Next Article in Journal
Motivational Climate in Youth Football Players
Previous Article in Journal
Safety of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation of Frontal, Parietal, and Cerebellar Regions in Fasting Healthy Adults
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Happiness, Hardiness, and Humor Styles of Students with a Bachelor’s Degree in Sport Sciences

by
Pınar Yaprak
*,
Mehmet Güçlü
and
Tebessüm Ayyildiz Durhan
Department of Recreation, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Gazi University, Ankara 06330, Turkey
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8(9), 82; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8090082
Submission received: 10 August 2018 / Revised: 4 September 2018 / Accepted: 6 September 2018 / Published: 12 September 2018

Abstract

:
Happiness, psychological hardiness, and humor (“the 3Hs”) are useful ways of assessing resilience to stress in positive psychology. The literature analyzing their confluence regarding sports is scarce. This study aims to analyze the participants’ 3H levels and the relationships between those levels and specific variables. The Psychological Hardiness Scale (Psikolojik Dayaniklilik Olcegi, PDO), the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire Short Form (OHQ-SF), and the Humor Styles Questionnaire (HSQ) were used. Students in pedagogical formation training during the 2016–2017 fall semester have participated in the study (N = 211). Significant differences were found regarding the levels of psychological hardiness in relation to gender, type of sport, and years of participation, and, in humor types regarding the variables of gender, age, residence and perceived income. There were significant differences in all instruments regarding gender. Negative relationships were found between the “aggressive humor” and “challenge” and “self-commitment” sub-dimensions of PDO, while the relationships were positive between “self-enhancing humor” and PDO and OHQ-SF, and between “affiliative humor” style and PDO and OHQ-SF. This study enhances the positive socio-psychological account in the literature by incorporating the issues of 3H and provides an understanding of particularities that may help improve the practice of relevant experts and individuals.

1. Introduction

Happiness, psychological hardiness, and humor (“the 3Hs”) are often pointed to as useful ways of assessing resilience to stress when undertaking professional tasks [1]. They are associated with positive psychology, usually as accompaniments to coping mechanisms. Positive psychology criticizes traditional psychology for having “devoted very little attention to the study of wellness and the ‘positive’ aspects of life, such as resilience, character strengths and well-being” and having “focused too heavily on pathology” and problems such as depression, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress disorder [2]. The field of positive psychology is about valued subjective experience (such as wellbeing and happiness), positive individual traits (such as the capacity for love, courage, interpersonal skill, forgiveness, and talent) and “the civic virtues and the institutions that move individuals toward better citizenship” (such as responsibility, nurturance, tolerance, and work ethic) [3]. The positive psychology concept has been used in sports regarding positive behaviors, environments, outcomes, and organizational psychology [4,5]. This approach to sport studies preexisted the notion of positive psychology, with considerable attention paid to “what it takes to be a great athlete and how to facilitate performance” [4]. The approach is embraced not only in the pursuit of individual athletic excellence in elite or high-performance sports but also regarding individual and organizational developments in sports organizations as sites of psychological strengths and capabilities [4,5].

1.1. Humor

Humor is one of the three main dimensions in the current study. The positive psychology perspective views humor “as a personal quality that promotes resilience and well-being by means of cognitive reappraisal of stressful events” [6,7]. The literature (sporting or non-sporting research) suggests that the particular usage of humor (such as positive humor) may contribute to healthy communication, relationships, quality of life, positive organizational culture and physical and psychological health in general [8,9,10,11,12]. On the other hand, negative usages of humor (such as to humiliate or exclude someone) may cause harm to one’s wellbeing or status; therefore, it is important to understand the particularities in relation to humor to foster positive usage and prevent psychological violence [8,9,10,11,12].

1.2. Resilience (Psychological Hardiness)

The positive use of humor is considered a personal trait that promotes resilience and wellbeing. Likewise, resilience or psychological hardiness (another dimension in the current study) has been examined as an internal resource and personality trait reducing the negative effects of stressful life events on physical and mental health [13,14,15,16,17]. There are other terms such as mental toughness and psychological resilience used in the literature to refer to a similar theoretical argument. Challenge, control, and commitment were the three most applied dimensions when testing mental toughness, psychological hardiness, or resilience [13,16].
There is a notable co-existing issue in relation to sports and resilience or psychological hardiness: (a) the ability of psychological hardiness or resilience to mitigate the negative impact of highly demanding sporting performance environment on wellbeing and (b) psychological resilience as a positive psychology developed through sports participation to facilitate improved performance [17]. The highly demanding performance environment of sporting tasks (including contexts of elite or amateur athletes, trainers, physical education teachers or other sporting employees) is considered to require physical and psychological resilience [17,18].
Although resilience or psychological hardiness is especially important in rescue work occupations, as these employees are more frequently exposed to stressful and potentially traumatic events [16], few relevant examinations in relation to sports are found in the literature, especially regarding athletes engaged in high-performance sports [17]. Hence, it is important to remember that sport sciences graduates have likely been employed in rescue work occupations and engaged in high-performance sports for some time.

1.3. The “3Hs”: Humor, Psychological Hardiness, and Happiness

The relationships among the 3Hs have been often found to be positive, while a few studies have referred to negative relationships between some elements of humor and resilience [7]. In addition to their interactions, attention should be paid to broader issues regarding the society or environment that a person interacts with. For example, professional development and the quality of teacher education or training programs are important for understanding the current state of sporting employees or athletes since such elements of social environments are connected to components of psychological wellbeing such as self-acceptance, a positive relationship with others, autonomy, environmental mastery, and personal growth [18,19]. Thus, it is important to increase knowledge regarding the relationships between the 3Hs and development on and beyond the sports field.
Moreover, from a resiliency perspective, “several positive character strengths, such as kindness, humor, leadership, love and social intelligence, all showed significant increases in growth” following major traumatic experiences, such as “a life-threatening accident, attack, or illness” [7,20]. Concordantly, the literature suggests that the social and “numerous psychological factors (relating to a positive personality, motivation, confidence, focus, and perceived social support) interact to influence the stress‒resilience‒performance relationship” [17]. Likewise, physical activity has been associated with higher levels of happiness, though the association with happiness was domain-specific [21].
Furthermore, resiliency-based approaches to humor in literature are limited due to their sole focus on humor as a positive attribute. Thus, the humor styles model, which acknowledges both positive and negative aspects of humor, is suggested to be used within a resiliency perspective [7]. However, more research is needed to understand the particularities in relation to the 3Hs regarding the aforementioned complex elements and their interactions. The literature on these complexities is growing. Such attempts to shed light on more aspects can offer significant contributions to the literature [8].
In line with some of these factors and the importance given to their co-existence and interaction, we aimed to analyze the (a) 3H levels of students who have a bachelor’s degree in sport sciences and (b) examine the relationships between those levels and specific variables, therefore providing an understanding of some of the particularities regarding the 3H scores of sport sciences graduates. Overall, the literature on the interactions between the elements of 3H is scarce, not only for sports but also in general. Sports studies, especially the sports psychology field, have played an important role in helping “the field of general psychology move into a millennium of positive psychology” [4]. However, there is a need to expand our relevant understanding to identify the psychosocial factors that underpin organizational excellence in those sporting social actors (such as athletes, physical education (PE) teachers, coaches, managers, service providers, administrators, and policy makers) who operate within these fields on a day-to-day basis [5]. This study aims to increase knowledge that will serve such actors well in this domain. Increasing the competitiveness of PE can be achieved by acquisition of complex and interdisciplinary professional knowledge and skills, optimizing the attitudes and behavior of teachers and diversifying proactive motivations in accordance with the modern didactic requirements of physical activities [22,23,24]. In addition to the pursuit of improved skills through examining these sporting contexts of positive psychology, the current study is important in terms of aiming to contribute to knowledge in “pursuit of employee psychological well-being beyond achieving high job performance” [18].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design and Participants

Quantitative methods and a general survey model were used in the study. A total of 211 students who have a bachelor’s degree in sport sciences and were in pedagogical formation training during the data collection participated in this study.
There was an approximately equal number of men (n = 105) and women (n = 106) participating in this study. Most of the students perceived their income as medium (86.3%, n = 182) and lived within metropolitan municipalities, megacities, and cities (N = 185), while fewer participants lived in towns (n = 26). Moreover, most students were not national athletes (81.5%, N = 172). However, approximately half the students were trained in an exercise sciences department (41.7%), where there have usually been more students with a high-performance sporting background compared to the sports management and recreation departments at bachelor’s level in Turkey. Half of the students had 10 years or more of sports participation; the participation years and number of respondents decreased together (Table 1).
While 31.8% of the respondents were engaged in both individual and team sports, slightly more than half (54.5%) of the participants were engaged in team sports only, and fewer participants were engaged in individual sports only (13.7%). Results of correlational analyses regarding all these sporting and demographical backgrounds are shared in the following main sections.

2.2. The Instruments

The Psychological Hardiness Scale (Psikolojik Dayaniklilik Olcegi, PDO) [25], the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire Short Form (OHQ-SF) [26], and the Humor Styles Questionnaire (HSQ) [27] were used together with an information form on sporting and socio-demographic background to determine the 3H levels of the participants and the relationships between the 3H levels and specific variables. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of the measurement tools were found to be 0.84, 0.74, and 0.76, respectively, in the current study.
The questions on sporting background were to provide data on a participant’s (sports) departments, length and type of sports participation, and whether they were national athletes or not, while the questions on socio-demographic background were to collect information on a participant’s gender, parents’ education, perceived income, and residential area (Table 1).
The PDO is one of the three instruments used in this study. This instrument “was developed to determine the extent of psychological hardiness” or resilience [13]. It is a 21-item self-reported questionnaire that consists of three sub-dimensions (commitment, control, and challenge). The items are rated on a five-point Likert type (1: Absolutely agree to 5: Absolutely disagree) [13].
The OHQ-SF is another instrument used extensively and internationally. The Turkish version [23] of the OHQ-SF [25] is a seven-item self-reported measure of happiness rated on a five-point Likert type (1: Absolutely agree to 5: Absolutely disagree). The OHQ-SF and HSQ instruments were recommended as a reliable and valid measure among Turkish university students [25,26].
Finally, the Turkish version [26] of HSQ, as in the original instrument [28], assesses the following four dimensions of individual differences in uses of humor (the first two are positive, while the last two are negative dimensions): (1) “relatively benign uses of humor to enhance the self (Self-enhancing)” and (2) “to enhance one’s relationships with others (Affiliative)”, (3) “use of humor to enhance the self at the expense of others (Aggressive)”, and (4) “use of humor to enhance relationships at the expense of self (Self-defeating)” [26,28].

2.3. Procedure

The idea of this research stemmed from a comprehensive study on humor and sports in which the perspectives of grounded theory methodology were adopted by the corresponding author. The second author has suggested using humor in relation to the psychological instruments. Therefore, the current article was organized by the research team (the aforementioned authors) with suggestions from the interdisciplinary experts who gave their permission to use the relevant scales.
The possible instruments in relation to humor were determined with the suggestions of the field specialists. A discussion panel was made up of field experts, including the scholars who developed or used the relevant instruments. Both these scholars and the literature [7] have suggested the use of either two or three instruments or their consideration together.
The proposal of this research was submitted for ethical review, guidance, and approval, and the approvals were provided by the institutional review board of Gazi University (issue 92236698-903.99-, decision number 321), in addition to the reviews and approvals by the field experts. The students included in the study were sport sciences graduates who volunteered to participate and were able to provide informed consent. The students were chosen in accordance with the convenience sample. They were in pedagogical formation training at the university during the end of fall semester of the 2016–2017 academic year and were easily accessible. Overall, this research was carried out in accordance with the ethical principles of the Helsinki Declaration. This article was presented at and published in the abstract book of the 15th International Sport Sciences Congress in Antalya, Turkey, in 2017 [1].

2.4. Analysis of Data

Descriptive statistics were used to summarize the sporting and socio-demographic backgrounds of the sample (Table 1) and the mean scores and basic features of the 3H instruments (Table 2). The t-test and one-way ANOVA were used to compare the 3H levels between (such as gender) or among (such as the type of sports and residence) different sporting and socio-demographic backgrounds, respectively. The Pearson correlation coefficient was used to measure the correlations between the scales, including their sub-dimensions. Overall, the descriptive, correlational (t-test, one-way ANOVA, and Pearson correlation coefficient), and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) analyses were performed using the SPSS software package.

3. Results

The results with significant differences are provided in the tables below. First of all, there were no significant differences in the levels of PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and respective sub-dimensions according to participants’ departments, being a national athlete or not, and their parents’ educational status.
Secondly, the mean scores of 3H instruments were relatively high in the current study (Table 2). In addition, compared to the negative ones (“aggressive and self-defeating”), participants scored higher in the positive humor (“affiliative and self-enhancing”) sub-dimensions.
When comparing males and females, significant differences were found in PDO total scores (p < 0.00) as well as in the “challenge” (p < 0.05) and “commitment” (p < 0.00) PDO sub-dimensions (Table 3). Moreover, women scored higher than men in all PDO items. In addition, women scored significantly higher than men in OHQ-SF (p < 0.00). However, a significant difference was only found in “affiliative” humor styles (p < 0.05); women scored higher in both positive humor styles (affiliative and self-enhancing) and men scored higher in both negative ones (aggressive and self-defeating) (Table 3).
While there were no significant differences between the age groups regarding the PDO and OHQ-SF, significant differences were found in the HSQ total score, “self-enhancing”, “aggressive”, and “self-defeating” humor styles (Table 4). According to these significant differences, the 23 and under age group scored the highest and the 32 and over age group scored the lowest (Table 4). The humor style scores decrease with age with respect to the total HSQ scores. However, in contrast to the total scores, the oldest age group (>32) has the highest score in “affiliative” humor style, although, the difference is not significant. Moreover, there is an increase in the 28–32 age group in “aggressive” humor; on the other hand, this group has the lowest score in “affiliative” humor (Table 4).
A significant difference was found regarding the PDO sub-dimension “commitment” when comparing the number of years respondents had participated in sports (p < 0.05). The “commitment” sub-dimension score was the highest in the group that participated in sports for “1 to 5 years”, while the participants who responded “6 to 9 years” scored lowest and relatively similar to the group participated in sports for “10 years or more” (Table 5).
Similar to the age groups, there were no significant differences among the residential groups regarding PDO and OHQ-SF, while significant differences were found in HSQ total scores and “affiliative, aggressive, and self-defeating” humor styles (Table 6). The participants who live in megacities had significantly higher HSQ total scores and “affiliative” humor style scores than those who live in metropolitan municipalities or towns. The participants who live in towns had significantly higher scores of “aggressive” humor style compared to respondents who live in cities (Table 6).
The PDO and OHQ-SF scores did not differ significantly, while significant differences were found in “aggressive” humor styles with respect to perceived income (Table 7). The students who perceived their income as “low” had significantly lower aggressive humor style scores than the students perceived their income as “high.” The students who perceived their income as “high” had significantly higher aggressive humor style scores than the students who perceived their income as “medium.” According to these results, the higher the income they perceived, the more aggressive humor style they had (Table 7).
When comparing the levels of PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and respective sub-dimensions between the respondents who participated in different types (individual, team, or both) of sports, significant differences were found in PDO total scores and PDO sub-dimensions of “challenge” and “commitment” (Table 8). According to the PDO total scores, the respondents with individual sports participation scored highest, while the students with team sports participation scored lowest. The results regarding the “challenge” and “commitment” sub-dimensions of PDO were similar to these results regarding PDO total score; the respondents with individual sports participation scored highest, while the students with team sports participation scored lowest (Table 8).
Based on the Alpar (2010) qualification regarding correlation coefficient ([29], Table A1), there was a significant very low-level positive relationship between the HSQ and OHQ-SF. In addition, there were a low-level (with “challenge” and “control”) and moderate (with commitment”) positive significant correlations between the OHQ-SF and PDO including the respective sub-dimensions (Figure 1).
There was a low-level positive significant relationship between “affiliative” humor and PDO sub-dimensions except for the “control” sub-dimension of PDO. The relationship between the “affiliative” humor and OHQ-SF was significant but very low (Figure 1).
Moreover, there were very low-level positive significant relationships between the “self-enhancing” sub-dimension of HSQ and the “challenge and control” sub-dimensions of PDO a low-level positive relationship between the “self-enhancing” and “commitment” PDO sub-dimensions, and between the “self-enhancing” sub-dimension and OHQ-SF (Figure 1). On the other hand, the relationships were low-level and negative between the “aggressive” humor and the PDO sub-dimensions “challenge” and “commitment”. In addition, there was a low-level negative relationship between “aggressive” humor and “affiliative” humor, while the relationship was very low and negative between the “self-defeating” and “affiliative” humor styles. Figure 1 provides more details on the relationships.

4. Discussion

4.1. Relationships Regarding the 3Hs

A vast literature supports the positive relationships between psychological resilience, humor, happiness, and sports participation. The relevant research using similar scales and different combinations of these dimensions point out some common aspects. For instance, psychological hardiness and humor influence wellbeing by reducing the perception of stressful events as threatening and enabling the use of effective coping strategies [7,13,26]. Along with happiness, psychosocial variables such as positive social relationships, self-esteem, self-efficacy, self-mastery, and optimism have also consistently been associated with resilience, better health, and reduced distress [30]. Moreover, “happiness is associated with physical activity participation across multiple countries” [21]. Furthermore, the literature suggests that extroverts are more likely to enjoy and take part in social activities and sports (especially team sports), use humor, seek recreation, and in turn, have increased tendencies for happiness [31,32,33,34,35,36]. However, in another relevant study, introverts enjoyed sports more compared to other social activities that extroverts enjoy [31]. This marks sports as being or having the potential to be a common cultural activity that is more likely to be enjoyed by diverse people and linked with happiness and resilience.
In the current study, all respondents had sports participation background and some of the results were relatively in line with the abovementioned literature. The respondents’ scores were relatively high and there were weak or moderate but positive significant correlations between the PDO sub-dimensions and OHQ-SF. In addition, there was a very low-level but positive significant relationship between the HSQ and OHQ-SF. The significant relationships were very low-level and positive between the “affiliative” humor style and the OHO-SF, and low-level between the “challenge and commitment” sub-dimensions of PDO, which corroborated previous studies that found a positive relationship, especially between the “affiliative” humor style and happiness [28,37].
On the other hand, low-level negative relationships were found between the “aggressive” humor sub-dimension of HSQ and the “challenge” and “self-commitment” sub-dimensions of PDO. Moreover, the negative relationship was significant and low-level between the “aggressive” and “affiliative” humor styles, while it was not significant but still negative between the “aggressive” humor and OHQ-SF and “control” sub-dimension of PDO. In other words, while aggressive humor was positively associated with overall HSQ (revealing a low-level positive significant relationship) in the current study, it also came with less “affiliative” humor style, “challenge” and “self-commitment” (a negative relationship with these sub-dimensions). These results could contribute to the discussions on the interpretation of higher resilience (more in women and persons participating in individual sports and with “affiliative” humor style) and humor of any kind as “healthy, positive, better, happier” since the experiences in the process and output are multifaceted, subjective, or not definite regarding their scientific, personal and societal perception, and interpretation. These results were also consistent with some of the complex explanations on resilience, wellbeing, and humor in the literature suggesting that there are also negative associations between hardiness and mental health outcomes [38]—the processes relating humor use to cognitive appraisals and performance may vary dramatically and may support some aspects of a resiliency model, but not others [7,39]. While “humor use can promote distancing from the sources of stress”, greater use of humor was linked with “more external attributions for failure on a bogus intelligence test” and those who “used humor also spent less time and performed poorer on a subsequent test” [7,40]. In addition, the higher levels of commitment athletes exhibited, the less likely they were to use humor and behavioral disengagement coping strategies [39]. It was also found that “athletes with higher confidence levels in their ability were less likely to use self-blame as a coping strategy” [39]. The participants’ total scores were higher in positive humor sub-dimensions than in negative ones in the current study. However, the overall results and literature revealed the complexity regarding not only the use of negative humor but also the use of positive humor, especially “aggressive, self-enhancing and self-defeating” humor. Their negative relationship with resilience is a recognized state in the literature that can provide more insights into wellbeing or explanations to detect some negative, reduced or adverse performance results. Saying that the studies found a positive relationship between positive humor and coping strategies of teachers, and pointed out positive humor as a “healthy” coping mechanism [8].
The mean scores of all 3H instruments were found to be relatively high. In addition, positive humor total scores were higher than negative ones (Table 2). However, higher PDO sub-dimension and total OHQ-SF and PDO scores refer to higher happiness and psychological resilience [13]. Thus, the humor aspect, especially, was distinct, providing results for positive and negative humor types as well as adverse meanings revealed through their correlation with other dimensions, as discussed above. Moreover, of the six sporting (2) and socio-demographic (4) aspect, the gender was the only common variable that differed significantly in all instruments in the current study (Table 9). Furthermore, the significant results in humor styles included more distinctive aspects. Overall, significant differences in the levels of psychological hardiness were found in relation to gender, type of sports, and the number of years that the respondents had participated in these sports (Table 9). On the other hand, overall significant differences in the humor style scores were in relation to the variables of gender, age, residence and perceived income (p < 0.05) (Table 9).

4.2. Gender, Age, Perceived Income, and Residence

There was no indication of gender forming a significant difference in many studies on resilience, humor, and happiness, even though gender is one dimension always considered in their methods. Thus, there are still many aspects to reveal regarding gender issues. The results in some of the studies on happiness (with university students from diverse departments) [25,31], humor (with lecturers at the Schools of Physical Education and Sport [12] and resilience (with elite cyclists) [41] illustrated no difference regarding gender.
However, in our sporting research population, women outperformed men since they scored significantly higher than men in OHQ-SF as well as in all PDO items. Moreover, women’s higher scores in positive humor styles (affiliative and self-enhancing) and men’s higher scores in negative ones (aggressive and self-defeating) in HSQ confirmed this. Although the differences in most HSQ sub-dimensions (other than the “affiliative” humor style) were not significant, the features of the instrument enable some interpretation. The instrument includes four humor styles that people use in daily life and that researchers usually distinguish between: “participatory or affiliative” and “self-enhancing” humor styles are positive or beneficial to the self or others, while “aggressive” and “self-defeating” humor styles are negative or detrimental to the self or others [26,37]. Results similar to those of the current study were found in research with high school [42] and university [43] students in Turkey. In these studies, the male students’ “aggressive” and “self-defeating” (negative) humor scores were higher than the scores of female students. In addition, many other international studies indicate that males frequently use the aggressive humor style [44,45,46,47]; according to a study with Chinese junior high school students, this was because males tend to exhibit less empathy than females [44]. Unlike the studies representing the significant difference, the humor styles scores of men and women in the current study were insignificant. This was probably because women with a sporting major exhibited more negative humor or men with a sporting major exhibited less negative humor. Previous studies that did not refer to gender differences and/or provide comparable statistics require further examination and reveal a need for comparable future studies.
However, some results were highlighted in the qualitative literature regarding gender as well as age. A study reporting on responses to “seeing a man riding a unicycle” suggested that women did not make aggressively humorous remarks but had warm, appreciative, and supportive responses with a concern for safety instead, while the “male joke” with repetitive and irritating content, and offensive intent was observed by male and female unicyclists in many parts of the world [47].
Moreover, adult males showed aggressive and a stereotyped humorous response that became less frequent in elderly men [47]. Furthermore, significantly higher scores were found in the aggressive humor styles of research assistants compared with lecturers, assistants, and associate professors and professors; in addition, professors’ life satisfaction levels together with some of their “emotional intelligence” scores (such as coping with stress, interpersonal relationships, and adjustment) were higher than research assistants’ [48]. Another study [49] found a relatively similar tendency regarding the humor type and age; while teachers’ perception of the humor type of the school principal did not differ according to many other variables, it differed significantly according to the teachers’ ages (the oldest group scored higher). While the highest educational leadership scores were given to principals with the generative humor type, the lowest scores were given to the principals with a non-humorous style [49]. In addition, a study of Portuguese athletes highlighted “the role of maturity and experience in the use of more functional and adaptive coping strategies, supporting the developmental and age differences hypothesis in the use of coping” [50].
In the current study, too, as noted, there were significant differences regarding age as well as perceived income and residence in relation to the humor aspect only, whereas their correlations were not significant in relation to the resilience and happiness dimensions. In particular, significant differences in “self-enhancing,” “aggressive,” and “self-defeating” humor styles were relatively similar to the literature [47,48,49]; these humor style scores (together with the total score) usually decreased as age increased. In contrast to this tendency (to decrease with age) in total score, the oldest age group (>32) had the highest score in “affiliative” humor style, which was also in line with the literature [47,48,49,50]. Moreover, there was an increase in the 28–32 age group in “aggressive” humor style; on the other hand, while this group had the lowest score in “affiliative” humor style (Table 4). These results indicate that although the “positive” use of humor was more likely to occur in later age, such adjustments have not been linear. The broader psycho-physiological, sociocultural, and economic factors linked with certain age groups and contexts may provide explanations for these changing results.
For example, an anticipation of coming conflict, such as competitions or in social interactions, can be related to aggression. Moreover, the 28–32 age group may be more critical in terms of stress factors such as career uncertainties. Furthermore, a weak but positive relationship was found between testosterone and human aggression; testosterone among young adults was high but decreased around middle age, which was correlated with physically aggressive behavior in males [51,52]. In addition, the level of testosterone increases when you win, and decreases when you lose in sports [51,53,54]; sporting individuals are inevitably exposed to such physical experiences or memories. However, social studies in sport and violence tend to situate arguments on male aggression away from the on “male nature” in order to point out the greater role of social constructs [55,56]. As a social behavior, aggressive behavior is “a product of predisposing personal factors and precipitating situational factors.” For instance, encoded social cognitions are influential, including schemas about the world and normative beliefs about what is appropriate to interact with situational primes to determine behavior [57]. Accordingly, higher use of aggressive humor regarding certain residential areas and income levels in the current study provided even more evidence for this argument.
Similar to the age groups, there were no significant differences among the residential groups regarding the PDO and OHQ-SF, while significant differences were found in HSQ total scores, “affiliative, aggressive, and self-defeating” humor styles scores (Table 5). The participants who live in megacities had significantly higher HSQ total scores and “affiliative” humor style scores than participants who live in towns or metropolitan municipalities. Students who indicated their residence as a megacity may live around their university, which has a central location in the capital city. Metropolitan municipalities are locations where surrounding residential areas such as towns and villages were integrated with a city or megacity in recent years. Thus, their results were expected to represent similarities to the results from town, as much as from (mega)cities. The participants who live in towns additionally differed in terms of their significantly higher scores of “aggressive” humor style compared to the respondents who live in a city (Table 5). Do aggressive humor affiliations of respondents who live in town result from the number of stress factors (such as the campus not being as accessible) or the socialization (acceptance of “aggressive” humor may be higher in towns)? Does the “affiliative” humor of respondents who live in megacities result from the number of factors providing relief in life (such as more accessible living may be possible and different social interactions may be flourishing their perspectives)? There is a need for further research to answer such questions.
Moreover, significant differences were found, especially in aggressive humor styles, with respect to the perceived income. The higher income they perceived, the higher the score they had for aggressive humor (Table 6). Studies on humor found that socioeconomic status was necessary for subjective wellbeing but not enough on its own. The effects of income on subjective wellbeing are not simple and linear; for example, rising material desires, stressors such as longer work hours, and higher expectations for achievement were among the negative associations with income [43,58].
All in all, alternative categorizations of results are possible which may help varying interpretations. For example, aggressive humor was associated with males, <23 and 28–32 age group, less resilience (self-commitment and challenge), higher income and living in town. On the other hand, “affiliative” humor was associated with females, >32 age group, living in megacity and resilience (“challenge and self-commitment”) positively.

4.3. Physical Activity Participation

In the current study, those who had participated in sports for the shortest time (1‒5 years) scored highest in the “commitment” sub-dimension of the PDO, while the participants who responded “6 to 9 years” scored the lowest, relatively similar to the group that had participated in sports for “10 years or more” (Table 4). Although the literature usually did not directly provide information about either the number of years of participation in sports or the relationship with resilience, their age-related results in relation to sports, happiness, humor, or resilience are partly in line with the current study, as indicated in the discussion.
Physical activity participation is associated with happiness in many studies and multiple countries [21,31,32,33,34,35,36]. Moreover, “athletes from individual sports reported higher levels of worry, somatic anxiety, threat perception, and a greater use of venting of emotions,” while “athletes from team sports reported a greater use of humor and substance abuse” [50]. Parallel to these results, the respondents with individual sports participation scored highest in total PDO as well as in the “challenge” and “commitment” sub-dimensions of PDO (similar to women’s results compared to men) in the current study, while the students with team sports participation scored lowest (Table 7). The “challenge” and “commitment” sub-dimensions were significantly and negatively related to “aggressive” humor style, and “aggressive” humor style was associated with the number of variables listed at the end of the previous sub-heading. This result is important in terms of respondents’ possible current or future roles in sports fields as employees or employers. Resilience, hope, and optimism, as components of organizational psychological capital, promote organizational commitment and job satisfaction [59,60].
Studies pointed to many stressors related to the social interactions in the context of the team environment, thus further examination of “the relationship between athletes’ cognitive appraisal processes and different coping strategies in diverse individual and team sports” was suggested [50,59]. Furthermore, the significant difference was not so high in the use of humor or in happiness regarding the type of sports in the current study; it is likely that respondents were sport sciences graduates and have all had both team and individual sports experience.
Furthermore, studies pointed out the possible relationships between team sports, extroversion, and happiness [31,32,33], as well as the greater consumption of alcohol or drugs by team sports athletes as team sports generate additional opportunities for social interaction [50]. These results provide more evidence on the complexity of the 3Hs, socio-demographic and sporting dimensions; more specifically, of the resilience aspect in relation to happiness and humor types [7,38,39].
This study expands knowledge especially in relation to humor styles, resilience, and their use in a facilitative manner, as well as regarding the 3Hs in relation to populations with a sports participation background.
However, there were a number of limitations to this study. The sample used in the analyses was an easily accessible group of graduates from the faculty of sport sciences who have been in pedagogical formation training during the data collection. Therefore, the results cannot be generalized even to the department of sport sciences the participants graduated from (any department other than physical education teaching such as exercise sciences, recreation, or sports management). Although the current study provides some insights into the differences between students participating in team and individual sports and the sporting population in general, the inclusion of non-sporting participants would be useful to determine the meanings of the results providing comparisons between sporting and non-sporting populations. In addition, a limited number of participants from the study population fell within each socio-demographic and sporting variable. Future studies with similar or larger cohorts and with quota sampling regarding sporting and socio-demographic aspects would provide additional validation. Moreover, the additional validation is needed in terms of any aspect that was not provided with comparable statistics in literature as discussed above. Furthermore, there is a lack of such quests to bring together evaluations from both qualitative and quantitative analyses. Thus, qualitative analyses regarding the 3Hs, relative subjective performance, wellbeing, and the perceptions and interpretations of individuals with different backgrounds (e.g., people with or without sports participation, at higher risk of facing stress) on relevant issues would provide valuable insights. For example, previous studies on humor have led to further research on the influence of socioeconomic status, and it was difficult to compare some of our results especially regarding income, residence, and length of sports participation and specific correlations of the 3Hs due to the sparse literature.

5. Conclusions

This study enhances the knowledge of positive psychology in the literature by incorporating the 3Hs and social variables such as gender, age, residence, perceived income, type of sports, and the number of years’ participation in sports. The students who were sport sciences graduates scored relatively highly in all 3H instruments and their positive humor total scores were higher than the negative ones. Moreover, the results were partly in line with the so-called conventional findings in the literature. Higher scores in the use of affiliative humor were seen in females, >32 age group and respondents who reside in a megacity as well as its positive relationship with “challenge and self-commitment”. The negative humor (especially “aggressive” humor) was more often used by respondents in youngest age group, residing in towns, with higher income, and by men; negative humor also came with less resilience (self-commitment and challenge). On the other hand, resilience score was highest in students affiliated with individual sports, affiliative humor and in women. Furthermore, some of the results and complex relationships were in the furtherance of scarce literature. For example, the relationships between humor and resilience or happiness were not always positive, higher scores in the use of negative humor were seen in the respondents with higher income; men used relatively less aggressive and more affiliative humor in older age while such increase with age was not linear.
Acknowledgment of both positive and negative humor has been increasingly suggested in the literature. Our study suggests that the quests should move beyond the positive‒negative discrimination, based on the negative relationships in our results and in the literature especially between the “aggressive, self-defeating and self-enhancing” humor and PDO sub-dimensions “self-commitment and challenge”. Overall, the humor aspect in the current study, especially, became distinct for not only providing results for positive and negative humor types but also the adverse meanings revealed through their correlation with other dimensions. Moreover, the significant results in humor styles included more distinctive aspects (age, residence, and perceived income in addition to gender) while the overall significant differences in the levels of psychological hardiness were found in relation to gender, type of sports, and the number of years that the respondents had participated in these sports. Among the six sporting (2) and socio-demographic (4) variables, the gender was the only common aspect that differed significantly in all instruments (p < 0.05).
The discussion of the number of questions produced in this study was bounded by the scarce literature concerning combinations of traits and specific and comprehensive questions. Therefore, future research considering diverse variables (such as sporting and socio-demographic variables, or different personality traits, attitudes, or thoughts) and methodologies (such as qualitative) will continue to provide deeper understanding regarding the 3Hs.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Validation, Supervision, P.Y. and M.G.; Investigation, Funding Acquisition, and Resources, P.Y.; Methodology, Formal Analysis, P.Y., M.G. and T.A.D.; Software, T.A.D.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Visualization, P.Y., T.A.D.; Writing—Review & Editing, P.Y.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Acknowledgments

We thank Assoc. Şerife IŞIK and other experts for providing their expertise in the choice of scales in this study, and permission to use their inventory.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. The Alpar (2014) qualification regarding correlation coefficient [29].
Table A1. The Alpar (2014) qualification regarding correlation coefficient [29].
r-ValueQualification
0.00–0.19No or negligible relationship
0.20–0.39Weak relationship
0.40–0.69Moderate
0.70–0.89Strong relationship
0.90–1.00Very strong relationship

References

  1. Yaprak-Kemaloglu, P.; Guclu, M.; Ayyildiz, T.; Yaman, M.; Ozcan, V. The happiness, psychological hardiness and humor styles of physical education and sport teacher candidates in pedagogical formation training. In Proceedings of the International Sport Sciences Congress, Antalya, Turkey, 14 November 2017. [Google Scholar]
  2. Brady, A.; Grenville-Cleave, B. Introducing positive psychology and its value for sport and physical activity. In Positive Psychology in Sport and Physical Activity: An Introduction, 1st ed.; Brady, A., Grenville-Cleave, B., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2017; p. 13, e-ISBN 9781315304380. [Google Scholar]
  3. Seligman, M.E.P.; Csikszentmihalyi, M. Positive psychology: An introduction. Am. Psychol. 2000, 55, 5–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Gould, D. Sport Psychology in the New Millennium: The Psychology of Athletic Excellence and Beyond. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 2002, 14, 137–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Wagstaff, C.R.D.; Fletcher, D.; Hanton, S. Positive organizational psychology in sport. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2012, 5, 87–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Fritz, H.L.; Russek, L.N.; Dillon, M.M. Humor use moderates the relation of stressful life events with psychological distress. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 43, 845–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Kuiper, N.A. Humor and resiliency: Toward a process model of coping and growth. Eur. J. Psychol. 2012, 8, 475–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Ozdemir, S.; Sezgin, F.; Kaya, Z.; Recepoglu, E. The relationship between primary school teachers’ coping styles with stress and humor styles. Educ. Adm. Theory Pract. 2011, 17, 405–428. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bacanli, H.; Ilhan, T.; Gungor, C.H. Adaptation of Relational humor inventory to Turkish. Turk. Psychol. Couns. Guid. J. 2012, 4, 11–23. [Google Scholar]
  10. Snyder, E.E. Sociology of sport and humor. Int. Rev. Soc. Sport 1991, 26, 119–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Yaprak-Kemaloglu, P.; Simsek, L. Humor in Sport. In ISSA2016 Book of Abstracts, Proceedings of the 51st World Congress of Sociology of Sport: Sports, Development and Social Change, Budapest, Hungary, 8–11 June 2016; ISSA: Budapest, Hungary, 2016; p. 34. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cengiz, R.; Sunay, H.; Yasar, O.M. Examine of mobbing via humour styles of lecturers at schools of physical education and sport. Univ. J. Educ. Res. 2018, 6, 577–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Isik, Ş. Developing the psychological hardiness scale: The validity and reliability study. JHW 2016, 4, 165–182. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bartone, P.T.; Valdes, J.J.; Jocoy, K.; Spinosa, A.; Bowles, S.V. Psychological hardiness predicts cardiovascular risk in healthy adults. In Proceedings of the 25th Annual Convention—Association for Psychological Science, Washington, DC, USA, 23–26 May 2013. [Google Scholar]
  15. Kobasa, S.C. Stressful life events, personality, and health: An inquiry into hardiness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1979, 37, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Meulena, E.; Veldena, P.G.; Settib, I.; Veldhoven, M.J.P.M. Predictive value of psychological resilience for mental health disturbances: A three-wave prospective study among police officers. Psychiatry Res. 2018, 260, 486–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Sarkar, M. Psychological resilience: Definitional advancement and research developments in elite sport. IJSPW 2017, 1, 1–4. [Google Scholar]
  18. Kim, M.; Perrewé, P.L.; Kim, Y.; Kim, A.C.H. Psychological capital in sport organizations: Hope, Efficacy, Resilience, and Optimism among Employees in Sport (HEROES). Eur. Sport Manag. Q. 2017, 17, 659–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ryff, C.D.; Keyes, C.L.M. The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 69, 719–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Peterson, C.; Park, N.; Pole, N.; D’Andrea, W.; Seligman, M. Strength of character and post-traumatic growth. J. Trauma Stress 2008, 21, 214–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Richards, J.; Jiang, X.; Kelly, P.; Chau, J.; Bauman, A.; Ding, D. Don’t worry, be happy: cross-sectional associations between physical activity and happiness in 15 European countries. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Badau, D. The educational impact of implementation the education through adventure discipline in physical education and sports academic curriculum. Phys. Educ. Stud. 2017, 21, 108–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Badau, D.; Baydil, B.; Badau, A. Differences among three measures of reaction time based on hand laterality in individual sports. Sports 2018, 6, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Badau, D.; Badau, A. The motric, educational, recreational and satisfaction impact of adventure education activities in the urban tourism environment. Sustainability 2018, 10, 2106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Dogan, T.; Akinci-Cotok, N. Adaptation of the Short Form of the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire into Turkish: A Validity and Reliability Study. Turk. Psychol. Couns. Guid. J. 2011, 4, 165–172. [Google Scholar]
  26. Yerlikaya, E. The Adaptation of Humor Styles Questionnaire into Turkish Language. Master’s Thesis, Cukurova University Institute of Social Sciences, Adana, Turkey, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hills, P.; Argyle, M. The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: a compact scale for the measurement of psychological well-being. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2002, 33, 1073–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Martin, R.A.; Puhlik-Doris, P.; Larsen, G.; Gray, J.; Weir, K. Individual differences in uses of humor and their relation to psychological well-being: Development of the Humor Styles Questionnaire. J. Res. Pers. 2003, 37, 48–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Alpar, R. Spor, Saglik ve Egitim Bilimlerinden Orneklerle Uygulamali Istatistik ve Gecerlik-Guvenirlik-SPSS’de Cozumleme Adimlari ile Birlikte, 3rd ed.; Detay Yayinlari: Ankara, Turkey, 2014; p. 356. ISBN 978-605-5681-87-6. [Google Scholar]
  30. Yi, J.P.; Vitaliano, P.P.; Smith, R.E.; Yi, J.C.; Weinger, K. The role of resilience on psychological adjustment and physical health in patients with diabetes. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2008, 13, 311–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  31. Argyle, M.; Lu, L. The happiness of extraverts. Pers. Individ. Differ. 1990, 11, 1011–1017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Furnham, A. Personality and activity preference. Br. J. Health Psychol. 1981, 20, 57–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Emmons, R.A.; Diener, E.; Larsen, R.J. Choice and avoidance of everyday situations and affect congruence: Two models of reciprocal interactionism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 815–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Emmons, R.A.; Diener, E. Situation selection as a moderator of response consistency and stability. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 1013–1019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Emmons, R.A.; Diener, E. Influence of impulsivity and sociability on subjective well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 50, 1211–1215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Thorne, A. The press of personality: A study of conversations between introverts and extraverts. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 53, 718–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ford, T.E.; Lappi, S.K.; Holden, C.J. Personality, Humor Styles and Happiness: Happy People Have Positive Humor Styles. Eur. J. Psychol. 2016, 12, 320–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Leslie, C.; Hutchinson, A.D. Emotional distress when studying sensitive topics in psychology, and its relationship with hardiness and mental health. High. Educ. Res. Dev. 2018, 37, 549–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Kaiseler, M.; Polman, R.; Nicholls, A. Mental toughness, stress, stress appraisal, coping and coping effectiveness in sport. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2009, 47, 728–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Geisler, F.C.M.; Weber, H. Harm that does not hurt: Humour in coping with self-threat. Motiv. Emot. 2010, 34, 446–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Altay, B.; Bastug, G.; Arikan, I.I. Goal commitment and psychological resilience among cycling athletes. Eur. J. Phys. Educ. Sport Sci. 2017, 3, 618–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Yerlikaya, Y. Examining the Relationship between High School Students’ Humor Styles and Styles of Coping with Stress. Master’s Thesis, Cukurova University Institute of Social Sciences, Adana, Turkey, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  43. Tümkaya, S. Humor styles and socio-demographic variables as predictors of subjective well-being of Turkish university students. Educ. Sci. 2011, 36, 158–170. [Google Scholar]
  44. Wu, C.L.; Lin, H.Y.; Chen, H.C. Gender differences in humour styles of young adolescents: Empathy as a mediator. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2016, 99, 139–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Martin, R. Sense of humor. In Positive Psychological Assessment: A Handbook of Models and Measures; Lopez, S.J., Snyder, C.R., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2003; pp. 313–326. [Google Scholar]
  46. Kotthoff, H. Gender and humor: The state of the art. J. Pragmat. 2006, 38, 4–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Shuster, S. The evolution of humor from male aggression. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2012, 5, 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Tümkaya, S.; Hamarta, E.; Deniz, M.E.; Çelik, M.; Aybek, B. Duygusal zeka mizah tarzı ve yaşam doyumu: Üniversite öğretim elemanları üzerine bir araştırma. Türk Psikol. Dan. Rehb. Derg. 2008, 3, 1–18. [Google Scholar]
  49. Recepoglu, E.; Ozdemir, S. The relationship between principals’ humor styles and instructional leadership behaviors. Inonu Univ. J. Fac. Educ. 2012, 13, 23–42. [Google Scholar]
  50. Dias, C.; Cruz, J.F.; Fonseca, A.M. Coping strategies, multidimensional competitive anxiety and cognitive threat appraisal: differences across sex, at appraisal: differences across sex, age and type of sport. Serb. J. Sport Sci. 2010, 4, 23–31. [Google Scholar]
  51. Björkqvist, K. Gender differences in aggression. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018, 19, 39–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Julian, T.; McKenry, P. Relationship of testosterone to men’s family functioning at mid-life: a research note. Aggress. Behav. 1989, 15, 281–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Elias, M. Serum cortisol, testosterone, and testosterone-binding globulin responses to competitive fighting in human males. Aggress. Behav. 1981, 7, 215–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Mazur, A.; Booth, A. Testosterone and dominance in men. Behav. Brain Sci. 1998, 21, 353–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Messner, M.A. When bodies are weapons: Masculinity and violence in sport. Int. Rev. Sociol. Sport 1990, 25, 202–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Yaprak-Kemaloglu, P. Sociological analyses on violence in sports. In Proceedings of the Violence and Deviance in Sport Conference, Society without Violence Association & Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey, 27 April 2017. [Google Scholar]
  57. Rowell, H.L. An integrative theoretical understanding of aggression: a brief exposition. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018, 19, 119–124. [Google Scholar]
  58. Holt, N.L.; Hogg, J.M. Perceptions of stress and coping during preparation for the 1999 women’s soccer world cup finals. Sport Psychol. 2002, 16, 251–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Çetin, F. The effects of the organizational psychological capital on the attitudes of commitment and satisfaction: A public sample in Turkey. Eur. J. Soc. Sci. 2011, 21, 373–380. [Google Scholar]
  60. Kalfa, M.; Karaman, M.; Tufan, D. Organizational commitment of people working for Turkish sports management organizations in terms of some variables. Eur. Sci. J. 2016, 12, 159–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Correlations between instruments and sub-dimensions. ** Weak (0.20–0.39), moderate (0.40–0.69) or strong (0.70–0.89) relationship based on the Alpar (2014) qualification regarding correlation coefficient (Table A1 in [29]). Sig.: 2-tailed test p-value; N: Total number of students who participated in the study.
Figure 1. Correlations between instruments and sub-dimensions. ** Weak (0.20–0.39), moderate (0.40–0.69) or strong (0.70–0.89) relationship based on the Alpar (2014) qualification regarding correlation coefficient (Table A1 in [29]). Sig.: 2-tailed test p-value; N: Total number of students who participated in the study.
Behavsci 08 00082 g001
Table 1. Characteristics, by frequency and percentage, of students in pedagogical formation training.
Table 1. Characteristics, by frequency and percentage, of students in pedagogical formation training.
N * = 211
n/f **% **
GenderFemale10650.2
Male10549.8
Age<234019.0
23–277937.4
28–326430.3
>322813.3
DepartmentSport Management6731.8
Trainer Education8841.7
Recreation5626.5
Sports participation1–5 Years3918.5
6–9 Years6631.3
10 Years or more10650.2
National athleteYes3918.5
No17281.5
Education (mother)Primary school7837.0
Secondary school4019.0
High school6028.4
University3315.6
Education (father)Primary school5124.2
Secondary school3918.5
High school7334.6
University4822.7
Perceived incomeLow94.3
Medium18286.3
High209.5
Residential areaMegacity2411.4
Municipality12257.8
City3918.5
Town2612.3
Sports involvedTeam sports11554.5
Individual sports2913.7
Both team and individual sports6731.8
* N: Total number of students who participated in the study. ** The frequency (n/f) and percentage (%) of students falling in the given subgroup in the far-left column.
Table 2. Arithmetic mean and standard deviation values of PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Table 2. Arithmetic mean and standard deviation values of PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
N * = 211
x ¯ *Sd *Min.*Max.*
Psychological Hardiness (PDO)80.5911.3046.00101.00
Challenge28.504.6614.0035.00
Commitment27.095.0511.0035.00
Control24.993.8915.0033.00
Happiness (OHQ-SF)25.984.5813.0035.00
Humor Styles (HSQ)131.0318.7063.00190.00
Affiliative38.768.4115.0056.00
Self-enhancing37.037.4316.0052.00
Aggressive24.627.618.0039.00
Self-defeating30.618.328.0048.00
* N: Total number of students who participated in the study; Arithmetic means ( x ¯ ) from the current study; Standard deviation (Sd); Minimum (Min.) and maximum (Max) values of the instruments.
Table 3. The t-test results comparing males and females regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Table 3. The t-test results comparing males and females regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
N ** = 211
GenderN ** x ¯ **Sd **t **p *
Psychological Hardiness (PDO)Female10682.7010.892.7750.00 *
Male10578.4511.35
ChallengeFemale10629.254.562.3780.01 *
Male10527.744.66
CommitmentFemale10628.224.633.3300.00 *
Male10525.965.22
ControlFemale10625.223.690.8830.37
Male10524.754.09
Happiness (OHQ-SF)Female10626.914.223.0120.00 *
Male10525.044.76
Humor Styles (HSQ)Female106132.6116.301.2340.21
Male105129.4320.81
AffiliativeFemale10640.057.342.2660.02 *
Male10537.459.21
Self-enhancingFemale10638.016.611.9390.05
Male10536.048.08
AggressiveFemale10623.947.03−1.3010.19
Male10525.308.13
Self-defeatingFemale10630.598.06−0.0300.97
Male10530.628.61
* p < 0.05 = statistically significant. ** N: Total number of students who participated in the study; the number (n) of students falling in the given subgroup; Arithmetic means ( x ¯ ); **Standard deviation (Sd), Independent t-test (t).
Table 4. The ANOVA results comparing age groups regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Table 4. The ANOVA results comparing age groups regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Age Groupn x ¯ SdFp *
Psychological Hardiness (PDO)<234080.6712.300.9890.39
23–277980.4811.59
28–326479.3110.57
>322883.7110.54
Total21180.5911.30
Challenge<234028.525.300.9200.43
23–277928.324.33
28–326428.124.76
>322829.824.39
Total21128.504.66
Commitment<234027.054.861.2170.30
23–277927.055.45
28–326426.504.72
>322828.674.80
Total21127.095.05
Control<234025.104.040.1890.90
23–277925.103.87
28–326424.683.96
>322825.213.74
Total21124.993.89
Happiness (OHQ-SF)<234026.604.240.4420.72
23–277925.974.69
28–326425.544.65
>322826.144.74
Total21125.984.58
Humor Styles (HSQ)<2340138.4217.065.1990.00 *
23–2779132.4119.22
28–3264128.8416.52
>3228121.5720.14
Total211131.0318.70
Affiliative<234038.658.061.3470.26
23–277939.488.65
28–326437.188.20
>322840.508.53
Total21138.768.41
Self-enhancing<234038.576.053.3300.02 *
23–277938.037.63
28–326436.347.17
>322833.608.27
Total21137.037.43
Aggressive<234027.156.622.7120.04 *
23–277923.847.75
28–326425.007.64
>322822.327.76
Total21124.627.61
Self-defeating<234034.056.656.9520.00 *
23–277931.058.60
28–326430.317.80
>322825.148.30
Total21130.618.32
* p < 0.05 = statistically significant. ** Total number (N) of students who participated in the study; the number (n) of students falling in the given subgroup; Arithmetic means ( x ¯ ); Standard deviation (Sd), one-way ANOVA F-test (F).
Table 5. The ANOVA results comparing the years participated in sports regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Table 5. The ANOVA results comparing the years participated in sports regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Year(s) Participated in Sportn ** x ¯ **Sd **Fp *
Psychological Hardiness (PDO)1–5 years3984.3310.272.8010.06
6–9 years6679.1811.45
10 years & above10680.0911.37
Total21180.5911.30
Challenge1–5 years3929.434.070.9620.38
6–9 years6628.244.98
10 years & above10628.324.66
Total21128.504.66
Commitment1–5 years3928.845.013.3150.03 *
6–9 years6626.274.54
10 years & above10626.975.25
Total21127.095.05
Control1–5 years3926.053.511.8110.16
6–9 years6624.664.14
10 years & above10624.803.83
Total21124.993.89
Happiness (OHQ-SF)1–5 years3926.664.611.5100.22
6–9 years6625.214.48
10 years & above10626.214.62
Total21125.984.58
Humor Styles (HSQ)1–5 years39131.6117.140.4190.65
6–9 years66129.2819.75
10 years & above106131.9018.68
Total211131.0318.70
Affiliative1–5 years3938.666.810.1270.88
6–9 years6638.378.09
10 years & above10639.039.16
Total21138.768.41
Self-enhancing1–5 years3938.616.512.8060.06
6–9 years6635.368.73
10 years & above10637.506.70
Total21137.037.43
Aggressive1–5 years3923.467.460.5900.55
6–9 years6624.687.23
10 years & above10625.007.91
Total21124.627.61
Self-defeating1–5 years3930.878.080.0980.90
6–9 years6630.867.51
10 years & above10630.358.93
Total21130.618.32
* p < 0.05 = statistically significant. ** The number (n) of students falling in the given subgroup; Arithmetic means ( x ¯ ); Standard deviation (Sd), one-way ANOVA F-test (F).
Table 6. The ANOVA results comparing the residential areas regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Table 6. The ANOVA results comparing the residential areas regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Residencen ** x ¯ **Sd **F **p *
Psychological Hardiness (PDO)Megacity2484.5411.912.0430.10
Municipality12280.5811.43
City3977.5110.71
Town2681.6110.18
Total21180.5911.30
ChallengeMegacity2429.754.241.2490.29
Municipality12228.454.73
City3927.534.91
Town2629.034.25
Total21128.504.66
CommitmentMegacity2428.295.062.6200.05
Municipality12227.334.95
City3925.175.03
Town2627.765.10
Total21127.095.05
ControlMegacity2426.504.451.3620.25
Municipality12224.793.78
City3924.793.42
Town2624.804.40
Total21124.993.89
Happiness (OHQ-SF)Megacity2427.794.782.4610.06
Municipality12226.134.09
City3924.665.27
Town2625.615.10
Total21125.984.58
Humor Styles (HSQ)Megacity24141.1219.563.0920.02 *
Municipality122129.9716.90
City39131.2818.56
Town26126.3423.51
Total211131.0518.70
AffiliativeMegacity2441.709.023.0980.02 *
Municipality12239.368.07
City3935.718.01
Town2637.808.97
Total21138.768.41
Self-enhancingMegacity2439.337.861.0530.37
Municipality12236.757.00
City3936.158.32
Town2637.577.56
Total21137.037.43
AggressiveMegacity2425.417.183.1230.02 *
Municipality12224.007.18
City3927.587.90
Town2622.468.50
Total21124.637.60
Self-defeatingMegacity2434.668.343.1830.02*
Municipality12229.867.86
City3931.878.36
Town2628.509.29
Total21130.618.32
* p < 0.05 = statistically significant. ** The number (n) of students falling in the given subgroup; Arithmetic means ( x ¯ ); Standard deviation (Sd), one-way ANOVA F-test (F).
Table 7. The ANOVA results comparing the perceived incomes regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Table 7. The ANOVA results comparing the perceived incomes regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Perceived Incomen ** x ¯ **Sd **F **p *
Psychological Hardiness (PDO)Low981.118.890.6040.548
Medium18280.8510.93
High2077.9515.19
Total21180.5911.30
ChallengeLow929.772.380.9070.405
Medium18228.564.52
High2027.406.41
Total21128.504.66
CommitmentLow928.003.420.7310.483
Medium18227.184.91
High2025.906.77
Total21127.095.05
ControlLow923.334.520.9760.379
Medium18225.103.85
High2024.653.99
Total21124.993.89
Happiness (OHQ-SF)Low926.005.800.9820.376
Medium18225.834.36
High2027.355.88
Total21125.984.58
Humor Styles (HSQ)Low9123.1126.601.6660.191
Medium182130.8218.65
High20136.4513.97
Total211131.0318.70
AffiliativeLow937.889.300.4510.638
Medium18238.978.52
High2037.207.09
Total21138.768.41
Self-enhancingLow934.888.030.6490.524
Medium18237.257.46
High2036.006.98
Total21137.037.43
AggressiveLow923.339.203.9000.022 *
Medium18224.197.48
High2029.056.93
Total21124.627.61
Self-defeatingLow927.008.762.8160.062
Medium18230.398.39
High2034.206.50
Total21130.618.32
* p < 0.05 = statistically significant. ** The number (n) of students falling in the given subgroup; Arithmetic means ( x ¯ ); Standard deviation (Sd), one-way ANOVA F-test (F).
Table 8. The ANOVA results comparing the sports participated by respondents regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Table 8. The ANOVA results comparing the sports participated by respondents regarding PDO, OHQ-SF, HSQ, and sub-dimensions.
Sportsn ** x ¯ **Sd **F **p *
Psychological Hardiness (PDO)Team sports11578.5811.444.1750.01 *
Individual sports2983.5811.38
Both (team and individual)6782.7410.45
Total21180.5911.30
ChallengeTeam sports11527.504.796.1510.00 *
Individual sports2930.004.57
Both (team and individual)6729.564.10
Total21128.504.66
CommitmentTeam sports11526.205.064.0410.01 *
Individual sports2928.244.88
Both (team and individual)6728.134.89
Total21127.095.05
ControlTeam sports11524.863.980.1800.83
Individual sports2925.344.05
Both (team and individual)6725.043.71
Total21124.993.89
Happiness (OHQ-SF)Team sports11525.734.430.3920.67
Individual sports2926.344.36
Both (team and individual)6726.264.96
Total21125.984.58
Humor Styles (HSQ)Team sports115132.0119.281.1390.32
Individual sports29133.5820.68
Both (team and individual)67128.3116.65
Total211131.0518.70
AffiliativeTeam sports11538.428.590.2340.79
Individual sports2939.488.79
Both (team and individual)6739.048.00
Total21138.768.41
Self-enhancingTeam sports11537.097.870.1830.83
Individual sports2937.657.62
Both (team and individual)6736.676.61
Total21137.037.43
AggressiveTeam sports11525.137.900.8020.45
Individual sports2924.937.22
Both (team and individual)6723.677.25
Total21124.637.60
Self-defeatingTeam sports11531.368.612.0390.13
Individual sports2931.518.31
Both (team and individual)6728.927.65
Total21130.618.32
* p < 0.05 = statistically significant. ** The number (n) of students falling in the given subgroup; Arithmetic means ( x ¯ ); Standard deviation (Sd), one-way ANOVA F-test (F).
Table 9. Common or different aspects regarding significant results.
Table 9. Common or different aspects regarding significant results.
InstrumentsCommon: Significantly Different Results in Three InstrumentsDifferent: Significantly Differ in Particular Instrument(s) Only and Absent in Other(s)
Happiness (OHQ-SF)Gender
Hardiness/Resilience (PDO)length of years participated in these sports, type of sports
Humor (HSQ)age, residence & perceived income

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yaprak, P.; Güçlü, M.; Ayyildiz Durhan, T. The Happiness, Hardiness, and Humor Styles of Students with a Bachelor’s Degree in Sport Sciences. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 82. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8090082

AMA Style

Yaprak P, Güçlü M, Ayyildiz Durhan T. The Happiness, Hardiness, and Humor Styles of Students with a Bachelor’s Degree in Sport Sciences. Behavioral Sciences. 2018; 8(9):82. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8090082

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yaprak, Pınar, Mehmet Güçlü, and Tebessüm Ayyildiz Durhan. 2018. "The Happiness, Hardiness, and Humor Styles of Students with a Bachelor’s Degree in Sport Sciences" Behavioral Sciences 8, no. 9: 82. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs8090082

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop