Next Article in Journal
Update on an Observational, Clinically Useful Gait Coordination Measure: The Gait Assessment and Intervention Tool (G.A.I.T.)
Previous Article in Journal
Dynamic Functional Connectivity of Emotion Processing in Beta Band with Naturalistic Emotion Stimuli
Previous Article in Special Issue
Self-Processing and Self-Face Reaction Time Latencies: A Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Self-Enhancement and the Medial Prefrontal Cortex: The Convergence of Clinical and Experimental Findings

1
Department of Biology, Montclair State University, 320 Science Hall, Montclair, NJ 07043, USA
2
Cognitive Neuroimaging Laboratory, Department of Biology, Montclair State University, 320 Science Hall, Montclair, NJ 07043, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(8), 1103; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081103
Submission received: 17 July 2022 / Revised: 12 August 2022 / Accepted: 16 August 2022 / Published: 19 August 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Neural Correlates of Self-Awareness and Self-Knowing)

Abstract

:
Self-enhancement (SE) is often overlooked as a fundamental cognitive ability mediated via the Prefrontal Cortex (PFC). Here, we present research that establishes the relationship between the PFC, SE, and the potential evolved beneficial mechanisms. Specifically, we believe there is now enough evidence to speculate that SE exists to provide significant benefits and should be considered a normal aspect of the self. Whatever the metabolic or social cost, the upside of SE is great enough that it is a core and fundamental psychological construct. Furthermore, though entirely theoretical, we suggest that a critical reason the PFC has evolved so significantly in Homo sapiens is to, in part, sustain SE. We, therefore, elaborate on its proximate and ultimate mechanisms.

1. The Prefrontal Cortex and Self-Enhancement

The Prefrontal Cortex (PFC) mediates or is involved in self-awareness, language, inhibition, planning, and abstraction, whilst also interacting with most other areas of the brain directly or indirectly [1,2]. The primary function of the PFC centers around executive functions, for which it has obtained the moniker of “central executive” [3]. Furthermore, the PFC’s subdivisions (e.g., the dorsal and medial PFC, the medial orbital frontal cortex, etc.) act as functionally specific processors that can operate and interact with one another [4,5], which, in turn, influences other cortical and non-cortical regions. Another prominent function of the PFC, at least in humans, is a “first-person-evaluator” [6], which refers to its ability to allow humans to develop and maintain a sense of self [7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. This function of the PFC dates to the early beginnings of neuroscience [14,15]. Alongside these significant processes, one must emphasize the PFC’s equally essential functions, such as memory [16,17], modeling outcomes [18], abstract thinking [19,20], emotion [21], cognitive control of internal goals [22,23], and language processing [24].
Here, we suggest that self-enhancement (SE) stands as a critical underlying factor as to why the PFC might have evolved so dramatically in humans. Self-enhancement is described as the tendency to unrealistically perceive one’s image of oneself in a positive direction [25,26,27]. When one self-enhances, they typically exaggerate their strengths and downplay their weaknesses. While the evolutionary advantages of other frontal cortex functions are immediately obvious (e.g., abstract thinking, planning, and emotional regulation), SE’s contribution may be less noticeable. Through analyzing how SE occurs, how the PFC is responsible for it, and its potential evolutionary purpose, we hypothesize that the PFC evolved, in part, to develop SE.
Self-enhancement refers to the tendency to maintain an often unrealistic, positive view of the self [25,27]. In order to maintain this tendency, SE creates a false self-perception where one makes judgments about oneself that are ungrounded in reality [28]. Self-enhancement is typically defined as something that occurs continuously, meaning when one produces an unrealistic, positive view of the self, it is maintained for an extended period [29] and extends across all dimensions of cognition, including exaggerating potential success in the future [30], only acknowledging positive feedback [31,32], falsely reporting higher test scores [33], and overestimating social approval [34].
The evidence that SE is mediated via the PFC is not simply correlational, as Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) studies have produced direct evidence for the involvement of the medial PFC, or MPFC [35]. This has been shown by demonstrating that stimulation of the MPFC reduces the tendency to self-enhance [36,37]. More specifically, disrupting the MPFC while participants were rating themselves or their best friend caused them to perceive themselves as less “enhanced” compared with no disruption of the MPFC. These studies demonstrate a causal link between the MPFC and SE, as “virtual” removal of the MPFC leads to a reduction in SE.
Self-enhancement appears to be mediated through an accumulation of processes centered within the PFC, which is not surprising given its rich interconnected nature [4,5,38] and its many functions, such as pain processing [5], memory formation [39], creativity [40], etc. Among the vast array of functions of the PFC, some appear to be more directly involved with SE, including memory retrieval [41], conscious deliberation [42], morality [43], emotion regulation [2,3], and self-evaluation [44,45,46].
The PFC is involved in autobiographical (i.e., episodic, first-person) memory recall [47] and the recollection of self-relevant information [48]. During the retrieval process, the PFC often places an emotional value to autobiographical memories [49]. Lin et al. observed this in a typical fMRI design [41]. Scans were taken while participants took part in autobiographical memory recall tasks, where they would recall an autobiographical memory and evaluate it emotionally. Their analysis revealed the presence of blood oxygen level-dependent signals in the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC) during the retrieval of the memory. These signals would modulate depending upon the emotional intensity, therefore, correlating with the emotional intensity of the memory. These findings suggested that the vmPFC processes self-relevant information and is involved in associating emotional values with autobiographical memories. The extent of complexity that the value has is unknown, but it has been observed that during activation of the vmPFC, memories can be associated with simple values, such as “liked” and “positive” or “disliked” and “negative” [39,41]. Due to the SE involving the creation of illusory realities, this function is likely essential in order for SE to occur. Self-enhancement could involve the changing of a previous event from a “disliked” memory to a “liked” memory or vice-versa. This simple value change could lead to a completely different outlook on a previous event, regardless of the reality it holds.

2. Consciousness, Morality, Self, and Self-Enhancement

Conscious deliberation is a process where one forms a perspective and prediction of the future based upon (typically) past, present, and future considerations [42]. The neural network most involved in this process consists of the vmPFC, medial temporal lobe, and medial posterior regions, which are commonly considered the default mode network [50,51]. Among the numerous brain areas involved in this neural network, the vmPFC plays an essential role by mentally simulating events in the future [52,53]. Mentally simulating the future allows individuals to self-enhance, and this is typically correlated with activity in the PFC [54,55]. As noted previously, some individuals deem the occurrence of positive future events as far more likely to occur than negative ones [56]. Furthermore, most individuals demonstrate a higher probability to self-enhance when speculating upon events that are relevant to personal goals [57] and focus on short-term consequences relating to themselves [58]. Research for how the vmPFC causes SE to occur is still ongoing. However, studies have suggested possible mechanisms. The vmPFC has been found to modulate mental simulations of future events by modulating the associated emotional valence, making the intensities of emotions invoked by the mental simulations either more or less intense [57]. The vmPFC does this with both near and future events through the activation of different sections of itself [59,60,61]. Alongside conscious deliberation and memory retrieval, the PFC also accomplishes SE during the development of morality via manipulating emotional context.
Moral decision-making is the evaluation of actions while considering established norms and values [62]. The moral decision-making process is often a conscious and effortful task [63]. Through interacting with other brain networks, such as the temporal lobes and subcortical limbic structures [64], the PFC allows for moral decision-making [65]. Moreover, the PFC can change the desirability of moral decisions through these interactions, alongside interactions with the striatum [43]. This leads to the development of morality, such that if an individual views an action as desirable, they will associate it with being morally right to avoid the psychological repercussions and potential conflicts [29,66]. This process, we believe, is often the basis for SE. Due to the power of SE, one can create an illusion of reality by convincing themselves that an action is morally right when, logically, it is wrong. By continuously desiring to perform morally wrong actions and repetitively associating them with being morally right, one can self-enhance, regardless of any actuality.
SE can result in numerous nuances in moral decision-making. In addition, the principles of utilitarianism and deontology are of interest when discussing our current argument about moral decision-making, and it can be explained through the trolley problem. A viewpoint from a utilitarian perspective would support that killing a loved one over a group of people is morally correct, as the positives outweigh the negatives. However, a viewpoint from a deontological perspective implies that both choices are morally incorrect, as purposefully harming others is unacceptable, regardless of the situation. Regarding previous statements, when activity in the MPFC is increased, it causes the affected person to feel more conscious of their decisions and the impact they will make. This, in turn, makes them more likely to perform SE, which is based on the idea that the PFC can change the desirability of certain actions; thus, an individual will see a certain action as desirable and associate it with being morally correct. When applying this to the trolley problem, saving a loved one over a group of unknown people appears more desirable. Therefore, the individual will SE and create a false reality, in which it seems they made the morally correct decision, even though it is logically wrong, to avoid conflict and psychological repercussions.
Persons with Narcissistic Personality Disorder (NPD) have excessive SE [67]. Typically, one’s emotions will fluctuate depending on events within their everyday life. This is demonstrated in studies examining how emotions are altered depending upon a person’s inclusion or exclusion from social groups [68]. Individuals with NPD have exhibited the ability to consistently and sturdily self-enhance their emotions during moments that were meant to invoke insecurity, causing them to feel positive emotions, such as grandiosity and high self-esteem [67,69]. These persons appear to use SE as a defensive measure against negative emotions, such as humiliation or shame, causing further SE by associating themselves with positive characteristics, such as “thick-skinned” [70,71]. This ability to self-enhance to avoid negative emotions, as well as increase positive ones in a similar fashion, has been seen in healthy individuals as well, albeit to a lesser extent [25,72]. NPD is associated with excessive activity in the PFC [73,74], and disruption of the PFC via TMS appears to decrease the degree of sub-clinical NPD an individual may possess [75].
Self-evaluation is a conscious process, whereby a decision is made regarding oneself [76]. The medial PFC (MPFC) mediates the conscious processes associated with self-evaluations [44,45,46]. More specifically, it has been suggested that the MPFC plays a role in allowing the consciousness to access self-knowledge [77,78,79]. Alongside this, the MPFC associates mental states or perspectives when accessing self-knowledge [78,80]. Through both performing self-evaluations and associating mental states with self-knowledge, the MPFC creates illusory realities regarding the present and past self [81]. Specifically, through associating unrealistic perspectives with the present or past self, one may create an objectively false self-image. This method of SE can be exhibited even by healthy individuals, causing them to associate with overly positive characteristics when they, in fact, lack those traits [82]. Such behavior plays a vital role in making individuals overconfident during task performance. Since individuals are able to self-enhance when considering one’s self-knowledge, individuals can self-enhance their self-perceived abilities [82,83]. As a result, individuals appear to develop overconfidence biases [84], where they believe they can perform better than their objective skills allow them to [85]. These changes in self-evaluation have also been found to boost implicit self-esteem, which affects how individuals evaluate objects that are relevant to their identity [86]. For example, people have been found to inflate the monetary value of their property [87], view individuals who are similar to them as more attractive [86], and view individuals within their social group more positively [34,88,89].
In a society where one’s intelligence is valued, it is not surprising that SE is seen in persons reporting what they “know”. This behavior of overclaiming can be isolated to the PFC using a word knowledge test. Participants were randomly presented with a list of words and asked if they knew the definitions. Unbeknownst to the participants, 50% of the words were fake, and, thus, claiming knowledge of these words was impossible. Without TMS and under sham conditions, overclaiming occurred at a significant rate. However, following MPFC TMS overclaiming was reduced [90]. The role of the MPFC in overclaiming appears to expand under conditions of social pressure [91], which implies that overclaiming via the PFC likely exists to give one a social advantage. This makes sense, as overclaiming knowledge can lead to personal gains [92,93,94,95].

3. The Costs and Benefits of Self-Enhancement

However, not surprisingly, the cost–benefit ratio of SE has been debated. Several clinical and social-personality psychologists have argued that SE is maladaptive, listing several indicators [96,97,98]. Some psychologists believe that SE could cause individuals to harm their interpersonal relationships, as SE can lead to making inappropriate and excessive demands of others [99], not acknowledging suffering in work and love lives [100], and alienating themselves from others due to the belief they are above others [101]. Moreover, it is possible that SE could also cause damage to the self by causing individuals to lose their sense of personal identity [102], never reach self-actualization, and face frequent failure due to the belief that they can accomplish insurmountable tasks [103]. These shortcomings could all be argued to stem from one foregoing self-adjustment and, instead, undergoing SE [29]. In other words, instead of admitting to a fault and fixing one’s flaws, one can simply create an illusory self-perception and rid themselves of the psychological pressure. Trivers has, in fact, elegantly laid out the costs and benefits of SE (a factor of self-deception) and argued that the increase in confidence provides performance and social benefits, particularly the ability to both become a better deceiver and a more convincing persuader [104]. These theories were tested through the manipulation of participants’ overconfidence. Increases in overconfidence lead to an increase in persuasiveness, which the authors directly tied to an evolved SE cognitive architecture [105].
In contrast, previous studies have demonstrated that self-enhancers will typically be perceived more positively by others. This may be due to the fact that self-enhancers have reduced illusory social constraints and form stronger social bonds [106]. Self-enhancers have more extensive social networks, are more associated with leadership behaviors by peers, and experience greater daily contact with loved ones [107]. In a series of experiments examining the relationship between overconfidence and status, it was found that overconfidence would lead to individuals enjoying a higher status in both short-term and long-term groups. Additionally, it was found that overconfidence would make an individual appear more competent to others. Likewise, self-enhancers have been discovered to be perceived as more physically attractive, as shown by Holtzman and Shrube [108] when they found a positive narcissism–attractiveness correlation. Alongside receiving benefits in their perception, self-enhancers have also been found to experience benefits in task performance. O’Mara and Gaertner found that self-enhancers are more confident in performing tasks and, therefore, perform them better [109]. They asked two groups to perform a creative task, but only allowed one group to perform SE prior to the task (they were instructed to exaggerate their creativity in comparison to others). They found that if the participants self-enhance, then participants perform better at creative tasks, such as listing the uses of mundane objects. In terms of further benefits, it appears that being able to self-deceive and self-enhance can provide individuals with a better ability to deceive and enhance others [104].
It has been argued that without SE, individuals would be more susceptible to depression of mood, becoming unmotivated, being negative, etc. [27]. Many individuals, when sad or depressed, will undergo personal adjustment to promote themselves to feel happy [101,110]. Though clearly over-simplified, there are often two ways to undergo personal adjustment, which is either through SE or real-life gains [27,29]. Through pursuing actual benefits, one could attain their goals and accomplish personal satisfaction in a physical (if any) and psychological fashion. Nevertheless, through SE, one can forgo the effort of accomplishing goals and receive psychological satisfaction. Psychologists have argued that going for SE instead of real-life gains can cause serious long-term harm if long-term problems are not solved [101,103]. For example, when individuals were asked to evaluate their academic ability and had their academic abilities tested, individuals who self-enhanced when evaluating their academic abilities appeared to become less motivated and disengaged from academics over time [103]. This disengagement is most likely due to individuals not acknowledging their shortcomings and attempting to better themselves.
SE apparently serves as a buffer against adversity within one’s environment [111]. Distorting reality can help face the harshness and negativity of life’s curveballs that are near impossible to fix. Being in a stressed or depressed state causes individuals to use more energy than usual, disrupting normal metabolic pathways within the brain, accelerating cell injury, and causing unnecessary immune system responses [112]. As the brain is already a voracious consumer of oxygen and sugar [113], it would be disadvantageous for even more energy to be used towards stress and depression. An example of this would be if someone caused a fire by leaving the stove on and, consequently, lost their favorite pet to it. One could go through all the past events that potentially caused the incident. As a result, one would use an exuberant amount of psychological and physical effort to consider what one could have done differently. On the other hand, one could also simply distort their own reality and state that it was not their fault. We have found that affect and self-enhancement are directly tied together in regions of the PFC, as determined via TMS [114]. This suggests at least some degree of mood enhancement, SE, and the PFC. This clearly makes sense as self, emotion, and the PFC are highly related [115,116,117,118,119,120].
Individuals self-enhance to avoid the adversities they face from societal pressures [29,121,122]. The self-centrality principle states that “self-centrality breeds self-enhancement”, or in other words, individuals typically self-enhance the most on traits that they consider central to their self-image [26]. Gebauer et al. (2017) tested this principle using three sets of studies to examine how the self-centrality principle applies to Christian populations. The results provided consistent evidence for the self-centrality principle, discovering that Christians self-enhanced more than non-believers in characteristics that reflected on core Christian beliefs. These characteristics included their knowledge of the different sects of Christianity, knowledge of communion, knowledge of agency, understanding of the commandments of faith, and understanding of the commandments of communion [26]. Most likely, if these traits were left not self-enhanced, the individuals would face psychological stress, as they would label themselves as ignorant or uninformed about the religion they believe in. SE, therefore, allowed them to avoid this stress. This principle also extends to cultural pressures [123,124,125]. Previous studies have shown that individuals self-enhance differently if they are from different cultural backgrounds [126,127,128]. East Asians and Asian Americans have shown fewer signs of self-serving bias when compared to Westerners [129]. Individuals from India have displayed higher levels of optimism than others when predicting the outcomes of negative events [127]. These differences have been attributed to the divergence of collectivist and individualistic cultures, meaning cultures that give more priority to their perceived “group” and cultures that give more priority to the individual, respectively [130]. Other studies suggest that these differences are attributable to cultural differences in modesty and the way it plays a role in societal pressure [131]. Again, individuals differentially modulate what they self-enhance based upon different societal pressures in order to avoid stress and adversity. It is important to note, however, that studying cultural differences in self-enhancement is very difficult and ever changing. This is due to the views of different cultures being heavily affected by the personal biases and experiences of researchers, causing misinterpretations of data [132,133].

4. A New Role for the Prefrontal Cortex

The argument that SE is used as a method to reduce and prevent depression provides a new fundamental role for the PFC. This view is supported by recent data collected by Duran et al., showing how the disruption of regions associated with self-enhancement leads to a significant decrease in mood [114]. Specifically, replicating the parameters of Kwan et al.’s study, it was found that TMS decreased mood following MPFC TMS. These data are comparable to the observations in some clinical populations where MDD is associated with a lack of self-enhancement [114]. Furthermore, social anxiety disorder (SAD) and the self are intimately related. Using behavioral measures, it was first found that the SAD group reported significantly greater embarrassment for self-faced images than the controls. Employing fMRI, the SAD group demonstrated enhanced self-related activation in the left PFC compared to the controls. Interestingly, there was a positive correlation between the self-related activity and the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale in the MPFC [134].
We suggest, here, that the importance of SE is as a reductive and preventative “medicine” against depression, alongside its ability to save psychological and physical energy. This provides the argument that the PFC evolved, in part, to allow for SE to occur. The evolutionary advantages of SE touch on many realms, including task-management, social perception, and one’s own self-concept (Figure 1). It is interesting to consider that hallucinogenic drugs are now being employed in the treatment of numerous disorders, including Major Depressive Disorder [135], and, as such, both the nature of reality and “the true self” are serious topics of scientific inquiry [136].
The statement that self-enhancement provides a positive psychological outlook is a claim that opposes many notions of mental health; therefore, it should be looked at with skepticism. A recent meta-analysis did just this [29]. The study examined more than 125,000 participants and found a direct positive correlation; as self-enhancement increased, so did successful, healthy personal adjustments. Further, the relationship seemed causal (i.e., self-enhancement causes a positive affect), as determined by longitudinal variables. While there were some negative social correlations, the overall impact of self-enhancement was strong and robust. This study, by far the largest on the topic, does not inform us about neurological correlates or if natural selection plays any role in self-enhancement. However, it does support the notion that perhaps the PFC serves as an anti-depressant buffer through altering reality.

Funding

This research was funded by The Weston Foundation, The Louis Stokes Alliance for Minority Participation, the Wehner Foundation, and the Crawford Foundation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Stuss, D.T.; Alexander, M.P.; Lieberman, A.; Levine, H. An extraordinary form of confabulation. Neurology 1978, 28, 1166–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Henri-Bhargava, A.; Stuss, D.T.; Freedman, M. Clinical Assessment of Prefrontal Lobe Functions. Contin. Lifelong Learn. Neurol. 2018, 24, 704–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Goldman-Rakic, P.S. The prefrontal landscape: Implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. In The Prefrontal Cortex Executive and Cognitive Functions; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1998; pp. 87–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Miller, E.K.; Cohen, J.D. An Integrative Theory of Prefrontal Cortex Function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 24, 167–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Seminowicz, D.A.; Moayedi, M. The Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Acute and Chronic Pain. J. Pain 2017, 18, 1027–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Stuss, D.T.; Anderson, V. The frontal lobes and theory of mind: Developmental concepts from adult focal lesion research. Brain Cogn. 2004, 55, 69–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Koski, J.E.; McHaney, J.R.; Rigney, A.E.; Beer, J.S. Reconsidering longstanding assumptions about the role of medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) in social evaluation. Neuroimage 2020, 214, 116752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Stuss, D.T.; Alexander, M.P. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view. Psychol. Res. 2000, 63, 289–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Stuss, D.T.; Rosenbaum, R.S.; Malcolm, S.; Christiana, W.; Keenan, J.P. The Frontal Lobes and Self-Awareness. In The Lost Self; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2005; pp. 50–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Platek, S.M.; Keenan, J.P.; Gallup, G.G., Jr.; Mohamed, F.B. Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. Cogn. Brain Res. 2004, 19, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Platek, S.M.; Kemp, S.M. Is family special to the brain? An event-related fMRI study of familiar, familial, and self-face recognition. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 849–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Platek, S.M.; Krill, A.L.; Wilson, B. Implicit trustworthiness ratings of self-resembling faces activate brain centers involved in reward. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 289–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Platek, S.M.; Loughead, J.W.; Gur, R.C.; Busch, S.; Ruparel, K.; Phend, N.; Panyavin, I.S.; Langleben, D.D. Neural substrates for functionally discriminating self-face from personally familiar faces. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2006, 27, 91–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Harlow, J.M. Recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Hist. Psychiatry 1993, 4, 274–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. O’Driscoll, K.; Leach, J.P. “No longer Gage”: An iron bar through the head. Early observations of personality change after injury to the prefrontal cortex. BMJ 1998, 317, 1673–1674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Blumenfeld, R.S.; Ranganath, C. The lateral prefrontal cortex and human long-term memory. In The Frontal Lobes; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2019; pp. 221–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Eichenbaum, H. Prefrontal–hippocampal interactions in episodic memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 547–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Chau, B.K.H.; Jarvis, H.; Law, C.-K.; Chong, T.T.J. Dopamine and reward: A view from the prefrontal cortex. Behav. Pharmacol. 2018, 29, 569–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Desai, R.H.; Reilly, M.; van Dam, W. The multifaceted abstract brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2018, 373, 20170122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Saez, A.; Rigotti, M.; Ostojic, S.; Fusi, S.; Salzman, C.D. Abstract Context Representations in Primate Amygdala and Prefrontal Cortex. Neuron 2015, 87, 869–881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Dixon, M.L.; Thiruchselvam, R.; Todd, R.; Christoff, K. Emotion and the prefrontal cortex: An integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 143, 1033–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Le Merre, P.; Esmaeili, V.; Charrière, E.; Galan, K.; Salin, P.-A.; Petersen, C.C.H.; Crochet, S. Reward-Based Learning Drives Rapid Sensory Signals in Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Dorsal Hippocampus Necessary for Goal-Directed Behavior. Neuron 2018, 97, 83–91.e5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ott, T.; Nieder, A. Dopamine and Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019, 23, 213–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Klaus, J.; Schutter, D.J.L.G. The Role of Left Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Language Processing. Neuroscience 2018, 377, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Alicke, M.D.; Sedikides, C. Self-enhancement and self-protection: What they are and what they do. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 20, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Gebauer, J.E.; Sedikides, C.; Schrade, A. Christian self-enhancement. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2017, 113, 786–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Taylor, S.E.; Brown, J.D. Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychol. Bull. 1988, 103, 193–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ross, M.; Wilson, A.E. Autobiographical Memory and Conceptions of Self. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 12, 66–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Dufner, M.; Gebauer, J.E.; Sedikides, C.; Denissen, J.J.A. Self-Enhancement and Psychological Adjustment: A Meta-Analytic Review. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 23, 48–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Shepperd, J.A.; Klein, W.M.P.; Waters, E.A.; Weinstein, N.D. Taking Stock of Unrealistic Optimism. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 8, 395–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gaertner, L.; Sedikides, C.; Cai, H. Wanting to Be Great and Better But Not Average. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2012, 43, 521–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sedikides, C. Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 65, 317–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kim, Y.-H.; Chiu, C.-Y. Emotional costs of inaccurate self-assessments: Both self-effacement and self-enhancement can lead to dejection. Emotion 2011, 11, 1096–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Brown, J.D. Evaluations of Self and Others: Self-Enhancement Biases in Social Judgments. Soc. Cogn. 1986, 4, 353–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Barrios, V.; Kwan, V.S.Y.; Ganis, G.; Gorman, J.; Romanowski, J.; Keenan, J.P. Elucidating the neural correlates of egoistic and moralistic self-enhancement. Conscious. Cogn. 2008, 17, 451–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Kwan, V.S.Y.; Barrios, V.; Ganis, G.; Gorman, J.; Lange, C.; Kumar, M.; Shepard, A.; Keenan, J.P. Assessing the neural correlates of self-enhancement bias: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Exp. Brain Res. 2007, 182, 379–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Luber, B.; Lisanby, S.H. Enhancement of human cognitive performance using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Neuroimage 2014, 85 Pt 3, 961–970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. O’Reilly, R.C. The What and How of prefrontal cortical organization. Trends Neurosci. 2010, 33, 355–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Tang, L.; Shafer, A.T.; Ofen, N. Prefrontal Cortex Contributions to the Development of Memory Formation. Cereb. Cortex 2018, 28, 3295–3308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Dietrich, A. How Creativity Happens in the Brain; Palgrave Macmillan UK: London, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Lin, W.-J.; Horner, A.J.; Burgess, N. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex, adding value to autobiographical memories. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 28630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Declerck, C.; Boone, C. Beyond Parochialism. In Neuroeconomics of Prosocial Behavior; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2016; pp. 147–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Carlson, R.W.; Crockett, M.J. The lateral prefrontal cortex and moral goal pursuit. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2018, 24, 77–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Murray, R.J.; Schaer, M.; Debbané, M. Degrees of separation: A quantitative neuroimaging meta-analysis investigating self-specificity and shared neural activation between self- and other-reflection. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 1043–1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ochsner, K.N.; Beer, J.S.; Robertson, E.R.; Cooper, J.C.; Gabrieli, J.D.E.; Kihsltrom, J.F.; D’Esposito, M. The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge. Neuroimage 2005, 28, 797–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Qin, P.; Northoff, G. How is our self related to midline regions and the default-mode network? Neuroimage 2011, 57, 1221–1233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Addis, D.R.; Wong, A.T.; Schacter, D.L. Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 1363–1377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Jankowski, K.F.; Moore, W.E.; Merchant, J.S.; Kahn, L.E.; Pfeifer, J.H. But do you think I’m cool? Developmental differences in striatal recruitment during direct and reflected social self-evaluations. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 40–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Holland, A.C.; Kensinger, E.A. The neural correlates of cognitive reappraisal during emotional autobiographical memory recall. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013, 25, 87–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Schacter, D.L.; Addis, D.R.; Buckner, R.L. Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007, 8, 657–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Schacter, D.L.; Addis, D.R.; Buckner, R.L. Episodic Simulation of Future Events. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1124, 39–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Okuda, J.; Fujii, T.; Ohtake, H.; Tsukiura, T.; Tanji, K.; Suzuki, K.; Kawashima, R.; Fukuda, H.; Itoh, M.; Yamadori, A. Thinking of the future and past: The roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. Neuroimage 2003, 19, 1369–1380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Stawarczyk, D.; D’Argembeau, A. Neural correlates of personal goal processing during episodic future thinking and mind-wandering: An ALE meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015, 36, 2928–2947. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. D’Argembeau, A. Zooming In and Out on One’s Life: Autobiographical Representations at Multiple Time Scales. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2020, 32, 2037–2055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Mitchell, J.P.; Schirmer, J.; Ames, D.L.; Gilbert, D.T. Medial prefrontal cortex predicts intertemporal choice. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 23, 857–866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Harris, A.J.L.; Hahn, U. Unrealistic optimism about future life events: A cautionary note. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 118, 135–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. D’Argembeau, A.; Ortoleva, C.; Jumentier, S.; Van der Linden, M. Component processes underlying future thinking. Mem. Cogn. 2010, 38, 809–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Brosch, T.; Stussi, Y.; Desrichard, O.; Sander, D. Not my future? Core values and the neural representation of future events. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2018, 18, 476–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Bechara, A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nat. Neurosci. 2005, 8, 1458–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Bechara, A.; Damasio, A.R. The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games Econ. Behav. 2005, 52, 336–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. D’Argembeau, A.; Xue, G.; Lu, Z.-L.; Van der Linden, M.; Bechara, A. Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near and far future. Neuroimage 2008, 40, 398–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Ciaramelli, E.; Muccioli, M.; Làdavas, E.; di Pellegrino, G. Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2007, 2, 84–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Bruch, E.; Feinberg, F. Decision-Making Processes in Social Contexts. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2017, 43, 207–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Eslinger, P.J.; Robinson-Long, M.; Realmuto, J.; Moll, J.; deOliveira-Souza, R.; Tovar-Moll, F.; Wang, J.; Yang, Q.X. Developmental frontal lobe imaging in moral judgment: Arthur Benton’s enduring influence 60 years later. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2009, 31, 158–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Reniers, R.L.E.P.; Corcoran, R.; Völlm, B.A.; Mashru, A.; Howard, R.; Liddle, P.F. Moral decision-making, ToM, empathy and the default mode network. Biol. Psychol. 2012, 90, 202–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Goodfriend, W.; Arriaga, X.B. Cognitive Reframing of Intimate Partner Aggression: Social and Contextual Influences. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 2464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Pincus, A.L.; Cain, N.M.; Wright, A.G.C. Narcissistic grandiosity and narcissistic vulnerability in psychotherapy. Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 2014, 5, 439–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Fonagy, P.; Gergely, G.; Jurist, E.L.; Target, M. The Roots of Borderline Personality Disorder in Disorganized Attachment. In Affect Regulation, Mentalization, and the Development of the Self; Routledge: London, UK, 2018; pp. 343–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Ronningstam, E. Intersect between self-esteem and emotion regulation in narcissistic personality disorder–implications for alliance building and treatment. Bord. Personal. Disord. Emot. Dysregulat. 2017, 4, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Bateman, A.W. Thick- and thin-skinned organisations and enactment in borderline and narcissistic disorders. In Key Papers on Borderline Disorders; Routledge: London, UK, 2018; pp. 11–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Ritter, K.; Vater, A.; Rüsch, N.; Schröder-Abé, M.; Schütz, A.; Fydrich, T.; Lammers, C.-H.; Roepke, S. Shame in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2014, 215, 429–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Baskin-Sommers, A.; Krusemark, E.; Ronningstam, E. Empathy in narcissistic personality disorder: From clinical and empirical perspectives. Pers. Disord 2014, 5, 323–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Fan, Y.; Wonneberger, C.; Enzi, B.; de Greck, M.; Ulrich, C.; Tempelmann, C.; Bogerts, B.; Doering, S.; Northoff, G. The narcissistic self and its psychological and neural correlates: An exploratory fMRI study. Psychol. Med. 2011, 41, 1641–1650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Scalabrini, A.; Huang, Z.; Mucci, C.; Perrucci, M.G.; Ferretti, A.; Fossati, A.; Romani, G.L.; Northoff, G.; Ebisch, S.J.H. How spontaneous brain activity and narcissistic features shape social interaction. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 9986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Kramer, R.; Duran, K.; Soder, H.; Applegate, L.; Youssef, A.; Criscione, M.; Keenan, J.P. The Special Brain: Subclinical Grandiose Narcissism and Self-Face Recognition in the Right Prefrontal Cortex. Am. J. Psychol. 2020, 133, 487–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. van der Meer, L.; de Vos, A.E.; Stiekema, A.P.M.; Pijnenborg, G.H.M.; van Tol, M.-J.; Nolen, W.A.; David, A.S.; Aleman, A. Insight in schizophrenia: Involvement of self-reflection networks? Schizophr. Bull. 2013, 39, 1288–1295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Gusnard, D.A.; Raichle, M.E. Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 2, 685–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Kelley, W.M.; Macrae, C.N.; Wyland, C.L.; Caglar, S.; Inati, S.; Heatherton, T.F. Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002, 14, 785–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Macrae, C.N. Medial Prefrontal Activity Predicts Memory for Self. Cerebral. Cortex 2004, 14, 647–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Gusnard, D.A.; Akbudak, E.; Shulman, G.L.; Raichle, M.E. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 4259–4264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Leszkowicz, E.; Maio, G.R.; Linden, D.E.J.; Ihssen, N. Neural coding of human values is underpinned by brain areas representing the core self in the cortical midline region. Soc. Neurosci. 2021, 16, 486–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Klayman, J. Varieties of Confirmation Bias. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1995; pp. 385–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Tversky, A.; Kahneman, D. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science 1974, 185, 1124–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Beer, J.S.; Lombardo, M.V.; Bhanji, J.P. Roles of medial prefrontal cortex and orbitofrontal cortex in self-evaluation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 2108–2119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Pallier, G.; Wilkinson, R.; Danthiir, V.; Kleitman, S.; Knezevic, G.; Stankov, L.; Roberts, R.D. The Role of Individual Differences in the Accuracy of Confidence Judgments. J. Gen. Psychol. 2002, 129, 257–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Falk, C.F.; Heine, S.J. What Is Implicit Self-Esteem, and Does it Vary Across Cultures? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2014, 19, 177–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Kahneman, D.; Knetsch, J.L.; Thaler, R.H. Experimental Tests of the Endowment Effect and the Coase Theorem. J. Political Econ. 1990, 98, 1325–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Hastorf, A.H.; Cantril, H. They saw a game. A case study. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1954, 49, 129–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Murray, S.L.; Holmes, J.G.; Griffin, D.W. The self-fulfilling nature of positive illusions in romantic relationships: Love is not blind, but prescient. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 71, 1155–1180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Amati, F.; Oh, H.; Kwan, V.S.Y.; Jordan, K.; Keenan, J.P. Overclaiming and the medial prefrontal cortex: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 1, 268–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Taylor-Lillquist, B.; Kanpa, V.; Crawford, M.; El Filali, M.; Oakes, J.; Jonasz, A.; Disney, A.; Keenan, J.P. Preliminary Evidence of the Role of Medial Prefrontal Cortex in Self-Enhancement: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Butler, J.R.; Nelson, N.L. Children overclaim more knowledge than adults do, but for different reasons. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2021, 201, 104969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Ross, M.Q.; Sterling-Maisel, O.A.; Tracy, O.; Putnam, A.L. Overclaiming responsibility in fictitious countries: Unpacking the role of availability in support theory predictions of overclaiming. Mem. Cognit. 2020, 48, 1346–1358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Steger, D.; Schroeders, U.; Wilhelm, O. Caught in the Act: Predicting Cheating in Unproctored Knowledge Assessment. Assessment 2021, 28, 1004–1017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Yamashiro, J.K.; Roediger, H.L., 3rd. Biased collective memories and historical overclaiming: An availability heuristic account. Mem. Cognit. 2021, 49, 311–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Gerrard, M.; Gibbons, F.X.; Reis-Bergan, M.; Russell, D.W. Self-esteem, self-serving cognitions, and health risk behavior. J. Personal. 2000, 68, 1177–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Humberg, S.; Dufner, M.; Schönbrodt, F.D.; Geukes, K.; Hutteman, R.; Küfner, A.C.P.; van Zalk, M.H.W.; Denissen, J.J.A.; Nestler, S.; Back, M.D. Is accurate, positive, or inflated self-perception most advantageous for psychological adjustment? A competitive test of key hypotheses. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2019, 116, 835–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Zhang, S.; Roberts, R.; Woodman, T.; Cooke, A. I Am Great, but Only When I Also Want to Dominate: Maladaptive Narcissism Moderates the Relationship Between Adaptive Narcissism and Performance Under Pressure. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2020, 42, 323–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Jahoda, M. Current Concepts of Positive Mental Health; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Holyoke, T.C.; Vaillant, G.E. Adaptation to Life. Antioch Rev. 1978, 36, 246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Colvin, C.R.; Block, J.; Funder, D.C. Overly positive self-evaluations and personality: Negative implications for mental health. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 68, 1152–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Allport, G.W. Restoring Morale in Occupied Territory. Public Opin. Q. 1943, 7, 606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Robins, R.W.; Beer, J.S. Positive illusions about the self: Short-term benefits and long-term costs. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 80, 340–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. von Hippel, W.; Trivers, R. The evolution and psychology of self-deception. Behav. Brain Sci. 2011, 34, 26–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Schwardmann, P.; van der Weele, J. Deception and self-deception. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2019, 3, 1055–1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Goorin, L.; Bonanno, G.A. Would You Buy a Used Car from a Self-enhancer? Social Benefits and Illusions in Trait Self-enhancement. Self Identity 2009, 8, 162–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Anderson, C.; Brion, S.; Moore, D.A.; Kennedy, J.A. A status-enhancement account of overconfidence. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 103, 718–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Holtzman, N.S.; Vazire, S.; Mehl, M.R. Sounds like a Narcissist: Behavioral Manifestations of Narcissism in Everyday Life. J. Res. Personal. 2010, 44, 478–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. O’Mara, E.M.; Gaertner, L. Does self-enhancement facilitate task performance? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2017, 146, 442–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Kurt, A.; Paulhus, D.L. Moderators of the adaptiveness of self-enhancement: Operationalization, motivational domain, adjustment facet, and evaluator. J. Res. Personal. 2008, 42, 839–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Bonanno, G.A.; Field, N.P.; Kovacevic, A.; Kaltman, S. Self-Enhancement as a Buffer Against Extreme Adversity: Civil War in Bosnia and Traumatic Loss in the United States. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2002, 28, 184–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Østergaard, L.; Jørgensen, M.B.; Knudsen, G.M. Low on energy? An energy supply-demand perspective on stress and depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 94, 248–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Posner, J. Increased default mode network connectivity in individuals at high familial risk for depression. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2016, 55, S306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Duran, K.A.; O’Halloran, H.; Soder, H.; Yasin, S.; Kramer, R.; Rosen, S.; Brenya, J.; Chavarria, K.; Savitska, L.; Keenan, J.P. The medial prefrontal cortex: A potential link between self-deception and affect. Int. J. Neurosci. 2020, 131, 701–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Alarcón, G.; Sauder, M.; Teoh, J.Y.; Forbes, E.E.; Quevedo, K. Amygdala Functional Connectivity During Self-Face Processing in Depressed Adolescents With Recent Suicide Attempt. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2019, 58, 221–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Liu, G.; Zhang, N.; Teoh, J.Y.; Egan, C.; Zeffiro, T.A.; Davidson, R.J.; Quevedo, K. Self-compassion and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity during sad self-face recognition in depressed adolescents. Psychol. Med. 2022, 52, 864–873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Quevedo, K.; Harms, M.; Sauder, M.; Scott, H.; Mohamed, S.; Thomas, K.M.; Schallmo, M.P.; Smyda, G. The neurobiology of self face recognition among depressed adolescents. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 229, 22–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Quevedo, K.; Liu, G.; Teoh, J.Y.; Ghosh, S.; Zeffiro, T.; Ahrweiler, N.; Zhang, N.; Wedan, R.; Oh, S.; Guercio, G.; et al. Neurofeedback and neuroplasticity of visual self-processing in depressed and healthy adolescents: A preliminary study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019, 40, 100707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Quevedo, K.; Ng, R.; Scott, H.; Martin, J.; Smyda, G.; Keener, M.; Oppenheimer, C.W. The neurobiology of self-face recognition in depressed adolescents with low or high suicidality. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2016, 125, 1185–1200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Quevedo, K.; Yuan Teoh, J.; Engstrom, M.; Wedan, R.; Santana-Gonzalez, C.; Zewde, B.; Porter, D.; Cohen Kadosh, K. Amygdala Circuitry During Neurofeedback Training and Symptoms’ Change in Adolescents With Varying Depression. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Salvador, C.E.; Kamikubo, A.; Kraus, B.; Hsiao, N.C.; Hu, J.F.; Karasawa, M.; Kitayama, S. Self-referential processing accounts for cultural variation in self-enhancement versus criticism: An electrocortical investigation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2021, 151, 1904–1918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Xia, R.; Su, W.; Wang, F.; Li, S.; Zhou, A.; Lyu, D. The Moderation Effect of Self-Enhancement on the Group-Reference Effect. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Chen, X.J.; Geagea, A.; Park, J.; Kwak, Y. Cultural modulation of early attentional responses to positive self-information: An ERP investigation of self-enhancement. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2020, 158, 34–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Kim, M.Y.; Han, K. For me or for others? The better-than-average effect and negative feelings toward average others during the COVID-19 pandemic. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Wetzel, E.; Lang, F.J.; Back, M.D.; Vecchione, M.; Rogoza, R.; Roberts, B.W. Measurement Invariance of Three Narcissism Questionnaires Across the United States, the United Kingdom, and Germany. Assessment 2021, 28, 29–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Cai, H.; Wu, L.; Shi, Y.; Gu, R.; Sedikides, C. Self-enhancement among Westerners and Easterners: A cultural neuroscience approach. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016, 11, 1569–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Joshi, M.S.; Carter, W. Unrealistic optimism: East and west? Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Kurman, J. Measured Cross-Cultural Differences in Self-Enhancement and the Sensitivity of the Self-Enhancement Measure to the Modesty Response. Cross-Cult. Res. 2002, 36, 73–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Heine, S.J.; Hamamura, T. In Search of East Asian Self-Enhancement. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2007, 11, 4–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Markus, H.R.; Kitayama, S. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 1991, 98, 224–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Kurman, J. Why is Self-Enhancement Low in Certain Collectivist Cultures? J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2003, 34, 496–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Takano, Y.; Osaka, E. An unsupported common view: Comparing Japan and the U.S. on individualism/collectivism. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 2, 311–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Takano, Y.; Sogon, S. Are Japanese More Collectivistic Than Americans? J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2008, 39, 237–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Kawaguchi, A.; Morita, T.; Okamoto, Y.; Nakaaki, S.; Kawaguchi, T.; Kan, H.; Yamada, T.; Akechi, T.; Sadato, N. Neural Basis of Self-Reflection on Self-Face Image in Patients with Social Anxiety Disorder. Anxiety Disord. Res. 2018, 10, 29–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Sherwood, A.M.; Prisinzano, T.E. Novel psychotherapeutics—A cautiously optimistic focus on Hallucinogens. Expert. Rev. Clin. Pharmacol. 2018, 11, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Halberstadt, A.L. Recent advances in the neuropsychopharmacology of serotonergic hallucinogens. Behav. Brain Res. 2015, 277, 99–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Yoon, L.; Kim, K.; Jung, D.; Kim, H. Roles of the MPFC and insula in impression management under social observation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2021, 16, 474–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Nenadić, I.; Lorenz, C.; Gaser, C. Narcissistic personality traits and prefrontal brain structure. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 15707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Teed, A.R.; Rakic, J.; Mark, D.B.; Krawcyzk, D.C. Relative activation patterns associated with self-transcendent and self-enhancement core values: An fMRI study of basic human values theory concepts in males. Soc. Neurosci. 2020, 15, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Kim, H. Stability or Plasticity?—A Hierarchical Allostatic Regulation Model of Medial Prefrontal Cortex Function for Social Valuation. Front. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  141. Parrish, M.H.; Dutcher, J.M.; Muscatell, K.A.; Inagaki, T.K.; Moieni, M.; Irwin, M.R.; Eisenberger, N.I. Frontostriatal functional connectivity underlies self-enhancement during social evaluation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2022, 17, 723–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. The PFC includes numerous subdivisions. The basic divisions are noted here in medial sagittal, lateral sagittal, and axial orientations. While there is not enough research on SE to draw firm delineations, here, we note differences that may exist. The rmPFC is involved in impression management [137], which may include the general SE abilities of the medial PFC [91,137,138,139]. The ventral PFC regions (sgACC, mOFC, and VLPFC) are involved in social SE [140,141]. The lateral regions of the PFC (DLPFCVLPFC) are involved in long-term SE of the core self [9].
Figure 1. The PFC includes numerous subdivisions. The basic divisions are noted here in medial sagittal, lateral sagittal, and axial orientations. While there is not enough research on SE to draw firm delineations, here, we note differences that may exist. The rmPFC is involved in impression management [137], which may include the general SE abilities of the medial PFC [91,137,138,139]. The ventral PFC regions (sgACC, mOFC, and VLPFC) are involved in social SE [140,141]. The lateral regions of the PFC (DLPFCVLPFC) are involved in long-term SE of the core self [9].
Brainsci 12 01103 g001
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yasin, S.; Fierst, A.; Keenan, H.; Knapp, A.; Gallione, K.; Westlund, T.; Kirschner, S.; Vaidya, S.; Qiu, C.; Rougebec, A.; et al. Self-Enhancement and the Medial Prefrontal Cortex: The Convergence of Clinical and Experimental Findings. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1103. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081103

AMA Style

Yasin S, Fierst A, Keenan H, Knapp A, Gallione K, Westlund T, Kirschner S, Vaidya S, Qiu C, Rougebec A, et al. Self-Enhancement and the Medial Prefrontal Cortex: The Convergence of Clinical and Experimental Findings. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(8):1103. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081103

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yasin, Saeed, Anjel Fierst, Harper Keenan, Amelia Knapp, Katrina Gallione, Tessa Westlund, Sydney Kirschner, Sahana Vaidya, Christina Qiu, Audrey Rougebec, and et al. 2022. "Self-Enhancement and the Medial Prefrontal Cortex: The Convergence of Clinical and Experimental Findings" Brain Sciences 12, no. 8: 1103. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081103

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop