Next Article in Journal
Working Memory-Related Neurofunctional Correlates Associated with the Frontal Lobe in Children with Familial vs. Non-Familial Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Speech, Gait, and Vestibular Function in Cerebellar Ataxia with Neuropathy and Vestibular Areflexia Syndrome
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

The Effect of Beta-Carotene on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review

by
Diana Marisol Abrego-Guandique
1,2,
Maria Luisa Bonet
3,4,5,
Maria Cristina Caroleo
1,2,
Roberto Cannataro
2,6,
Paola Tucci
7,
Joan Ribot
3,4,5 and
Erika Cione
2,7,*
1
Department of Health Sciences, University of Magna Graecia Catanzaro, 88100 Catanzaro, Italy
2
Galascreen Laboratories, University of Calabria, 87036 Rende, Italy
3
Laboratory of Molecular Biology, Nutrition, and Biotechnology (LBNB), Universitat de les Illes Balears, 07122 Palma, Spain
4
Institut d’Investigació Sanitària Illes Balears (IdISBa), 07120 Palma, Spain
5
CIBER de Fisiopatología de la Obesidad y Nutrición (CIBERobn), 07122 Palma, Spain
6
Research Division, Dynamical Business & Science Society, DBSS International SAS, Bogota 110311, Colombia
7
Department of Pharmacy, Health and Nutritional Sciences, University of Calabria, 87036 Rende, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(10), 1468; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13101468
Submission received: 17 September 2023 / Revised: 11 October 2023 / Accepted: 15 October 2023 / Published: 17 October 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Nutrition in Neurodegenerative Diseases: Prevention and Treatment)

Abstract

:
β-carotene is a powerful antioxidant and dietary precursor of vitamin A whose role in maintaining mental health and cognitive performance, either alone or in combination with other dietary compounds, has been a topic of recent research. However, its effectiveness is still unclear. This systematic review, conducted according to the PRISMA guideline and assisted by the MySLR platform, addressed this issue. A total of 16 eligible original research articles were identified. Dietary intake or β-carotene serum levels were associated with improved measures of cognitive function in 7 out of 10 epidemiological studies included. In intervention studies, β-carotene consumption alone did not promote better cognitive function in the short term, but only in a long-term intervention with a mean duration of 18 years. However, all but one intervention study suggested the beneficial effects of β-carotene supplementation at doses ranging from 6 mg to 50 mg per day in combination with a multicomplex such as vitamin E, vitamin C, zinc, or selenium for a period of 16 weeks to 20 years. Despite the current limitations, the available evidence suggests a potential association between β-carotene dietary/supplementary intake and the maintenance of cognitive function. The β-carotene most probably does not act alone but in synergy with other micronutrients.

1. Introduction

Cognitive functions are mental domains that enable us to receive, process, and elaborate information. These functions include complex attention, executive function, language, learning and memory, perceptual-motor function, and social cognition. There is continuous coordination and switching between memory, attention, and executive function sub-processes [1]. Cognitive dysfunction can be defined as a disruption or lack of equilibrium within the brain’s structural and functional organization at distinct levels: the molecular/cellular level, the level of local circuits, and the level of large-scale networks that involve neural interactions, protein–protein interaction networks, and gene–miRNA interactions [2]. miRNAs have potential as biomarkers [3] and can be associated with poor nutritional status [4].
Cognitive behaviors are crucial for individuals’ overall health and well-being regardless of their specific condition. Maintaining optimal cognitive function is essential for daily functioning, learning, work performance, and overall quality of life [5]. Cognitive function is vital for athletes’ sports performance because it affects their decision-making skills and reaction time. It may be more relevant in open sports that require constant attention or adaptation to changing situations [6,7].
Cognitive function evolves throughout life. The likelihood of experiencing mild cognitive impairment increases with age [8]. As people live longer, cognitive impairment is becoming more prevalent, posing a significant public health challenge. The prevalence of cognitive impairment is estimated to be 19% for those under 75 years old and 29% for those over 85. In addition, 30% of people over 65 are diagnosed with Alzheimer’s [9]. With the rise in the number of older adults, these numbers are expected to increase. Additionally, chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment, often called “chemobrain”, can affect cancer patients during and after treatment [10,11]. Moreover, many conditions are associated with brain fog, a nebulous concept that eluded scientific examination until the COVID-19 outbreak put it under the spotlight [12,13,14]. Therefore, it is crucial to develop strategies to combat cognitive decline, with prevention by diet changes or supplementation seen as a possible strategy.
There is evidence that oxidative damage contributes to age-related cognitive decline [15]. Among the leading causes of cognitive decline, the increase in oxidative stress in the aging brain is widely documented in humans [16]. Oxidative stress also underlies chemotherapy-induced mild cognitive impairment. Chemotherapy causes lipid peroxidation, depleting cellular levels of antioxidant molecules such as minerals and vitamins, which in turn generates free radicals contributing to oxidative stress [17].
Considering that carotenoids act as antioxidants and anti-inflammatory agents [18], their intake in the diet could be a prevention strategy to maintain and improve cognitive health. No specific recommendations regarding the intake of carotenoids have been issued to date. The total carotenoid intake in European countries (median values) ranges from ~9.5 to 16 mg/d (~3 to 6 mg/d for β-carotene) [19]. β-carotene is the most important natural carotenoid and the primary dietary source of pro-vitamin A. It is known for its antioxidant properties and free radical scavenging actions due to abundant unsaturated bonds in its molecule. It contributes to about 30–35% of the dietary intake of vitamin A in Western countries, while in developing countries it is the most important source of vitamin A [20]. β-carotene is found in vegetables, fruits, and soup/bouillon [21] and it is sometimes used as a food coloring additive. The importance of vitamin A spans from vision to sustaining immunity and fertility [22,23,24]. Therefore, it is important to understand the impact of β-carotene on cognitive function and consider its action, either alone or in combination with other bioactive compounds, for future intervention in this specific area.
We here conducted a systematic review of studies combining psychological approaches and nutrition with the aim of evaluating the impact of β-carotene on cognitive domains in adults. The review was facilitated by a semi-automated tool that, after the identification and loading of databases on the platform, selects and evaluates quality contributions and analyzes and synthesizes the results. In particular, we used the MySLR digital platform, a digital tool that implements the Latent Dirichlet Allocation (LDA) algorithm [25] to analyze a large number of scientific publications using text mining. To date, findings regarding the possibility that dietary intake or supplementation with β-carotene and interaction may delay the onset of cognitive decline or even ameliorate cognitive performance remain inconsistent across studies. There is currently no published systematic review or meta-analysis to our knowledge that provides a quantitative measure of the association between β-carotene and cognitive function. Therefore, a systematic review was conducted to evaluate the potential clinical effects of β-carotene intake on cognition in adults.

2. Methods

The systematic review was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [26] and registered at the PROSPERO International Prospective Registry (CRD42023422784). The methodological approach is based on three steps: (i) paper location and selection, (ii) paper analysis, and (iii) results presentation. We adopted a semi-automated approach using the MySLR platform upon registration (available at https://myslr.unical.it, accessed on 31 May 2023). This digital tool reproduces “human-like intelligence” as closely as possible by implementing the LDA algorithm. We loaded the papers on MySLR to offer a complete and exhaustive overview of scientific research.

2.1. Paper Location and Selection

Two investigators (D.M.A-G. and E.C.) independently conducted the searches on the PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science databases to identify publications in peer-reviewed journals published before 31 May 2023. The search was conducted using the Boolean operators “AND” and “OR” to combine the following terms: (“Cognition” OR “Cognitive function” OR “Cognitive Dysfunction” OR “Cognitive decline” OR “Cognitive Outcomes” OR “Cognitive impairment” OR “Mild cognitive impairment”) AND (“Memory” OR “Attention” OR “Executive Function” OR “Working Memory” OR “processing speed” OR “visuospatial” OR “verbal fluency” OR “word fluency” OR “Learning” OR “Thinking”) AND (“beta carotene” OR “beta-carotene” OR “beta-carotene” OR “βcarotene” OR “β-carotene” OR “β carotene”).

2.2. Study Selection and Data Extraction

Studies were included in the systematic review that assessed the effect of β-carotene alone or in combination with other compounds on cognitive function, which were all published before 31 May 2023. The inclusion criteria for this systematic review were as follows:
Population: studies in humans conducted with participants ≥18 years of age without mental disorders.
Intervention: studies that reported the β-carotene content of foods or dose-containing supplements or studies that reported blood levels of β-carotene.
Outcomes: studies that provided sufficient information about cognitive outcomes.
Types of Study: randomized clinical trials or prospective (cross-sectional study) or longitudinal study.
Articles were excluded from the systematic review for the following reasons: studies not published in English; reviews, meta-analysis, letters, conference papers, comments, or book chapters; studies on animal models or in vitro experiments.
Disagreements were resolved through discussion in order to reach a consensus or by means of a third reviewer (M.L.B. and M.C.C.)
Data from all included articles were extracted by one author (D.M.A-G.) and checked by two authors (E.C. and M.L.B.). The following information was recorded: authors’ names, publication year, study country, study design, participant characteristics (sample size, gender, and age), dietary intake/intervention/blood levels, outcomes of interest, cognitive tests used, and results. The levels of evidence within each condition are evaluated using a levels of evidence framework for quantitative research on intervention studies that was adapted from Melnyk and Fineout-Overholt [27].

2.3. Quality Assessment and Levels of Evidence

The possibility of bias in the design and analysis of each study was assessed by two different evaluators (D.M.A-G. and E.C.) using the NIH Study Quality Assessment Tool https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools (accessed on 10 June 2023) and three more reviewers were consulted when necessary (R.C., P.T., and J.R.). Specifically, the Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-sectional Studies and the Quality Assessment of Controlled Intervention Studies were used; both forms have 14 questions designed to help focus on the key concepts for evaluating the internal validity of a study, such as the risk of potential selection, method, information, measurement, and confounding bias.

2.4. Results Presentation

The final stage of the methodological approach is explained in the sections “Results” and “Discussion.” This step aims to comprehensively define and analyze the outcomes obtained from the MySLR procedure. It involves meticulously examining the relevant literature to describe and discuss the results clearly.

3. Results

A total of 168 records were identified from the initial literature search of the three databases (PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science). After excluding the duplicate records, 115 items remained. Of these, MySLR removed 37 papers because they were reviews, book chapters, meta-analyses, or other irrelevant publications for our systematic review. Of the 78 studies remaining, 39 were discarded based on their abstracts. After full-text reading and analysis, 23 records were further excluded for failing to meet the inclusion/exclusion criteria. Therefore, a total of 16 studies were considered eligible. The PRISMA flowchart in Figure 1 shows the selection of the studies for this systematic review.
Epidemiological studies on the relationship between β-carotene and cognitive function have not shown a regular trend over time, as shown in Figure 2. The first study was published in 1997 [28]. Four scientific articles were published in the last five years [29,30,31,32].

3.1. Characteristics of the Studies Included

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the studies reporting dietary β-carotene or β-carotene supplementation, while Table 2 summarizes those of the studies reporting blood levels of β-carotene. Articles in both tables appear in descending order by year of publication.
Of the 16 eligible studies, 4 were prospective studies, 5 were cross-sectional studies, and 7 were randomized intervention studies. The studies were published from 1997 to 2023 and conducted in seven countries: the United States, Germany, France, Switzerland, the United Kingdom, China, and the Netherlands. Nine studies were conducted in both sexes [28,29,33,34,35,36,37,38,39], whereas two studies were exclusively conducted in males [30,40] and five in females [31,32,41,42,43]. The ages of all the participants ranged from 30 to 100 years (mean 53.2 ± 13.6).
All studies measured cognitive function in healthy subjects, except for two studies focused on participants affected by low cognitive function [29] and mild cognitive impairment [32] and one study in women with cardiovascular disease [42].
Table 1. Studies reporting dietary β-carotene or supplementation.
Table 1. Studies reporting dietary β-carotene or supplementation.
StudyLocationLevel of EvidenceStudy DesignParticipant CharacteristicsDietary Intake/InterventionConditionsOutcome of
Interest
Cognitive Test
Used
Reference
Zhong et al. (2023)USA4Cross-sectionaln = 2009; 985 males and 1024 females; aged >60 yDietary BC intake:
Q1 (≤338 μg/d);
Q2 (338 to ≤819 μg/d);
Q3 (819 to ≤2222.5 μg/d);
Q4 (>2222.5 μg/d)
Participants in NHANES 2011–2014 older than 60 y that had completed data on cognitive functionMemory
Processing speed and working memory
Categorical verbal fluency
CERAD WL
DSST
AFT
[29]
Beydoun et al. (2020)USA3Prospective cohortn = 1251; male; aged 30–65 y at baselineDietary BC intake:
overall, 1819 ± 2882 μg/d;
T1 = 309 ± 289 μg/d;
T2 = 1226 ± 987 μg/d;
T3 = 3980 ± 4152 μg/d
Healthy subjects; BC intake assessed at V1; cognitive performance assessed at V1 (2004–2009) and V2 (2009–2013)Global cognition
Attention
Learning/memory
Executive function
Visuo-spatial/visuo-construction ability
Psychomotor speed
Language/verbal
MMSE
CVLT
Digit span test
BVRT
AFT
BTA
TMT A/B
CDT
[30]
Yuan et al. (2020)USA3Prospective cohortn = 49493; female (nurses); mean age 48 yDietary BC intake:
Q1 = 2.5 ± 0.5 mg/d; without supplements 2.3 ± 5.4 mg/d;
Q5 = 8.6 ± 2.1 mg/d; without supplements 6.9 ± 2.1 mg/d
Healthy subjects; FFQs collected at baseline and periodically until 2006; self-reported SCF in 2012 and 2014General memory Executive function Attention Visuospatial skillsAssessment of SCF based on 7 yes/no questions on recent changes[31]
Li et al. (2015)China2Interventionn = 276; 116 male and 160 female; aged 67.06 ± 5.33 yGroups A, B, C, and D received 200 mg/d VE and 300 mg/d VC, combined with 16.7, 8.4, 5.6, or 0 mg/d BC, respectively. Group E: 5 mg/d VEHealthy subjects; 5 groups A-E (n = 60 per group, there were 24 drop outs); 16 weeks of intervention; cognitive function assessed prior to and after the interventionGlobal cognitive functionMMSE
HDS
[33]
Nooyens et al. (2015)Netherland3Prospective cohortn = 2613; both sexes; aged 43–70 y at baselineDietary intake at baseline and follow-up through a validated self-administered semi-quantitative FFQ. Estimated BC intake: 1480 ± 593 μg/dHealthy subjects; cognitive performance assessed 5 y after baselineGlobal cognitive function
Memory
Processing speed
Cognitive flexibility
15 Words Learning test
Stroop test
WFT
Letter digit substitution test
[34]
Kesse-Guyot et al. (2011)France2Randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trialn = 4447; both sexes; aged 45–60 yMultivitamin (6 mg/d BC) or placeboHealthy subjects; 8 y of intervention; cognitive performance assessed 6 y after the end of the interventionEpisodic memory
Executive function
Verbal memory
Verbal fluency
TMT
FDST
RI-48
Semantic fluency
Phonemic fluency
[37]
Péneau et al. (2011)France3Prospective cohortn = 2533; male and female; aged 45–60 y at baselineDietary intake of FVs grouped based on their nutrient content: folate-rich FVs and BC-rich FVsHealthy subjects; cognitive performance assessed 13 y after baselineEpisodic memory
Lexical semantic memory
Mental flexibility
Working memory
RI-48
Verbal fluency test
TMT
FDST
[38]
Kang et al. (2009)USA22 × 2 × 2 randomized placebo-controlled trialn = 2824; female; aged >65 yBC (50 mg every other day) or placebo alone or combined with VE, VC, or bothWomen with cardiovascular disease; 3.5 y of interventionGlobal cognition
Verbal memory
Category fluency
TICS
EBMT
CFT
[42]
Grodstein et al. (2007)USA2Randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trialn = 4052; male; mean age 55.9 y50 mg BC on alternate daysHealthy subjects;
18 y of intervention
Global cognition
Verbal memory
Category fluency
TICS
EBMT
CFT
[40]
Wolters et al. (2005)Germany2Randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trialn = 220; female; aged 60–91 yMultivitamin (9 mg/d BC) or placeboHealthy subjects, free-living women; 6 months of interventionTotal Intellectual Quotient
Intelligence
Assessment of mild up to severe memory disorders
WAIS-III
KAI
BAT
[41]
Smith et al. (1999)UK2Randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trialn = 205; male and female; aged 60–80 yMultivitamin (12 mg/d BC) or placebo12 months of interventionEpisodic memory
Psychomotor speed
Attention
NART score
CFQ score
[39]
T, tertile; Q, quintile; V, visit; VC, vitamin C; VE, vitamin E. Other abbreviations are presented in the Abbreviations section.
Table 2. Studies reporting serum levels of β-carotene.
Table 2. Studies reporting serum levels of β-carotene.
StudyLocationLevel of EvidenceStudy DesignParticipant CharacteristicsBlood LevelsConditionOutcome of InterestCognitive Test UsedReference
Gerger et al. (2019) Germany2Multi-centered randomized controlled trialn = 56; female; aged 73.1 ± 5,8 yBC (μM): 0.74 ± 0.65Subjects with mild cognitive impairment participating in the NeuroExercise studyGlobal cognition
Verbal memory
Working memory
Attention
Executive function
MoCA
ISLT
ONB
TMT
[32]
Johnson et al. (2013)USA(Georgia)4Cross-sectional n = 298, of which 78 were octogenarians and 220 centenarians; male and femaleBC (nmol/L):
octogenarians, 568 ± 855; centenarians, 460 ± 432
Institutionalized and community-dwelling subjects compared; study conducted from 2001 to 2009 Memory processing speed
Attention
Executive functioning
MMSE
GDRS
SIB
FOME
WAIS-III
Similarities subtest
BDS
COWAT
[35]
Akbaraly et al. (2007)France4Cross-sectionaln = 589; 361 female and 228 male; aged 73.5 ± 3 yTrans-BC levels, 0.73 ± 0.52 μmol/L; cis-BC levels, 0.10 ± 0.12 μmol/LHealthy old subjectsGlobal cognition
Motor speed
Working memory
Executive function
Attention and logical reasoning
Verbal fluency
MMSE
TMTA
TMTB
DSST
WFT
[36]
Perkins et al. (1999) USA4Cross-sectional n = 4809; female; aged >60 yBC normalized on units of total cholesterol: <0.06; 0.06–0.09; 0.09–0.15; >0.15Elderly, multi-ethnic group followed from 1988 to 1994MemoryAssessed using delayed recall (6 points from a story and 3 words), with poor memory defined by a combined score < 4[43]
Perrig et al. (1997)Switzerland4Longitudinal and cross-sectional comparisonsn = 442;
312 male, 132 female;
aged 65 to 94 y
BC measured in 1971 (T1) and 1993 (T2); BC levels (µg/dL):
T1, 0.51 ± 0.31;
T2, 0.72 ± 0.48
Healthy old subjects
selected in T2 by random sampling from a large cohort established in T1; cognitive function assessed in T2
Implicit and explicit memory
Working memory
Semantic memory
Free recall and recognitionWAIS-R
Vocabulary test
[28]
T, time. Other abbreviations are presented in the Abbreviations section.
The systematic review included studies that utilized a wide variety of cognitive test batteries (Figure 3), such as the MMSE [30,33,35,36], TICS [40,42], MoCA [32], and SIB [35] for global cognition. Memory was assessed at different levels (lexical and semantic memory, through verbal fluency tests [34,38], the EBMT [40,42], the CFT [29,40,42], the COWAT [35], and the WFT [34,36]; working memory and speed processing, through the digit span test which includes the FDST [30,37,38], CERAD WL [29], ONB [32], and DSST [29,36]; episodic memory, with RI-48 [37,38] and FOME [30]; and verbal memory, with the CVLT [30] and ISLT [32]), whereas visual memory/visual perception was evaluated using the BVRT [30]. The KAI, BAT, and WAIS-III/R tests were administered for Total Intellectual Quotient and Intelligence [28,35,41]. The relationship between motor coordination and executive function was evaluated using the TMT [30,32,36,37,38], while the CDT was used for executive function practice–constructive skills and visuospatial abilities [30]. Subjective cognitive impairment was measured by the CFQ, which is a global recognized method, while for premorbid cognitive ability the NART score was used [39]. Finally, attention was measured using the BTA [30] and the DSST [36].
Grodstein et al. carried out the most important and prolonged intervention study with β-carotene alone, with a mean treatment period of 18 years [40]. Five studies assessed daily dietary intake including β-carotene using food frequency questionnaires [29,30,31,34,38], and five studies used multivitamin supplementation (controlled dosage of β-carotene) [33,41], of which three used a placebo [37,39,42].
The MySLR platform generates synthesis results. We adjusted the number of topics to be extracted (k value) to two, which ensured a satisfactory value of topic coherence (−1.25) with easy interpretation of the results [25]. The MySLR platform allowed us to use the LDA algorithm, which identifies the most relevant words for each topic, creating a word cloud. The words “vitamin” and “supplementation” received the higher scores in Topic 1; while the word “antioxidant” received the second highest score in Topic 2 after “vitamin”.

3.2. Risk of Bias Assessment

The risk of bias assessment is summarized in Figure 4A,B. As mentioned above, we used two tools: the Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-sectional Studies (n = 9) and Quality Assessment of Controlled Intervention Studies (n = 7). Using these tools, out of the 16 studies included in the systematic review, 8 studies were rated as “good” [30,31,34,37,38,40,41,42] and 8 as “fair” [28,29,32,33,35,36,39,43].

3.3. Topic Identification

The LDA algorithm allowed us to identify two topics regarding β-carotene and cognitive function in older adults, which are presented and discussed in this section. We developed the discussion starting from Topic 2 since it deals with the impact of dietary β-carotene intake, and we then treated Topic 1, which is more specific as it examines the influence of supplementation of β-carotene, more generally as part of multivitamin formulations.

3.3.1. Topic 2: Antioxidants: β-carotene Dietary Intake and Its Relationship with Cognitive Function

By examining the top 30 most significant terms and their frequency within the ten papers categorized under this topic [28,29,30,31,32,34,35,36,38,43] and further analyzing them, it became apparent that the fundamental aspect of Topic 2 was the relationship between cognitive function and the dietary intake or serum levels of β-carotene. The concentration of carotenoids in serum is considered to reflect short-term dietary intake and it is widely accepted as a good biomarker of fruit and vegetable intake [45,46].
Studies clustered in Topic 2 and their main results are summarized in Table 3. These studies aim to understand whether higher consumption of β-carotene-rich foods such as vegetables and fruits or increased serum levels of β-carotene are associated with improved cognitive function. It is currently hypothesized that consuming fruits and vegetables can protect against age-related cognitive impairments. It is probable that by increasing the intake of dietary antioxidants such as β-carotene, the damaging effects of free radicals on neurons can be slowed down, potentially protecting against cognitive decline and conditions such as dementia. Modifying antioxidant intake through supplementation or dietary changes is relatively easy to implement, making it an attractive strategy.

3.3.2. Topic 1. The Impact of β-carotene Supplementation with a Multivitamin on Cognitive Performance

The other topic we have identified is the effect of β-carotene supplementation on cognitive performance. Supplementation with β-carotene as a single molecule is rarely studied because it is usually studied in synergy with other antioxidant molecules or vitamins.
Studies clustered in Topic 1 and their main results are summarized in Table 4. These studies monitored the effect of β-carotene supplementation on various aspects of cognitive function, including memory, attention, and executive function. Only one study focused on evaluation of the effects of supplementation of β-carotene as a single molecule [40]. The other studies used controlled dosages of β-carotene in combination with multivitamins or minerals. Interestingly, one study evaluated the blood levels of β-carotene during supplementation [37].

4. Discussion

The purpose of the present review was to summarize the available clinical evidence regarding the potential use of β-carotene intake as a nutritional strategy for cognitive maintenance. To date, few clinical trials have investigated the relationship between β-carotene and cognition, and epidemiological studies examining the association between dietary and supplementary β-carotene intake and cognitive function have yielded varying and inconclusive results. Here, we extrapolate that dietary intake and β-carotene serum levels have been associated with measures of cognitive function in some studies [28,29,31,32,35,38], but not in all of them [34,36,43]. In particular, Zhong et al., using NHANES data, reported that β-carotene dietary intake was inversely associated with cognitive function decline. There was an approximately linear dose–response relationship between β-carotene dietary intake and CERAD WL, AFT, and DSST test results; moreover, they reported differences in cognitive function between sexes [29]. It is important to underlay here that these batteries of cognitive function tests have been widely used in large epidemiological and clinical studies [47,48]. Their validity in clinical assessment was determined in neuropathological patients and, for CERAD WL, also confirmed post-mortem by brain autopsy in a group of 176 primary dementing illness patients [48]. Similarly, the study of Bacchetti et al. observed sex differences in plasma β-carotene levels, suggesting that the protective effect of β-carotene on cognitive decline is higher in women than in men [49]. Two other studies have shown that the association between β-carotene and ISLT has good sensitivity to verbal memory alteration [28,32]. It has been demonstrated that the ISLT is sensitive to verbal memory deficits, has the potential to be adapted to cultural groups, and allows for accurate comparison [50]. In particular, Perrig and co-workers found a positive correlation between elevated levels of β-carotene in blood and improved semantic memory performance according to the ISLT and concluded that β-carotene serum levels remain a significant predictor of semantic memory performance [28]. Johnson et al. examined the influence of plasma β-carotene, which correlated positively with most measures of cognitive function [35]. In accordance, a lower consumption of β-carotene was linked in the Rotterdam study to decreased cognitive performance as assessed by the MMSE [51]. In addition, Yuan et al. described that long-term greater dietary intake of total β-carotene was associated with lower odds of moderate and poor subjective cognitive function [31].
Besides this epidemiological evidence, the findings of several randomized clinical trials support the protective effect of β-carotene against cognitive impairment. In the first, the Physicians’ Health Study (PHS), men treated with 50 mg of β-carotene on alternate days over the course of 18 years showed higher overall scores in terms of verbal and cognitive memory than those treated with placebo. However, there was no effect in men who underwent a single year of supplementation [40]. These findings show that the purported cognitive benefits of β-carotene supplementation may be related to earlier age or a longer duration of exposure. In the second study, indicated as the SU.VI.MAX study, higher episodic memory and semantic fluency test scores were reported alongside supplementation with various antioxidants, namely VC, VE, and β-carotene, particularly among non-smokers and subjects with low serum concentrations of antioxidants at baseline. However, the independent contribution of β-carotene to the general observed effects in this study cannot be determined [37]. In agreement with these previous findings, the supplementation strategy of VE and VC combined with β-carotene significantly improved cognitive function in the elderly subjects, particularly with higher doses of β-carotene [33]. In a randomized trial in women with cardiovascular disease, β-carotene treatment showed no effect on cognitive decline after 10 years, although β-carotene-containing supplements were effective in slowing cognitive decline in a subgroup of women who consumed little β-carotene in their diet [42].
On the contrary, higher intakes of FVs, vegetables alone, and β-carotene-rich FVs were associated with poorer executive function and poor performance on the FDST [38]; this inverse association could be linked to the presence of pesticides in vegetables, as exposure to pesticides elevates the risk of cognitive impairment [52]. We also reported the study of Beydoun et al. in which dietary β-carotene intake was associated with faster cognitive decline and poorer performance on the CDT [30]. Finally, in two studies, the supplementation of multivitamin complexes containing β-carotene at 9 mg (for six months) and 12 mg (for one year) per capsule, respectively, had no effect on cognitive performance [39,41].
The benefits of β-carotene on cognitive function, as found in a significant part of the studies reviewed herein, could be explained by several non-mutually exclusive biochemical mechanisms, starting with the antioxidant activity of this compound. β-carotene is considered a powerful chelator of singlet oxygen and reacts with several species of free radicals. The conjugated double chain is responsible for eliminating singlet oxygen, and the greater the number of conjugated bonds, the greater the ability to eliminate singlet oxygen [53]. It has also been noted that β-carotene and other antioxidant substances, such as vitamin E, can function synergistically; in fact, studies suggest that low serum levels and low intake of tocopherols and tocotrienols are associated with the risk of cognitive impairment in older adults, which reinforces the hypothesis that vitamin E in its different forms plays a role in the maintenance of cognitive function in aging [54]. At the same time, β-carotene and vitamin E work together to prevent lipid peroxidation [55]. However, in some conditions, including high doses of antioxidant intake or the oxidative status of smokers, β-carotene may increase lipid peroxidation and DNA oxidative damage. β-carotene is sensitive to degradation and oxidation, and under certain conditions it can act as a pro-oxidant [56], favoring the production of reactive oxygen species (ROS) such as epoxides and carbonyls [57].
Additionally, β-carotene intake could help to mitigate brain fog or cognitive impairment through its effect on calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase IV (CAMKIV). This enzyme belongs to the Ser/Thr kinase family. In the brain, it is found in the cerebellar cortical granules. CAMKIV plays a role in angiogenesis, the inhibition of apoptosis, and cell signaling in a calcium-dependent manner. At elevated intracellular calcium ion concentration, CAMKIV forms Ca2+/calmodulin complexes and induces the phosphorylation of transcription factors. CAMKIV is considered an important factor in neurodegenerative disorders, as well as in several types of cancer [58]. It has been shown that β-carotene, by itself, can bind to the active site of CAMKIV with high affinity, forming a stable complex which, in turn, results in decreased CAMKIV activity [59]. This ability could make β-carotene an attractive supplement in neurodegenerative disorders, and also in cancer treatment, since it has shown no cytotoxic effects in vitro [59,60]. Interestingly, Kim et al. showed that β-carotene is associated with increased levels of Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) in animal models [61]. BDNF functions in the brain to regulate synapses, exhibiting structural and functional effects on excitatory or inhibitory synapses [62], and is a key molecule in plastic changes related to learning and memory [63].
Finally, there is also evidence to support the role of β-carotene in favorably influencing cognition through its role as a vitamin A precursor. The conversion of vitamin A to retinoic acid, which activates retinoic acid receptors, occurs more quickly in the brain than in other target organs. Synaptic plasticity in areas of the brain involved in learning and memory, such as the hippocampus, is controlled by the controlled synthesis of retinoic acid [64]. Studies in rodents have shown that retinoic acid induces hippocampal neurogenesis and neuronal differentiation [65], whereas vitamin A deficiency decreases hippocampal plasticity and increases amyloid β deposition in rodent models [66].
The results of the current systematic review have been mixed, with a majority of studies reporting positive effects on cognitive performance while others show no significant improvement or even a negative impact. The disparity of results could be due to differences in the features of study populations. It is necessary to enhance our knowledge of the impact of age and gender on β-carotene absorption and its conversion to vitamin A, as well as to comprehend the effects of short-term versus long-term supplementation and the influence of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), such as in cognitive function-related genes and carotenoid retinoid metabolism-related genes. β-carotene may provide some form of defense against cognitive aging in people who are more genetically predisposed to it, as shown for the APOε 4 allele [67]. It is also worth mentioning that many neurodegenerative disorders, including Alzheimer’s disease, often display coexistence with metabolic dysfunctions and that ablation of the β,β-Carotene-9’,10’-oxygenase 2 (BCO2) enzyme that catalyzes the asymmetric cleavage of carotenoids remodels the hypothalamic metabolome, leading to metabolic disorders in mice [66].
β-carotene could also provide such defense selectively in people with low basal β-carotene levels owing to nutritional or genetic traits. Thus, we could assume that the small benefit observed in some studies could represent a larger beneficial effect in a small fraction of the studied population depleted in β-carotene and having impaired cognitive function. This β-carotene depletion could be due to genetic or nutritional reasons. Considering both genetic traits and β-carotene serum levels with a personalized nutritional approach tailored to individuals showing deficiency of β-carotene, the effect of this carotenoid on cognition may be much greater.
Further randomized clinical trials (double-blind and placebo-controlled, considered the “gold standard” of clinical studies) assessing β-carotene and its influence on cognitive function are deserved. Of particular interest would be studies in adults aged 60–70 years, since cognitive decline is a common phenomenon that often accompanies aging. Studying this age group would allow researchers to target a population where cognitive concerns are more prevalent. Such studies should contemplate measurements of serum β-carotene at baseline and a battery of cognitive tests validated at various intervals.

5. Conclusions

This is the first systematic review that evaluates the relationship between β-carotene and cognitive function. Most epidemiological evidence supports β-carotene having a positive effect through dietary consumption and vegetable and fruit intake. Some studies have suggested that combining β-carotene with other nutrients with antioxidant properties, such as vitamin E, vitamin C, zinc, or selenium, may have a greater impact on cognitive function when serum levels of it are not depleted.

6. Strength and Limitation

This systematic review had a particular strength in its study selection process, assisted by an automated cloud platform. Additionally, the literature search was conducted using a predefined search strategy across multiple databases. Chances for pooling data for meta-analysis were explored but limited because of heterogeneity among the included studies in terms of participant characteristics, dosage and the duration of supplementation, adjustment covariates, and the cognitive assessment tools employed. As for other limitations, the reliability and authenticity of the results may be influenced by the timing of the questionnaires used to evaluate β-carotene intake, which may not accurately reflect long-term dietary habits. The results may also be affected by the use of self-reported data, which could involve inherent social bias. Finally, the measurement of β-carotene in serum only captures a portion of its content, disregarding its presence in body fat and thereby potentially compromising the accuracy of the results.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.M.A.-G. and E.C.; methodology, D.M.A.-G.; writing—review and editing, all authors. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by PNRR Project: H23C22000370006-T4Y S5G4PP1 to Erika Cione. M. Luisa Bonet and Joan Ribot are members of the Spanish Carotenoid Network (CaRed), grant RED2022-134577-T, funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

AFTAnimal Fluency Test
BATBerliner Amnesie Test
BCβ-carotene
BDNFBrain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor
BDSBehavioral Dyscontrol Scale
BTABrief Test of Attention
BVRTBenton Visual Retention Test
CERAD WLConsortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word Learning
CFQ scoreCognitive Failures Questionnaire
CDTClock Drawing Test
CFTCategory Fluency Test
CVLTCalifornia Verbal Learning Test
COWATControlled Oral Word Association Test
DCSDoetinchem Cohort Study
DFRDelayed Free Recall
DSSTDigit Symbol Substitution Test
EBMTEast Boston Memory Test
FDSTForward digit span test
FFQFood Frequency Questionnaire
FOMEFuld Object Memory Evaluation
FVsFruit and vegetables
GDRSGlobal Deterioration Rating Scale
HDSHasegawa Dementia Scale
ISLTInternational Shopping List Task
RI-48Cued Recall Test
KAIKurztest fuer Allgemeine Intelligenz
LDALatent Dirichlet Allocation
MMSEMini-mental state examination
MoCAMontreal Cognitive Assessment
NARTNational Adult Reading Test
NIHNational Institutes of Health
ONBOne Back Task
PHSPhysicians’ Health Study
PHSIIPhysicians’ Health Study II
SIBSevere Impairment Battery
NHANESNational Health and Nutrition Examination Survey
NHSNurses’ Health Study
SCFSubjective Cognitive Function
TICSTelephone Interview of Cognitive Status
TMTTrail Making Test
TMTATrail Making Test A
TMTBTrail Making Test B
VAVitamin A
VEVitamin E
VCVitamin C
WAIS-IIIWechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised
WFTWord Fluency Test
WACSWomen’s Antioxidant Cardiovascular Study

References

  1. Robinson, M. Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well-being Research. Ref. Rev. 2015, 29, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Birle, C.; Slavoaca, D.; Balea, M.; Livint Popa, L.; Muresanu, I.; Stefanescu, E.; Vacaras, V.; Dina, C.; Strilciuc, S.; Popescu, B.O.; et al. Cognitive function: Holarchy or holacracy? Neurol. Sci. 2021, 42, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Cione, E.; Abrego Guandique, D.M.; Caroleo, M.C.; Luciani, F.; Colosimo, M.; Cannataro, R. Liver Damage and microRNAs: An Update. Curr. Issues Mol. Biol. 2022, 45, 78–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Iannone, F.; Montesanto, A.; Cione, E.; Crocco, P.; Caroleo, M.C.; Dato, S.; Rose, G.; Passarino, G. Expression patterns of muscle-specific miR-133b and miR-206 correlate with nutritional status and sarcopenia. Nutrients 2020, 12, 297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Van Dijk, D.M.; Van Rhenen, W.; Murre, J.M.J.; Verwijk, E. Cognitive functioning, sleep quality, and work performance in non-clinical burnout: The role of working memory. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0231906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Williams, A.M.; Ford, P.R.; Eccles, D.W.; Ward, P. Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport and its acquisition: Implications for applied cognitive psychology. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2011, 25, 432–442. Available online: http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&PAGE=reference&D=emed10&NEWS=N&AN=2011280445 (accessed on 9 July 2023). [CrossRef]
  7. Hernández-Mendo, A.; Reigal, R.E.; López-Walle, J.M.; Serpa, S.; Samdal, O.; Morales-Sánchez, V.; Juárez-Ruiz de Mier, R.; Tristán-Rodríguez, J.L.; Rosado, A.F.; Falco, C. Physical Activity, Sports Practice, and Cognitive Functioning: The Current Research Status. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Murman, D.L. The Impact of Age on Cognition. Semin. Hear. 2015, 36, 111–121. Available online: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4906299/ (accessed on 9 July 2023). [CrossRef]
  9. Pais, R.; Ruano, L.; Carvalho, O.P.; Barros, H. Global cognitive impairment prevalence and incidence in community dwelling older adults—A systematic review. Geriatrics 2020, 5, 84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Walczak, P.; Janowski, M. Chemobrain as a Product of Growing Success in Chemotherapy—Focus On Glia As Both A Victim And A Cure. Neuropsychiatry 2019, 9, 2207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Fehlauer, F.; Tribius, S.; Mehnert, A.; Rades, D. Health-related quality of life in long term breast cancer survivors treated with breast conserving therapy: Impact of age at therapy. Breast Cancer Res. Treat. 2005, 92, 217–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Sukel, K. Lifting the fog. New Sci. 2022, 254, 38–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Guo, P.; Benito Ballesteros, A.; Yeung, S.P.; Liu, R.; Saha, A.; Curtis, L.; Cheke, L.G. COVCOG 2: Cognitive and Memory Deficits in Long COVID: A Second Publication From the COVID and Cognition Study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2022, 14, 804937. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/35370620/ (accessed on 9 July 2023). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Guo, P.; Benito Ballesteros, A.; Yeung, S.P.; Liu, R.; Saha, A.; Curtis, L.; Cheke, L.G. COVCOG 1: Factors Predicting Physical, Neurological and Cognitive Symptoms in Long COVID in a Community Sample. A First Publication From the COVID and Cognition Study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2022, 14, 804922. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/35370617/ (accessed on 9 July 2023). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Kandlur, A.; Satyamoorthy, K.; Gangadharan, G. Oxidative Stress in Cognitive and Epigenetic Aging: A Retrospective Glance. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2020, 13, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Berr, C.; Balansard, B.; Arnaud, J.; Roussel, A.M.; Alpérovitch, A. Cognitive decline is associated with systemic oxidative stress: The EVA study. Etude du Vieillissement Artériel. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2000, 48, 1285–1291. Available online: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11037017 (accessed on 9 July 2023). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Zhang, J.; Lei, W.; Chen, X.; Wang, S.; Qian, W. Oxidative stress response induced by chemotherapy in leukemia treatment (Review). Mol. Clin. Oncol. 2018, 8, 391–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Bohn, T.; Bonet, M.L.; Borel, P.; Keijer, J.; Landrier, J.F.; Milisav, I.; Ribot, J.; Riso, P.; Winklhofer-Roob, B.; Sharoni, Y.; et al. Mechanistic aspects of carotenoid health benefits—Where are we now? Nutr. Res. Rev. 2021, 34, 276–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Rodriguez-Concepcion, M.; Avalos, J.; Bonet, M.L.; Boronat, A.; Gomez-Gomez, L.; Hornero-Mendez, D.; Limon, M.C.; Meléndez-Martínez, A.J.; Olmedilla-Alonso, B.; Palou, A.; et al. A global perspective on carotenoids: Metabolism, biotechnology, and benefits for nutrition and health. Prog. Lipid Res. 2018, 70, 62–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Shete, V.; Quadro, L. Mammalian metabolism of β-carotene: Gaps in knowledge. Nutrients 2013, 5, 4849–4868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Jenab, M.; Salvini, S.; Van Gils, C.H.; Brustad, M.; Shakya-Shrestha, S.; Buijsse, B.; Verhagen, H.; Touvier, M.; Biessy, C.; Wallström, P.; et al. Dietary intakes of retinol, β-carotene, vitamin D and vitamin E in the european prospective investigation into cancer and nutrition cohort. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2009, 63, S150–S178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Saari, J.C. Vitamin A and Vision. In The Biochemistry of Retinoid Signaling II. Subcellular Biochemistry; Asson-Batres, M., Rochette-Egly, C., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherland, 2016; Volume 81, pp. 231–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Huang, Z.; Liu, Y.; Qi, G.; Brand, D.; Zheng, S.G. Role of vitamin A in the immune system. J. Clin. Med. 2018, 7, 258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Malivindi, R.; Rago, V.; De Rose, D.; Gervasi, M.C.; Cione, E.; Russo, G.; Aquila, S. Influence of all-trans retinoic acid on sperm metabolism and oxidative stress: Its involvement in the physiopathology of varicocele-associated male infertility. J. Cell. Physiol. 2018, 233, 9526–9537. Available online: http://www.embase.com/search/results?subaction=viewrecord&from=export&id=L624279221%0A (accessed on 9 July 2023). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Ammirato, S.; Felicetti, A.M.; Rogano, D.; Linzalone, R.; Corvello, V. Digitalising the Systematic Literature Review process: The MySLR platform. Knowl. Manag. Res. Pract. 2022, 21, 777–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2009, 62, 1006–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Melnyk, B.M.; Fineout-Overholt, E. Evidence-Based Practice in Nursing & Healthcare: A Guide to Best Practice, 5th ed.; Wolters Kluwer: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2023; ISBN 9781975185725. [Google Scholar]
  28. Perrig, W.J.; Perrig, P.; Stähelin, H.B. The relation between antioxidants and memory performance in the old and very old. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 1997, 45, 718–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Zhong, Q.; Sun, W.; Qin, Y.; Xu, H. Association of Dietary α-Carotene and β-Carotene Intake with Low Cognitive Performance in Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Nutrients 2023, 15, 239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Beydoun, M.A.; Canas, J.A.; Fanelli-Kuczmarski, M.T.; Maldonado, A.I.; Shaked, D.; Kivimaki, M.; Evans, M.K.; Zonderman, A.B. Association of antioxidant vitamins a, c, e and carotenoids with cognitive performance over time: A cohort study of middle-aged adults. Nutrients 2020, 12, 3558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Yuan, C.; Fondell, E.; Ascherio, A.; Okereke, O.I.; Grodstein, F.; Hofman, A.; Willett, W.C. Long-Term Intake of Dietary Carotenoids Is Positively Associated with Late-Life Subjective Cognitive Function in a Prospective Study in US Women. J. Nutr. 2020, 150, 1871–1879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Gerger, P.; Pai, R.K.; Stuckenschneider, T.; Falkenreck, J.; Weigert, H.; Stahl, W.; Weber, B.; Nelles, G.; Spazzafumo, L.; Schneider, S.; et al. Associations of lipophilic micronutrients with physical and cognitive fitness in persons with mild cognitive impairment. Nutrients 2019, 11, 902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Li, Y.; Liu, S.; Man, Y.; Li, N.; Zhou, Y. Effects of vitamins E and C combined with β carotene on cognitive function in the elderly. Exp. Ther. Med. 2015, 9, 1489–1493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Nooyens, A.C.J.; Milder, I.E.J.; Van Gelder, B.M.; Bueno-De-Mesquita, H.B.; Van Boxtel, M.P.J.; Verschuren, W.M.M. Diet and cognitive decline at middle age: The role of antioxidants. Br. J. Nutr. 2015, 113, 1410–1417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Johnson, E.J.; Vishwanathan, R.; Johnson, M.A.; Hausman, D.B.; Davey, A.; Scott, T.M.; Poon, L.W. Relationship between serum and brain carotenoids, α -tocopherol, and retinol concentrations and cognitive performance in the oldest old from the georgia centenarian study. J. Aging Res. 2013, 2013, 951786. Available online: http://www.embase.com/search/results?subaction=viewrecord&from=export&id=L369322094%0A (accessed on 9 July 2023). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Akbaraly, N.T.; Faure, H.; Gourlet, V.; Favier, A.; Berr, C. Plasma carotenoid levels and cognitive performance in an elderly population: Results of the EVA study. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 24, 308–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kesse-Guyot, E.; Fezeu, L.; Jeandel, C.; Ferry, M.; Andreeva, V.; Amieva, H.; Galan, P. French adults’ cognitive performance after daily supplementation with antioxidant vitamins and minerals at nutritional doses: A post hoc analysis of the supplementation in vitamins and mineral antioxidants (SU.VI.MAX) trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 94, 892–899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Péneau, S.; Galan, P.; Jeandel, C.; Ferry, M.; Andreeva, V.; Hercberg, S.; Kesse-Guyot, E. Fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function in the SU.VI.MAX 2 prospective study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 94, 1295–1303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Smith, A.P.; Clark, R.E.; Nutt, D.J.; Haller, J.; Hayward, S.G.; Perry, K. Vitamin C, Mood and Cognitive Functioning in the Elderly. Nutr. Neurosci. 1999, 2, 249–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Grodstein, F.; Kang, J.H.; Glynn, R.J.; Cook, N.R.; Gaziano, J.M. A randomized trial of beta carotene supplementation and cognitive function in men: The physicians’ health study II. Arch. Intern. Med. 2007, 167, 2184–2190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Wolters, M.; Hickstein, M.; Flintermann, A.; Tewes, U.; Hahn, A. Cognitive performance in relation to vitamin status in healthy elderly German women—The effect of 6-month multivitamin supplementation. Prev. Med. 2005, 41, 253–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kang, J.H.; Cook, N.R.; Manson, J.E.; Buring, J.E.; Albert, C.M.; Grodstein, F. Vitamin E, Vitamin C, Beta carotene, and cognitive function among women with or at risk of cardiovascular disease: The women’s antioxidant and cardiovascular study. Circulation 2009, 119, 2772–2780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Perkins, A.J.; Hendrie, H.C.; Callahan, C.M.; Gao, S.; Unverzagt, F.W.; Xu, Y.; Hall, K.S.; Hui, S.L. Association of antioxidants with memory in a multiethnic elderly sample using the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination survey. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1999, 150, 37–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. NIH. Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies—National Institutes of Health. Available online: https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-pro/guidelines/in-develop/cardiovascular-risk-reduction/tools/cohort (accessed on 9 July 2023).
  45. Bohn, T.; Mcdougall, G.J.; Alegría, A.; Alminger, M.; Arrigoni, E.; Aura, A.-M.; Brito, C.; Cilla, A.; El, S.N.; Karakaya, S.; et al. Mind the gap-deficits in our knowledge of aspects impacting the bioavailability of phytochemicals and their metabolites-a position paper focusing on carotenoids and polyphenols. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2015, 59, 1307–1323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Bohn, T.; Desmarchelier, C.; Dragsted, L.O.; Nielsen, C.S.; Stahl, W.; Rühl, R.; Keijer, J.; Borel, P. Host-related factors explaining interindividual variability of carotenoid bioavailability and tissue concentrations in humans. Mol. Nutr. Food Res. 2017, 61, 1600685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Plassman, B.L.; Langa, K.M.; Fischer, G.G.; Heeringa, S.G.; Weir, D.R.; Ofstedal, M.B.; Burke, J.R.; Hurd, M.D.; Potter, G.G.; Rodgers, W.L.; et al. Prevalence of Dementia in the United States: The Aging, demographics, and memory study. Neuroepidemiology 2007, 29, 125–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Fillenbaum, G.G.; van Belle, G.; Morris, J.C.; Mohs, R.C.; Mirra, S.S.; Davis, P.C.; Tariot, P.N.; Silverman, J.M.; Clark, C.M.; Welsh-Bohmer, K.A.; et al. Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD): The first twenty years. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2008, 4, 96–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Bacchetti, T.; Turco, I.; Urbano, A.; Morresi, C.; Ferretti, G. Relationship of fruit and vegetable intake to dietary antioxidant capacity and markers of oxidative stress: A sex-related study. Nutrition 2019, 61, 164–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Maruff, P.; Thompson, T.A.C.; Wilson, P.H.; Snyder, P.J.; Pietrzak, R.H.; Darby, D.; Buschke, H. Sensitivity and Test-Retest Reliability of the International Shopping List Test in Assessing Verbal Learning and Memory in Mild Alzheimer’s Disease. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2011, 26, 412–424. [Google Scholar]
  51. Jama, J.W.; Launer, L.J.; Witteman, J.C.M.; Den Breeijen, J.H.; Breteler, M.M.B.; Grobbee, D.E.; Hofman, A. Dietary antioxidants and cognitive function in a population-based sample of older persons: The Rotterdam Study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1996, 144, 275–280. Available online: https://login.pallas2.tcl.sc.edu/login?url=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=ccm&AN=107147513&site=ehost-live (accessed on 9 July 2023). [CrossRef]
  52. Chen, L.; Yoo, S.E.; Na, R.; Liu, Y.; Ran, Q. Cognitive impairment and increased Aβ levels induced by paraquat exposure are attenuated by enhanced removal of mitochondrial H2O2. Neurobiol. Aging 2012, 33, e15–e432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Marcelino, G.; Machate, D.J.; Freitas, K.d.C.; Hiane, P.A.; Maldonade, I.R.; Pott, A.; Asato, M.A.; Candido, C.J.; Guimarães, R.D.C.A. β-Carotene: Preventive Role for Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus and Obesity: A Review. Molecules 2020, 25, 5803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Rondanelli, M.; Faliva, M.; Peroni, G.; Moncaglieri, F.; Infantino, V.; Naso, M.; Perna, S. Focus on Pivotal Role of Dietary Intake (Diet and Supplement) and Blood Levels of Tocopherols and Tocotrienols in Obtaining Successful Aging. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2015, 16, 23227–23249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Fiedor, J.; Burda, K. Potential Role of Carotenoids as Antioxidants in Human Health and Disease. Nutrients 2014, 6, 466–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Chen, J.; Li, F.; Li, Z.; McClements, D.J.; Xiao, H. Encapsulation of carotenoids in emulsion-based delivery systems: Enhancement of β-carotene water-dispersibility and chemical stability. Food Hydrocoll. 2017, 69, 49–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Hix, L.M.; Lockwood, S.F.; Bertram, J.S. Bioactive carotenoids: Potent antioxidants and regulators of gene expression. Redox Rep. 2004, 9, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Naz, H.; Islam, A.; Ahmad, F.; Hassan, M.I. Calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase IV: A multifunctional enzyme and potential therapeutic target. Prog. Biophys. Mol. Biol. 2016, 121, 54–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Naz, H.; Khan, P.; Tarique, M.; Rahman, S.; Meena, A.; Ahamad, S.; Luqman, S.; Islam, A.; Ahmad, F.; Hassan, M.I. Binding studies and biological evaluation of β-carotene as a potential inhibitor of human calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase IV. Int. J. Biol. Macromol. 2017, 96, 161–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Silalahi, J. Anticancer and health protective properties of citrus fruit components. Asia Pac. J. Clin. Nutr. 2002, 11, 79–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Kim, N.R.; Kim, H.Y.; Kim, M.H.; Kim, H.M.; Jeong, H.J. Improvement of depressive behavior by Sweetme Sweet PumpkinTM and its active compound, β-carotene. Life Sci. 2016, 147, 39–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Lu, B.; Nagappan, G.; Lu, Y. BDNF and Synaptic Plasticity, Cognitive Function, and Dysfunction. Neurotrophic Factors 2014, 220, 223–250. [Google Scholar]
  63. Miranda, M.; Morici, J.F.; Zanoni, M.B.; Bekinschtein, P. Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor: A Key Molecule for Memory in the Healthy and the Pathological Brain. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Wołoszynowska-Fraser, M.U.; Kouchmeshky, A.; McCaffery, P. Vitamin A and Retinoic Acid in Cognition and Cognitive Disease. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 2020, 40, 247–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Mishra, S.; Kelly, K.K.; Rumian, N.L.; Siegenthaler, J.A. Retinoic Acid Is Required for Neural Stem and Progenitor Cell Proliferation in the Adult Hippocampus. Stem Cell Rep. 2018, 10, 1705–1720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Husson, M.; Enderlin, V.; Delacourte, A.; Ghenimi, N.; Alfos, S.; Pallet, V.; Higueret, P. Retinoic acid normalizes nuclear receptor mediated hypo-expression of proteins involved in β-amyloid deposits in the cerebral cortex of vitamin A deprived rats. Neurobiol. Dis. 2006, 23, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Hu, P.; Bretsky, P.; Crimmins, E.M.; Guralnik, J.M.; Reuben, D.B.; Seeman, T.E. Association between serum beta-carotene levels and decline of cognitive function in high-functioning older persons with or without apolipoprotein E 4 alleles: MacArthur studies of successful aging. J. Gerontol.—Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2006, 61, 616–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram showing the algorithm for selecting eligible studies.
Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram showing the algorithm for selecting eligible studies.
Brainsci 13 01468 g001
Figure 2. Distribution by year of the studies on the relationship between β-carotene and cognitive function included in the systematic review (n = 16) (created with MySLR).
Figure 2. Distribution by year of the studies on the relationship between β-carotene and cognitive function included in the systematic review (n = 16) (created with MySLR).
Brainsci 13 01468 g002
Figure 3. Scheme of the cognitive test batteries and assessment tools used in the systematically revised literature. Global cognition was assessed with the following: MMSE: mini-mental state examination; TICS: Telephone Interview of Cognitive Status; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SIB: Severe Impairment Battery. Lexical/semantic memory was assessed with the following: AFT: Animal Fluency Test; EBMT: East Boston Memory Test; CFT: Category Fluency Test; COWAT: Controlled Oral Word Association Test; WFT: Word Fluency Test. Working memory/speed processing was assessed with the following: the digit span test; CERAD WL: Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word Learning; ONB: One Back Task; DSST: Digit Symbol Substitution Test. Episodic memory was assessed with the following: RI-48: Cued Recall Test; FOME: Fuld Object Memory Evaluation. Verbal memory was assessed with the following: CVLT: California Verbal Learning Test; ISLT: International Shopping List Task. Intellectual quotient/intelligence was assessed with the following: KAI: Kurztest fuer Allgemeine Intelligenz; BAT: Berliner Amnesie Test; WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised. Motor coordination was assessed with the following: TMT, Trail Making Test; CDT: Clock Drawing Test. Visual memory/visual perception was assessed with the BVRT: Benton Visual Retention Test. Executive function was assessed with the BDS: Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale. Attention was assessed with the following: BTA: Brief Test of Attention; DSST: Digit Symbol Substitution Test. Premorbid cognitive ability was assessed with the NART: National Adult Reading Test. Subjective cognitive impairment was assessed with the CFQ: Cognitive Failures Questionnaire. The visualization was created with BioRender.
Figure 3. Scheme of the cognitive test batteries and assessment tools used in the systematically revised literature. Global cognition was assessed with the following: MMSE: mini-mental state examination; TICS: Telephone Interview of Cognitive Status; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SIB: Severe Impairment Battery. Lexical/semantic memory was assessed with the following: AFT: Animal Fluency Test; EBMT: East Boston Memory Test; CFT: Category Fluency Test; COWAT: Controlled Oral Word Association Test; WFT: Word Fluency Test. Working memory/speed processing was assessed with the following: the digit span test; CERAD WL: Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease Word Learning; ONB: One Back Task; DSST: Digit Symbol Substitution Test. Episodic memory was assessed with the following: RI-48: Cued Recall Test; FOME: Fuld Object Memory Evaluation. Verbal memory was assessed with the following: CVLT: California Verbal Learning Test; ISLT: International Shopping List Task. Intellectual quotient/intelligence was assessed with the following: KAI: Kurztest fuer Allgemeine Intelligenz; BAT: Berliner Amnesie Test; WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised. Motor coordination was assessed with the following: TMT, Trail Making Test; CDT: Clock Drawing Test. Visual memory/visual perception was assessed with the BVRT: Benton Visual Retention Test. Executive function was assessed with the BDS: Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale. Attention was assessed with the following: BTA: Brief Test of Attention; DSST: Digit Symbol Substitution Test. Premorbid cognitive ability was assessed with the NART: National Adult Reading Test. Subjective cognitive impairment was assessed with the CFQ: Cognitive Failures Questionnaire. The visualization was created with BioRender.
Brainsci 13 01468 g003
Figure 4. (A) Summary of risk-of-bias assessment according to the National Institutes of Health Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies (NIH, 2014) [44]. (B) Summary Quality Assessment according to Health Quality Controlled Intervention Studies (NIH, 2014). The quality rating is 0 for poor (0–4 out of 14 questions), 1 for fair (5–9 out of 14 questions), or 2 for good (>10 out of 14 questions) [28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43]. NA: not applicable, NR: not reported; created with BioRender.
Figure 4. (A) Summary of risk-of-bias assessment according to the National Institutes of Health Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies (NIH, 2014) [44]. (B) Summary Quality Assessment according to Health Quality Controlled Intervention Studies (NIH, 2014). The quality rating is 0 for poor (0–4 out of 14 questions), 1 for fair (5–9 out of 14 questions), or 2 for good (>10 out of 14 questions) [28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43]. NA: not applicable, NR: not reported; created with BioRender.
Brainsci 13 01468 g004aBrainsci 13 01468 g004b
Table 3. List of papers clustered in Topic 2.
Table 3. List of papers clustered in Topic 2.
StudyMethodsResultsReference
Zhong et al. (2023)Dietary intakeQ1 of BC vs. Q4 had lower risks of both CERAD WL decline [odds ratio (OR) = 0.63, 95% confidence interval (CI): 0.44–0.90] and AFT decline (OR = 0.66, 95% CI: 0.47–0.94). Q3 of BC dietary intake had a significantly decreased risk of lower DSST scores (OR = 0.67, 95% CI: 0.48–0.83). For males, dietary BC intake was associated with a decreased risk of AFT decline (OR = 0.51, 95% CI: 0.30–0.85). For females, dietary BC intake was associated with decreased risks of CERAD WL and AFT decline (OR = 0.37, 95% CI: 0.21–0.64; OR = 0.58, 95% CI: 0.37–0.91, respectively)[29]
Yuan et al. (2020)Dietary intakeLong-term intakes of total BC and dietary BC were each associated with lower odds of moderate and poor SCF (OR = 0.77 in Q5; p < 0.001)[31]
Beydoun et al. (2020)Dietary intakeBC intake was associated with a faster decline and poorer performance on the CDT[30]
Gerger et al. (2019)Serum levelsPositive correlation of BC serum concentration with verbal memory assessed through the ISLT (p < 0.01)[32]
Nooyens et al. (2015)Dietary intakeNo association between BC intake and cognitive decline[34]
Johnson et al. (2013)Serum levelsBC serum concentrations positively correlated with most measures of better cognitive function (p < 0.05)[35]
Péneau et al. (2011)Dietary intake of BC-rich FVsThe intake of BC-rich FVs was negatively associated with executive functioning scores (p = 0.02)[38]
Akbaraly et al. (2007)Serum levelsNo associations were found between lower BC serum levels and cognitive impairment[36]
Perkins et al. (1999)Serum levelsDecreasing serum levels of BC were not associated with poor memory performance[43]
Perrig et al. (1997)Serum levelsBC serum concentration (β0.106, p = 0.035) remained a significant predictor of semantic memory performance[28]
Q, quartile. Other abbreviations are presented in the Abbreviations section.
Table 4. List of papers clustered in Topic 1.
Table 4. List of papers clustered in Topic 1.
StudySupplementationResultsReference
Li et al. (2015)Daily VE (200 mg) and VC (300 mg) combined with BC at 16.7 (group A), 8.4 (group B), 5.6 (group C), or 0 mg/day (group D) or VE alone (5 mg) (group E) for 16 weeksMMSE scores in A and B were 23.49 ± 4.40 and 23.44 ± 3.62, respectively, significantly higher compared to E (22.32 ± 4.23; p < 0.05).
HDS scores in A and B were 22.46 ± 4.96 and 21.38 ± 3.97, respectively, significantly higher than the corresponding scores prior to the treatment (18.68 ± 5.77 for A and 19.75 ± 5.46 for B; p < 0.05). HDS scores in A and B (22.46 ± 4.96 and 21.38 ± 3.97; p < 0.05) were significantly higher compared to E (18.87 ± 4.70; p < 0.05).
[33]
Kesse-Guyot et al. (2011)Daily VC (120 mg), BC (6 mg), VE (30 mg), selenium (100 μg), and zinc (20 mg) in combination or placebo for 8 ySubjects receiving active antioxidant supplementation had better episodic memory scores (mean difference: 0.61; 95% CI: 0.02, 1.20).[37]
Kang et al. (2009)BC (50 mg) every other day or placebo, either alone or combined with VE every other day (402 mg), VC daily (500 mg), or both, for 3.5 yThey found that BC supplements were beneficial among those with low dietary intakes of total carotenoids but not among those with higher intakes (p for interaction = 0.02).[42]
Grodstein et al. (2007)BC 50 mg every other day for 18 yImprovement in global cognitive score (p = 0.03), verbal memory (p = 0.007), and TICS score (p = 0.04).[40]
Wolters et al. (2005)Multivitamin capsule (9 mg/d BC) for 6 months No effect on cognitive performance.[41]
Smith et al. (1999)12 mg/d BC, 400 mg/d VE, and 500 mg/d VC in combination or placebo for 1 yThere were very few significant differences between the placebo and multivitamin groups.[39]
Abbreviations are presented in the corresponding list.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Abrego-Guandique, D.M.; Bonet, M.L.; Caroleo, M.C.; Cannataro, R.; Tucci, P.; Ribot, J.; Cione, E. The Effect of Beta-Carotene on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 1468. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13101468

AMA Style

Abrego-Guandique DM, Bonet ML, Caroleo MC, Cannataro R, Tucci P, Ribot J, Cione E. The Effect of Beta-Carotene on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(10):1468. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13101468

Chicago/Turabian Style

Abrego-Guandique, Diana Marisol, Maria Luisa Bonet, Maria Cristina Caroleo, Roberto Cannataro, Paola Tucci, Joan Ribot, and Erika Cione. 2023. "The Effect of Beta-Carotene on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review" Brain Sciences 13, no. 10: 1468. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13101468

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop