences Ethics Committee (Protocol Code: (Ref. SOSBİL 2020/12-107)).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Questionnaire

Dark Triad
Narcissism
  • I would like/care about being appreciated by others.
  • I would like/import others to favour me.
  • I would like/important for others to pay attention to me.
  • I would like/important to gain prestige or status.
  • I want/care that others feel/think that they owe me something.
  • I want/care about being dominant in social situations.
  • I want/important to appear ostentatious or proud.
  • I want/care to be more special than others.
  • I want/care about being better than others.
  • I would like/care about making myself look higher/better than I am.
  • I would like/important to be selfish.
Psychopathy
  • I do not feel remorse for what I have done.
  • I try to be callous or insensitive.
  • I do not care about ethical or moral behaviour.
  • I tend to be sceptical/cynical.
  • I feel easily frustrated.
  • I tend to get angry quickly.
Machiavellianism
  • When it comes to my interests, I consider every means as permissible/legitimate.
  • I tend to manipulate others for my own interests.
  • I flatter/praise others for my own interests.
  • I tend to use others for my own interests.
  • I tend not to care about the feelings/senses of others.
Abusive Supervision
  • He/she ridicules me.
  • He/she says that my thoughts/feelings are stupid.
  • He/she is sneaky with me.
  • He/she humiliates me in front of others.
  • Does not respect my private life.
  • Reminds me of my past mistakes and failures.
  • He/she does not respect me in critical matters.
  • He/she blames me when he is embarrassed.
  • He/she does not keep his promises.
  • When he/she is angry for another reason, he/she takes it out on me.
  • He/she makes negative comments about me to others.
  • He/she is rude to me.
  • He/she does not allow me to interact with my co-workers.
  • Tells me that I’m incompetent.
  • He/she lies to me.
Organisational Gossip
Organisational Harm
  • The gossips among my colleagues at my school demoralize me.
  • The gossips in my school cause disagreements among us.
  • I see it as a waste of time to be in the gossip environments at my school
  • The gossips at my school cause groupings among my colleagues.
  • I lose my confidence in my colleagues who gossips at my school.
  • The gossips among my colleagues at my school reduce my motivation.
  • I am reluctant to go to school at times when gossips are common in my school.
  • The gossips about my colleagues in my school cause me to misunderstand them.
  • The gossips about my colleagues at my school create prejudice against them.
  • I keep distance with my colleagues who gossip about me in my school.
Information Gathering
  • I learn many things about my colleagues in my organisation through gossip.
  • I learn some of the thoughts of my colleagues in my organisation through gossip.
  • I learn a lot of information about new colleagues in my organisation through gossip.
  • I learn about the experiences of my colleagues through gossip in my organisation.
  • I hear information about my colleagues in my organisation through gossip before official communication channels.
  • I learn many things through gossip that I cannot learn from my colleagues in my organisation.
  • I hear about some events that are tried to be covered up in my organisation through gossip.
Relationship Building
  • Gossiping with my colleagues in my organisation increases our sincerity.
  • I share my opinions freely in gossip environments in my organisation.
  • I try to relax by gossiping with my colleagues in my organisation.
  • I express my thoughts freely in gossip environments in my organisation.
  • I make new friends in the gossip environments in my organisation.
  • Gossip environments in my organisation strengthen my friendship ties.
  • I have fun gossiping with my colleagues in my organisation.
Negative Influence
  • Gossip among colleagues in my organisation demoralizes me.
  • Gossip in my organisation causes disagreements between me and my colleagues.
  • I see being in a gossip environment in my organisation as a waste of time.
  • Gossip in my institution causes grouping among my colleagues.
  • I lose trust in those who gossip in my organisation.
  • Gossip among colleagues in my organisation decreases my motivation.
  • I go to work reluctantly when gossip is common in my organisation.
  • Gossip about my colleagues in my organisation causes me to misunderstand them.
  • Gossip about my colleagues in my organisation creates prejudice against them.
  • I keep my distance from those who gossip about me in my organisation.

References

  1. Dores Cruz, T.D.; Nieper, A.S.; Testori, M.; Martinescu, E.; Beersma, B. An integrative definition and framework to study gossip. Group Organ. Manag. 2021, 46, 252–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Khan, A.; Chaudhary, R. Gossip at work: A model of narcissism, core self-evaluation and perceived organizational politics. Int. J. Manpow. 2022, 44, 197–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Sun, T.; Schilpzand, P.; Liu, Y. Workplace gossip: An integrative review of its antecedents, functions, and consequences. J. Organ. Behav. 2022, 44, 311–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Waddington, K. Gossip, Organization and Work: A Research Overview; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  5. Grosser, T.J.; Lopez-Kidwell, V.; Labianca, G. A social network analysis of positive and negative gossip in organizational life. Group Organ. Manag. 2010, 35, 177–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Zagenczyk, T.J.; Gibney, R.; Murrell, A.J.; Boss, S.R. Friends don’t make friends good citizens, but advisors do. Group Organ. Manag. 2008, 33, 760–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Kong, M. Effect of perceived negative workplace gossip on employees’ behaviors. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Liu, T.; Wu, L.; Yang, Y.; Jia, Y. Work-to-Family Spillover Effects of Workplace Negative Gossip: A Mediated Moderation Model. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Michelson, G.; Mouly, V.S. Do loose lips sink ships? The meaning, antecedents, and consequences of rumour and gossips in organisations. Int. J. Corp. Commun. 2004, 9, 189–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Beersma, B.; Van Kleef, G.A.; Dijkstra, M.T.M. Antecedents and consequences of gossip in work groups. In The Oxford Handbook of Gossip and Reputation; Giardini, F., Wittek, R., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2019; pp. 417–434. [Google Scholar]
  11. DiFonzo, N.; Bordia, P. Rumor, gossip and urban legends. Diogenes 2007, 54, 19–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Foster, E.K. Research on gossip: Taxonomy, methods, and future directions. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2004, 8, 78–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Hartung, F.-M.; Krohn, C.; Pirschtat, M. Better than its reputation? Gossip and the reasons why we and individuals with “dark” personalities talk about others. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Beersma, B.; Van Kleef, G.A. How the grapevine keeps you in line: Gossip increases contributions to the group. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2011, 2, 642–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Beersma, B.; Van Kleef, G.A. Why people gossip: An empirical analysis of social motives, antecedents, and consequences. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 42, 2640–2670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Houmanfar, R.; Johnson, R. Organizational implications of gossip and rumor. J. Organ. Behav. Manag. 2004, 23, 117–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Naeem, M.; Weng, Q.D.; Ali, A.; Hameed, Z. An eye for an eye: Does subordinates’ negative workplace gossip lead to supervisor abuse? Pers. Rev. 2020, 49, 284–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ye, C.; He, B.; Sun, X. Subordinates’ negative workplace gossip leads to supervisor abuse: Based on the conservation of resources theory. Chin. Manag. Stud. 2022, 16, 315–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Zhong, R.; Yu, L.; Zhu, J.; Wang, Y. Team Gossip Prevents the Negative Effect of Abusive Supervision on Team Norms and Effectiveness. Acad. Manag. Proc. 2020, 2020, 17557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ye, Y.; Zhu, H.; Deng, X.; Mu, Z. Negative workplace gossip and service outcomes: An explanation from social identity theory. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2019, 82, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Yu, B.; Meng, Y.; Li, C.; Liu, D. How Does Employees’ Narcissism Influence Organizational Commitment? The Role of Perceived Supervisor Support and Abusive Supervision. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 910739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Tepper, B.J. Consequences of abusive supervision. Acad. Manag. J. 2000, 43, 178–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Mitchell, M.S.; Ambrose, M.L. Employees’ behavioral reactions to supervisor aggression: An examination of individual and situational factors. J. Appl. Psychol. 2012, 97, 1148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Zellars, K.L.; Tepper, B.J.; Duffy, M.K. Abusive supervision and subordinates’ organizational citizenship behavior. J. Appl. Psychol. 2002, 87, 1068–1076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Tepper, B.J. Abusive supervision in work organizations: Review, synthesis, and research agenda. J. Manag. 2007, 33, 261–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Decoster, S.; Camps, J.; Stouten, J.; Vandevyvere, L.; Tripp, T.M. Standing by your organization: The impact of organizational identification and abusive supervision on followers’ perceived cohesion and tendency to gossip. J. Bus. Ethics 2013, 118, 623–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Mawritz, M.B.; Dust, S.B.; Resick, C.J. Hostile climate, abusive supervision, and employee coping: Does conscientiousness matter? J. Appl. Psychol. 2014, 99, 737–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Zhang, J.; Liu, J. Is abusive supervision an absolute devil? Literature review and research agenda. Asia Pac. J. Manag. 2018, 35, 719–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Blickle, G.; Schütte, N.; Genau, H.A. Manager psychopathy, trait activation, and job performance: A multi-source study. Eur. J. Work. Organ. Psychol. 2018, 27, 450–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Greenbaum, R.L.; Hill, A.; Mawritz, M.B.; Quade, M.J. Employee Machiavellianism to unethical behavior: The role of abusive supervision as a trait activator. J. Manag. 2017, 43, 585–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Tett, R.P.; Burnett, D.D. A personality trait-based interactionist model of job performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 500–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Thoroughgood, C.N.; Lee, K.; Sawyer, K.B.; Zagenczyk, T.J. Change is coming, time to undermine? Examining the countervailing effects of anticipated organizational change and coworker exchange quality on the relationship between Machiavellianism and social undermining at work. J. Bus. Ethics 2022, 181, 701–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hogg, M.A.; Vaughan, G.M. Social Psychology: An Introduction; Harvester Wheatsheaf: Coleshill, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  34. Janowski, A.; Szczepańska-Przekota, A. The Trait of Extraversion as an Energy-Based Determinant of Entrepreneur’s Success—The Case of Poland. Energies 2022, 15, 4533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kowalski, R.M. Behaving Badly: Aversive Behaviors in Interpersonal Relationships; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  36. Mischel, W. The interaction of person and situation. In Personality at the Crossroads: Current Issues in Interactional Psychology; Magnusson, D., Endler, N.S., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: New York, NY, USA; Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1977; pp. 333–352. [Google Scholar]
  37. Gumport, P.J. Public universities as academic workplaces. Daedalus 1997, 126, 113–136. [Google Scholar]
  38. Ashforth, B.E. Petty tyranny in organizations: A preliminary examination of antecedents and consequences. Can. J. Adm. Sci. Rev. Can. Des Sci. De L’administration 1997, 14, 126–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Schat, A.C.H.; Desmarais, S.; Kelloway, E.K. Effects of Organizational Tolerance on the Incidence and Consequences of Workplace Aggression. In Proceedings of the Academy of Management 2005 Annual Meeting, Honolulu, Hawaii, 5–10 August 2005. [Google Scholar]
  40. Duffy, M.K.; Ganster, D.C.; Pagon, M. Social undermining in the workplace. Acad. Manag. J. 2002, 45, 331–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Keashly, L.; Trott, V.; MacLean, L.M. Abusive behavior in the workplace: A preliminary investigation. Violence Vict. 1994, 9, 341–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mitchell, M.S.; Ambrose, M.L. Abusive supervision and workplace deviance and the moderating effects of negative reciprocity beliefs. J. Appl. Psychol. 2007, 92, 1159–1168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Tepper, B.J.; Duffy, M.K.; Henle, C.A.; Lambert, L.S. Procedural injustice, victim precipitation, and abusive supervision. Pers. Psychol. 2006, 59, 101–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Tepper, B.J.; Simon, L.; Park, H.M. Abusive supervision. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 2017, 4, 123–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Duffy, M.K.; Ganster, D.C.; Shaw, J.D.; Johnson, J.L.; Pagon, M. The social context of undermining behavior at work. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2006, 101, 105–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Tepper, B.J.; Duffy, M.K.; Hoobler, J.; Ensley, M.D. Moderators of the relationships between coworkers’ organizational citizenship behavior and fellow employees’ attitudes. J. Appl. Psychol. 2004, 89, 455–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Harris, K.J.; Kacmar, K.M.; Zivnuska, S. An investigation of abusive supervision as a predictor of performance and the meaning of work as a moderator of the relationship. Leadersh. Q. 2007, 18, 252–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Chaby, L.E.; Sheriff, M.J.; Hirrlinger, A.M.; Braithwaite, V.A. Does early stress prepare individuals for a stressful future? Stress during adolescence improves foraging under threat. Anim. Behav. 2015, 105, 37–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Chaby, L.E.; Sheriff, M.J.; Hirrlinger, A.M.; Braithwaite, V.A. Can we understand how developmental stress enhances performance under future threat with the Yerkes-Dodson law? Commun. Integr. Biol. 2015, 8, e1029689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Rosnow, R.L. Rumor and gossip in interpersonal interaction and beyond: A social exchange perspective. In Behaving Badly: Aversive Behaviors in Interpersonal Relationships; Kowalski, R.M., Ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  51. Han, B.; Dağlı, A. Organizational Gossip Scale: Validity and Reliability Study. Electron. Turk. Stud. 2018, 13, 829–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Hafen, S. Organizational gossip: A revolving door of regulation and resistance. South. J. Commun. 2004, 69, 223–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Noon, M.; Delbridge, R. News from behind my hand: Gossip in organizations. Organ. Stud. 1993, 14, 23–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kurland, N.B.; Pelled, L.H. Passing the word: Toward a model of gossip and power in the workplace. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2000, 25, 428–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Thomas, S.A.; Rozell, E.J. Gossip and nurses: Malady or remedy? Health Care Manag. 2007, 26, 111–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Ahmad, B.; Tariq, H.; Weng, Q.D.; Shillamkwese, S.S.; Sohail, N. When a proximate starts to gossip: Instrumentality considerations in the emergence of abusive supervision. Empl. Relat. Int. J. 2019, 41, 851–875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Tett, R.P.; Guterman, H.A. Situation trait relevance, trait expression, and cross-situational consistency: Testing a principle of trait activation. J. Res. Personal. 2000, 34, 397–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Furnham, A.; Richards, S.C.; Paulhus, D.L. The Dark Triad of personality: A 10 year review. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2013, 7, 199–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Paulhus, D.L.; Williams, K.M. The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. J. Res. Personal. 2002, 36, 556–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Baughman, H.M.; Dearing, S.; Giammarco, E.; Vernon, P.A. Relationships between bullying behaviours and the Dark Triad: A study with adults. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 571–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Morf, C.C.; Rhodewalt, F. Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychol. Inq. 2001, 12, 177–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Vazire, S.; Funder, D.C. Impulsivity and the self-defeating behavior of narcissists. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 10, 154–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Christie, R. Why Machiavelli? In Studies in Machiavellianism; Christie, R., Geis, F.L., Eds.; Academic Press: London, UK, 1970; pp. 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  64. Hodson, G.; Hogg, S.M.; MacInnis, C.C. The role of “dark personalities”(narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy), Big Five personality factors, and ideology in explaining prejudice. J. Res. Personal. 2009, 43, 686–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hare, R.D. Psychopathy, affect and behavior. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research and Implications for Society; Cooke, D.J., Forth, A.E., Hare, R.D., Eds.; Springer: Lisboa, Portugal, 1998; pp. 105–137. [Google Scholar]
  66. Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. Introducing the short dark triad (SD3) a brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment 2014, 21, 28–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Allport, G.W. Concepts of trait and personality. Psychol. Bull. 1927, 24, 284–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Jones, D.N.; Mueller, S.M. Is Machiavellianism dead or dormant? The perils of researching a secretive construct. J. Bus. Ethics 2022, 176, 535–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Lyons, M.T.; Hughes, S. Malicious mouths? The Dark Triad and motivations for gossip. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 78, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Shirokova, G.; Selivanovskikh, L.; Morris, M.H.; Bordunos, A. Unravelling the Relationship between Dark Triad Traits and Effectuation and Causation within Small-and Medium-sized Enterprises. Br. J. Manag. 2022, 34, 1555–1583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Brown, M.E.; Treviño, L.K.; Harrison, D.A. Ethical leadership: A social learning perspective for construct development and testing. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2005, 97, 117–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. De Hoogh, A.H.B.; Den Hartog, D.N.; Belschak, F.D. Showing one’s true colors: Leader Machiavellianism, rules and instrumental climate, and abusive supervision. J. Organ. Behav. 2021, 42, 851–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Kranefeld, I.; Blickle, G. When the Whole is Less than the Sum of its Parts: The Case of Psychopathy at Work. Acad. Manag. Proc. 2019, 2019, 16474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Schyns, B.; Wisse, B.; Sanders, S. Shady strategic behavior: Recognizing strategic followership of Dark Triad followers. Acad. Manag. Perspect. 2019, 33, 234–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Saunders, M.; Lewis, P.; Thornhill, A. Research Methods; Pearson Education Limited: London, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  76. Lykken, D.T. Statistical significance in psychological research. Psychol. Bull. 1968, 70, 151–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Jonason, P.K.; Webster, G.D. The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psychol. Assess. 2010, 22, 420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Litman, J.A.; Pezzo, M.V. Individual differences in attitudes towards gossip. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2005, 38, 963–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Eckhaus, E.; Ben-Hador, B. Gossip and gender differences: A content analysis approach. J. Gend. Stud. 2019, 28, 97–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Massar, K.; Buunk, A.P.; Rempt, S. Age differences in women’s tendency to gossip are mediated by their mate value. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 106–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Wang, H.; Zheng, X.; Liu, Y.; Xu, Z.; Yang, J. Alleviating doctors’ emotional exhaustion through sports involvement during the COVID-19 pandemic: The mediating roles of regulatory emotional self-efficacy and perceived stress. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 11776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Zhang, Y.; Liu, S.; Yang, L.; Feng, T.; Yang, X. Coparenting matters: The mediating effect of implicit theories of intelligence and depression on the relation between coparental conflict and STEM self-efficacy among Chinese adolescents. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 41, 6687–6699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Anderson, J.C.; Gerbing, D.W. Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 1988, 103, 411–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Preacher, K.J.; Hayes, A.F. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Methods 2008, 40, 879–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Hu, L.-T.; Bentler, P.M. Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychol. Methods 1998, 3, 424–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Gelman, A.; Carlin, J. Beyond power calculations: Assessing type S (sign) and type M (magnitude) errors. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 9, 641–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Gelman, A.; Tuerlinckx, F. Type S error rates for classical and Bayesian single and multiple comparison procedures. Comput. Stat. 2000, 15, 373–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Porumbescu, G.A.; Piotrowski, S.J.; Mabillard, V. Performance information, racial bias, and citizen evaluations of government: Evidence from two studies. J. Public Adm. Res. Theory 2021, 31, 523–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Bollen, K.A. Overall fit in covariance structure models: Two types of sample size effects. Psychol. Bull. 1990, 107, 256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Nunnally, J.C. An overview of psychological measurement. In Clinical Diagnosis of Mental Disorders: A Handbook; Wolman, B.B., Ed.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1978; pp. 97–146. [Google Scholar]
  91. Hayes, A.F.; Coutts, J.J. Use omega rather than Cronbach’s alpha for estimating reliability. But…. Commun. Methods Meas. 2020, 14, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Gefen, D.; Straub, D.; Boudreau, M.-C. Structural equation modeling and regression: Guidelines for research practice. Commun. Assoc. Inf. Syst. 2000, 4, 1–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Bagozzi, R.P.; Yi, Y. On the evaluation of structural equation models. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 1988, 16, 74–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Hair, J.; Joseph, F.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.M.; Sarstedt, M. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM); Sage: London, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  95. Nunnally, J.C.; Bernstein, I.H. Psychometric Theory; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  96. Podsakoff, P.M.; MacKenzie, S.B.; Lee, J.-Y.; Podsakoff, N.P. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 3rd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  99. Hoftede, G.; Hofstede, G.J.; Minkov, M. Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind: Intercultural Cooperation and Its Importance for Survival; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  100. Richman, L.S.; Leary, M.R. Reactions to discrimination, stigmatization, ostracism, and other forms of interpersonal rejection: A multimotive model. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 116, 365–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Difonzo, N.; Bordia, P. Rumor Psychology: Social And Organizational Approaches; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  102. Brady, D.L.; Brown, D.J.; Liang, L.H. Moving beyond assumptions of deviance: The reconceptualization and measurement of workplace gossip. J. Appl. Psychol. 2017, 102, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Duchon, D.; Drake, B. Organizational narcissism and virtuous behavior. J. Bus. Ethics 2009, 85, 301–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Pertuz-Peralta, V.; Arias-Pérez, J.; Daza-Calier, Y. Knowledge sharing among academics: Why organizational narcissism in higher education matters? VINE J. Inf. Knowl. Manag. Syst. 2022, 52, 141–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Xiong, Q.; Pan, Q.; Nie, S.; Guan, F.; Nie, X.; Sun, Z. How Does Collective Moral Judgment Induce Unethical Pro-Organizational Behaviors in Infrastructure Construction Projects: The Mediating Role of Machiavellianism. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Liu, C.C. The relationship between Machiavellianism and knowledge sharing willingness. J. Bus. Psychol. 2008, 22, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Sutton, A.; Roche, M.; Stapleton, M.; Roemer, A. Can psychopathy be adaptive at work? Development and application of a work focused self-and other-report measure of the triarchic psychopathy model. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Skeem, J.L.; Polaschek, D.L.L.; Patrick, C.J.; Lilienfeld, S.O. Psychopathic personality: Bridging the gap between scientific evidence and public policy. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2011, 12, 95–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. The role of impulsivity in the Dark Triad of personality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2011, 51, 679–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Smalley, R.L.; Stake, J.E. Evaluating sources of ego-threatening feedback: Self-esteem and narcissism effects. J. Res. Personal. 1996, 30, 483–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Birkás, B.; Pátkai, G.; Csathó, Á. The mediating role of the dark triad between life history strategy and perceived stress factors. Psychol. Rep. 2020, 123, 252–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Hurst, C.; Simon, L.; Jung, Y.; Pirouz, D. Are “bad” employees happier under bad bosses? Differing effects of abusive supervision on low and high primary psychopathy employees. J. Bus. Ethics 2019, 158, 1149–1164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Becker, J.A.H.; Dan O’Hair, H. Machiavellians’ motives in organizational citizenship behavior. J. Appl. Commun. Res. 2007, 35, 246–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Dukerich, J.M.; Golden, B.R.; Shortell, S.M. Beauty is in the eye of the beholder: The impact of organizational identification, identity, and image on the cooperative behaviors of physicians. Adm. Sci. Q. 2002, 47, 507–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Van Dick, R.; Ullrich, J.; Tissington, P.A. Working under a black cloud: How to sustain organizational identification after a merger. Br. J. Manag. 2006, 17, S69–S79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Ertem, H.Y. Does academic freedom make sense in Turkey? A content analysis study. Glob. Soc. Educ. 2021, 19, 511–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Coşkun, H.; Yildiz, I.; Alkan, Ö. Prevalence and predictors of psychological assistance services for older individuals in Turkish society. Front. Public Health 2023, 10, 1060845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. The conceptual model.
Figure 1. The conceptual model.
Behavsci 13 00730 g001
Table 1. Reliabilities and factor loadings.
Table 1. Reliabilities and factor loadings.
Sample 1 (n = 470)Sample 2 (n = 990)
ConstructMeanSDCRAVEωCFA RangeMeanSDCRAVEωCFA Range
Abusive Supervision1.160.380.940.510.930.63–0.811.470.720.960.590.950.70–0.84
Narcissism2.610.990.900.550.880.65–0.782.920.900.870.600.860.61–0.75
Machiavellianism1.380.590.890.620.860.63–0.881.490.710.890.620.870.67–0.86
Psychopathy1.700.710.790.530.770.66–0.761.930.720.760.590.710.68–0.74
İnformation Gathering2.141.050.930.660.940.72–0.872.621.090.940.690.940.76–0.87
Relationship Building2.000.960.920.630.940.74–0.841.980.910.910.590.910.67–0.82
Negative Influence3.521.130.950.670.950.73–0.883.720.930.930.570.920.65–0.84
χ2/df = 2.13 CFI =0.90 RMSEA = 0.05χ2/df = 3.11 CFI = 0.91 RMSEA = 0.05
Table 2. Correlations and scale reliabilities.
Table 2. Correlations and scale reliabilities.
123456789
Sample 1 (n = 470)
1. Gender______
2. Age−0.32 **––––––
3. Abusive Supervision−0.06−0.030.93
4. Narcissism0.01−0.13 **0.14 **0.87
5. Machiavellianism−0.02−0.070.29 **0.19 **0.87
6. Psychopathy−0.06−0.040.28 **0.36 **0.52 **0.75
7. İnformation Gathering0.06−0.11 *0.28 **0.38 **0.36 **0.44 **0.94
8. Relationship Building0.07−0.10 *0.24 **0.38 **0.43 **0.42 **0.67 **0.94
9. Negative Influence−0.020.03−0.15 **0.11 *−0.20 **−0.13 **−0.10 *−0.24 **0.94
Sample 2 (n = 990)
1. Gender––––
2. Age−0.30 **––––––
3. Abusive Supervision−0.040.010.95
4. Narcissism0.14 **−0.13 **0.15 **0.84
5. Machiavellianism−0.02−0.09 **0.23 **0.28 **0.87
6. Psychopathy−0.04−0.08 **0.22 **0.32 **0.45 **0.67
7. İnformation Gathering0.08 **−0.09 **0.24 **0.29 **0.26 **0.31 **0.94
8. Relationship Building0.02−0.060.21 **0.29 **0.40 **0.44 **0.51 **0.91
9. Negative Influence−0.050.14 **−0.03−0.05−0.20 **−0.16 **−0.13 **−0.37 **0.92
** p < 0.05; * p < 0.10; Cronbach’s alpha (in the diagonal).
Table 3. HTMT ratio.
Table 3. HTMT ratio.
Sample 1Sample 2
123456123456
1. Machiavellianism- -
2. Narcissism0.18- 0.22-
3. Psychopathy0.630.36- 0.430.27-
4. Abusive Supervision0.270.100.22- 0.200.120.15-
5. Information Gathering0.320.410.430.21- 0.240.280.350.19-
6. Relationship Building0.360.420.470.200.69-0.380.260.310.170.48-
7. Negative Influence−0.14.15−0.12−0.08−0.02−0.19−0.15−0.03−0.26−0.04−0.09−0.30
Table 4. Fornell and Larcker criterion.
Table 4. Fornell and Larcker criterion.
Sample 1Sample 2
12345671234567
1. Machiavellianism0.79 0.77
2. Narcissism0.220.74 0.280.77
3. Psychopathy0.700.460.73 0.620.410.79
4. Abusive Supervision0.280.110.250.72 0.230.150.210.77
5. Information Gathering0.350.420.520.220.81 0.290.330.360.210.83
6. Relationship Building0.390.420.520.210.700.79 0.450.320.560.190.540.77
7. Negative Influence−0.160.11−0.13−0.10−0.04−0.220.82−0.18−0.04−0.20−0.05−0.11−0.350.75
Correlations among constructs compared with the square root of the AVE (in the diagonal).
Table 5. Mediated regression results of information gathering gossip.
Table 5. Mediated regression results of information gathering gossip.
Sample 1 (n = 470)Sample 2 (n = 990)
PathwaysStd.βSEp95% CIType S/MStd.βSEp95% CIType S/M
LowerUpperLowerUpper
Direct Effect
Abusive Supervision → Information Gathering (H1a)0.090.110.09−0.010.190.14/6.260.110.050.000.040.180.04/3.63
Abusive Supervision → Narcissism0.120.110.020.020.22 0.150.04<0.0010.070.24
Narcissism → Information Gathering0.300.05<0.0010.190.41 0.240.04<0.0010.160.32
Abusive Supervision → Machiavellianism0.290.07<0.0010.150.44 0.240.03<0.0010.150.33
Machiavellianism → Information Gathering0.120.080.02−0.020.25 0.120.05<0.0010.040.20
Abusive Supervision → Psychopathy0.270.08<0.0010.120.42 0.220.03<0.0010.120.32
Psychopathy → Information Gathering0.320.09<0.0010.160.47 0.190.06<0.0010.080.30
Gender → Information Gathering0.060.090.20−0.040.14 0.060.070.07−0.010.12
Age → Information Gathering−0.030.050.45−0.150.08 −0.070.030.02−0.150.02
Specified Indirect Effect.
Abusive Supervision → Narcissism → Information Gathering (H2a)0.080.040.010.010.180.10/5.190.050.020.000.020.080.01/1.92
Abusive Supervision → Machiavellianism → Information Gathering (H3a)0.070.050.050.000.200.05/3.780.040.010.000.010.070.01/2.35
Abusive Supervision → Psychopathy → Information Gathering (H4a)0.190.080.000.070.300.14/6.080.050.020.000.020.110.30/12.90
Total Effect0.240.05<0.0010.130.34 0.220.030.000.150.28
χ2/df = 2.16 CFI = 0.95 RMSEA = 0.05 χ2/df = 3.38 CFI = 0.95 RMSEA = 0.05
Table 6. Mediated regression results of relationship building gossip.
Table 6. Mediated regression results of relationship building gossip.
Sample 1 (n = 470)Sample 2 (n = 990)
PathwaysStd.βSEp95% CIType S/MStd.βSEp95% CIType S/M
LowerUpperLowerUpper
Direct Effect
Abusive Supervision → Relationship Building (H1b)0.060.100.26−0.050.170.19/7.420.040.040.24−0.030.110.04/3.57
Abusive Supervision → Narcissism0.120.110.020.020.22 0.150.04<0.0010.070.24
Narcissism → Relationship Building0.300.04<0.0010.200.41 0.170.03<0.0010.090.25
Abusive Supervision → Machiavellianism0.290.07<0.0010.150.44 0.240.03<0.0010.150.33
Machiavellianism → Relationship Building0.180.07<0.0010.020.34 0.270.04<0.0010.170.36
Abusive Supervision → Psychopathy0.270.08<0.0010.120.43 0.220.03<0.0010.120.33
Psychopathy → Relationship Building0.260.08<0.0010.130.45 0.380.06<0.0010.260.50
Gender → Relationship Building0.050.080.24−0.040.14 −0.010.060.63−0.070.05
Age → Relationship Building0.020.050.67−0.100.14 0.040.030.18−0.040.12
Specified Indirect Effect.
Abusive Supervision → Narcissism → Relationship Building (H2b)0.070.040.010.020.160.07/4.340.030.010.000.010.050.00/1.97
Abusive Supervision → Machiavellianism → Relationship Building (H3b)0.100.060.010.010.250.03/3.420.070.020.000.040.110.00/1.07
Abusive Supervision → Psychopathy → Relationship Building (H4b)0.150.070.000.050.310.15/6.440.090.030.000.040.150.01/1.80
Total Effect0.220.060.000.100.33 0.210.040.000.130.30
χ2/df = 2.18 CFI = 0.95 RMSEA = 0.05 χ2/df = 3.41 CFI = 0.97 RMSEA = 0.05
Table 7. Mediated regression results of negative influence gossip.
Table 7. Mediated regression results of negative influence gossip.
Sample 1 (n = 470)Sample 2 (n = 990)
PathwaysStd.βSEp95% CIType S/MStd.βSEp95% CIType S/M
LowerUpperLowerUpper
Direct Effect
Abusive Supervision → Negative Influence (H1c)−0.050.140.34−0.150.060.47/109.340.000.040.99−0.070.070.37/21.56
Abusive Supervision → Narcissism0.270.110.020.020.21 0.150.04<0.0010.070.27
Narcissism → Negative Influence0.170.06<0.0010.060.29 0.050.040.19−0.040.14
Abusive Supervision → Machiavellianism0.290.07<0.0010.150.46 0.240.03<0.0010.150.33
Machiavellianism → Negative Influence−0.140.110.01−0.27−0.01 −0.120.05<0.001−0.21−0.03
Abusive Supervision → Psychopathy0.270.09<0.0010.110.42 0.210.03<0.0010.110.31
Psychopathy → Negative Influence−0.080.110.19−0.240.08 −0.140.06<0.001−0.24−0.04
Gender → Negative Influence0.020.110.61−0.080.12 −0.030.070.35−0.110.04
Age → Negative Influence0.030.070.49−0.100.16 0.120.03<0.0010.040.21
Specified Indirect Effect.
Abusive Supervision →Narcissism → Negative Influence (H2c)0.050.030.010.010.150.00/2.160.010.010.23−0.010.030.02/2.76
Abusive Supervision → Machiavellianism → Negative Influence (H3c)−0.110.060.02−0.02−0.260.48/132.38−0.040.020.00−0.07−0.010.28/11.76
Abusive Supervision →Psychopathy → Negative Influence (H4c)−0.050.070.26−0.230.050.06/4.06−0.040.020.00−0.08−0.010.33/15.35
Total Effect0.270.080.000.190.42 −0.050.040.21−0.130.03
χ2/df = 2.06 CFI = 0.95 RMSEA = 0.05 χ2/df = 3.14 CFI = 0.92 RMSEA = 0.05
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Uçan, F.; Avci, S.B. How Does Abusive Supervision Affect Organisational Gossip? Understanding the Mediating Role of the Dark Triad. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 730. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13090730

AMA Style

Uçan F, Avci SB. How Does Abusive Supervision Affect Organisational Gossip? Understanding the Mediating Role of the Dark Triad. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(9):730. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13090730

Chicago/Turabian Style

Uçan, Fatih, and Salih Börteçine Avci. 2023. "How Does Abusive Supervision Affect Organisational Gossip? Understanding the Mediating Role of the Dark Triad" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 9: 730. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13090730

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop