Next Article in Journal
Training Emotional Intelligence Online: An Evaluation of WEIT 2.0
Next Article in Special Issue
The ANTI-Vea-UGR Platform: A Free Online Resource to Measure Attentional Networks (Alertness, Orienting, and Executive Control) Functioning and Executive/Arousal Vigilance
Previous Article in Journal
The Development of Flexible Problem Solving: An Integrative Approach
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Evolution and Future Development of Attention Networks
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Opinion

Contributions of Lower Structures to Higher Cognition: Towards a Dynamic Network Model

1
Center for Accessible Neuropsychology, Sagol School of Neuroscience, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel
2
Department of Occupational Therapy, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel
3
Department of Psychology, the Institute of Information Processing and Decision Making, University of Haifa, Haifa 3498838, Israel
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Intell. 2023, 11(6), 121; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060121
Submission received: 12 April 2023 / Revised: 8 June 2023 / Accepted: 11 June 2023 / Published: 14 June 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue On the Origins and Development of Attention Networks)

Abstract

:
Researchers often attribute higher cognition to the enlargement of cortical regions throughout evolution, reflecting the belief that humans sit at the top of the cognitive pyramid. Implicitly, this approach assumes that the subcortex is of secondary importance for higher-order cognition. While it is now recognized that subcortical regions can be involved in various cognitive domains, it remains unclear how they contribute to computations essential for higher-level cognitive processes such as endogenous attention and numerical cognition. Herein, we identify three models of subcortical–cortical relations in these cognitive processes: (i) subcortical regions are not involved in higher cognition; (ii) subcortical computations support elemental forms of higher cognition mainly in species without a developed cortex; and (iii) higher cognition depends on a whole-brain dynamic network, requiring integrated cortical and subcortical computations. Based on evolutionary theories and recent data, we propose the SEED hypothesis: the Subcortex is Essential for the Early Development of higher cognition. According to the five principles of the SEED hypothesis, subcortical computations are essential for the emergence of cognitive abilities that enable organisms to adapt to an ever-changing environment. We examine the implications of the SEED hypothesis from a multidisciplinary perspective to understand how the subcortex contributes to various forms of higher cognition.

1. Introduction

Cognition is a broad term, used to refer to the montage of mental processes that organisms use in variable environments. Although a classic cognitive psychology textbook is likely to cover a broad range of our mental capacities, we generally think of the terms in a hierarchical manner, treating the cognitive capacity required of an ant to find its way back to the nest as “simpler” than that required of the mountaineer planning a three-week trek in the Himalayas. Reflecting our species-centric belief that humans sit at the top of the cognitive pyramid, we point to various metrics of brain anatomy (e.g., size or surface area) as correlates of intelligence. Notably, with a few exceptions (Ullman 2001; Conway 2020; Walenski et al. 2007), most models of complex cognition neglect subcortical circuits while focusing mainly on the cortex.
Implicit by “neglect” is the assumption that the subcortex is of secondary importance when considering higher-level cognition, such as attention and math abilities. While research from the past decade has provided evidence that subcortical regions are engaged in some cognitive tasks (Janacsek et al. 2022; Saban et al. 2018c, 2021a), it is still a question of how (vs. if) the subcortex contributes to the computations essential for higher cognitive processes, with a focus on endogenous attention and numerical cognition.
In this paper, we suggest that the neuroscientific literature still needs to: (i) expand our explorations regarding the functional contributions of subcortical regions to higher cognition; (ii) define, explicitly, the role of subcortical mechanisms in higher cognition; and (iii) portray models that will map how the brain, as a whole, supports higher cognition. Taking an evolutionary perspective and recent empirical data, we suggest an integrative model for the functional role of subcortical regions in cognition, which highlights the evolutionary development of cortical–subcortical relations.
Across domains of study, the development of the cortex has been seen as fundamental for the emergence of the complex cognitive operations required for tasks such as volitional attention and arithmetic (Arsalidou and Taylor 2011; Corbetta et al. 2000; Frith and Dolan 1996). For instance, from this traditional perspective, a hallmark of human cognition is our ability to perform complex arithmetic operations that are mainly dependent on frontoparietal cortical regions (Arsalidou and Taylor 2011; Dehaene et al. 2004). As an historical example, neuropsychological observations from the 19th century led to the classic “Broca–Wernicke–Lichtheim–Geschwind” model of language (Geschwind 1970; Poeppel 2014), which focuses mainly on cortical layers of the brain.
The more advanced cognitive abilities of primates have been explained by the relatively larger expansion of the neocortex (Jerison 1985; Van Horik et al. 2012), and specifically the association cortex, with the apex manifest in the human brain (Bush and Allman 2004; Parks and Smaers 2018). From this phylogenetic perspective, it is appealing to adopt the view that the behavioral and cognitive differences between humans and other species can be attributed to our exceptionally large neocortical tissue.
Brodmann estimated that the prefrontal cortex represents a larger percentage of the cortex in humans (29%) than in the chimpanzee (17%) and the rhesus monkey (11.5%) (Brodmann 1902). From an ontogenetic perspective, the most complex cognitive capabilities of humans are associated with brain regions that show relatively protracted maturation, such as frontal cortical regions. For example, significant changes in white and gray matter in frontoparietal regions continue into late adolescence (Colver and Longwell 2013; Lebel and Beaulieu 2011) and, presumably, support the emergence of higher cognition.
While there is compelling evidence supporting the essential role of the neocortex in sophisticated cognition, to date, there has been little consideration of the role of subcortical structures in higher-order cognition (Janacsek et al. 2022; Parvizi 2009; but see (Ullman 2001; Conway 2020)). Subcortical regions, which are defined as the gray matter structures below the cortex, include areas such as the cerebellum, thalamus, and basal ganglia (BG). The term “higher-order cognition” refers to complex abilities such as endogenous attention, language, and math.

2. The Cortico-Centric Bias

In a review of this topic, Parvizi (Parvizi 2009) suggested that neuroscience and neurology are “cortico-centric”—a conceptual bias that minimizes the possibility that “higher” functions might also depend on “lower” structures. Parvizi wrote, “We do not have sufficient knowledge about the mode of subcortical involvement in cognition and behavioral regulation. In fact, we know very little about the role of subcortical structures in these ‘higher’ functions, precisely because a significant proportion of current research does not see beyond the cerebral neocortex”. Parvizi also notes that some key neuroscientific methods are biased to highlight cortical mechanisms. The use of electroencephalography, magnetoencephalography, near-infrared spectroscopy, optical imaging, and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) has typically been limited to the cortex. Whereas scalp-recorded electroencephalography (EEG) is considered to be cortico-centric, other electrophysiological methods such as intracranial EEG and single unit recording can provide valuable information about both cortical and subcortical activity. Furthermore, studies have shown that TMS of the cerebellum is feasible (Fernandez et al. 2020). In addition to Parvizi’s claims regarding methodological bias, one of the most commonly used methods for exploring the neural substrates of cognition also suffers from a “cortico-centric” tendency. A fundamental methodological limitation of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is its poor ability to detect activations in subcortical structures (LaBar et al. 2001), mainly because the structures are small and susceptible to artifacts.
Moreover, there is a self-fulfilling prophecy at play in this literature: Given assumptions that higher-order cognition is associated with cortical mechanisms, scanning protocols are frequently chosen in a way that results in minimal or suboptimal coverage of the subcortex, especially the cerebellum. Thus, the results from this work are biased and prone to overemphasizing the neocortex’s role in cognitive processes at the expense of the subcortex.
More than a decade after Parvizi’s provocative review, our knowledge of how subcortical regions contribute to high-order cognition remains quite murky (see also Janacsek et al. 2022). The “cortico-centric myopia” (Parvizi 2009) still appears in the current literature. Indeed, over the last two decades, there are consistently more PubMed results on cognition and cortical regions than cognition and subcortical regions (see Figure 1).

3. Undermining the Cortex’s Exclusive Role in Cognition

We propose to develop a granular understanding of how subcortical structures contribute to higher-level cognitive processes with a focus on endogenous attention and numerical cognition. Empirical investigations focusing on the role of subcortical regions in high-level cognitive processes are essential, not only to clarify the functional domain of different subcortical structures but also to provide a more integrative view of the cortico-subcortical networks that support cognition. In addition to the reasons described above, there are several rationales to undermine the exclusive role of the cortex in cognition.
One critical method to explore how subcortical regions contribute to cognition is studying non-human species, which do not have fully developed cortical regions. However, the evolutionary milieu shaping cognition varies greatly across species, making it difficult to design experiments that assay cognition in a way that allows for easy comparisons between human and non-human cognition. Most animals cannot perform the typical tasks employed to study human cognition, or at least require idiosyncratic methodologies. For example, tasks designed to study higher-order cognition typically rely on explicit instructions, an option that is not possible in studies with other species. More importantly, while there are undoubtedly species-general cognitive abilities (e.g., attention, memory) that can provide a point of comparison between species, there are many species-specific abilities (e.g., echolocation in bats, haptic sensing in spiders) that preclude between-species comparisons. In addition, an organism’s cognition is formed within their particular species-specific environment. This environment may restrict, to some level, the development of their cognitive repertoire. For instance, why should the house cockroach learn to ride a bicycle, as humans do? It is of no relevance to them in their specific environment. Thus, measuring animals’ cognitive abilities requires consideration of their specific ecology. The variance in cognition across species requires an extensive set of tools and perspectives that go well beyond gross neuroscientific measures such as brain size (Van Horik et al. 2012).
It is reasonable to assume that as species evolved, a wide range of cognitive processes were necessary for survival in an ever-changing environment. Indeed, organisms that do not possess a developed cortex (e.g., fish, honeybees, spiders) are capable of what we consider high-level cognition, such as attention, executive functions, and numerical abilities (Agrillo et al. 2012; Clayton and Emery 2015; Güntürkün and Bugnyar 2016; Saban et al. 2017). Whether through adaptations arising from a common ancestor (homology) or adaptations reflective of convergent evolution, different species have developed critical cognitive abilities to survive even without a developed neocortex.
Even the traditional assumption that qualitative advances in cognition accompany an increase in brain size can be questioned (Roth and Dicke 2005). Studies involving bats, goats, and fish indicate that some mammalian brains have undergone dramatic evolutionary reductions in size (Niven 2005). Rather than an increase in brain size, interconnectivity may be a more prominent feature for explaining cognitive development (Chittka and Niven 2009). While functional connectivity studies have become quite prominent in studies of human cognition, the focus has remained on cortico–cortico connectivity. Expanding this line of work to consider cortical–subcortical networks is likely to be fruitful for a more comprehensive view of complex cognition.
The “traditional” perspective of cognitive neuroscience is based on the premise that subcortical regions have no role in higher cognition such as attention, language, and mathematics (Arsalidou and Taylor 2011; Bush and Allman 2004; Corbetta et al. 2000; Dehaene et al. 2004; Geschwind 1970; Poeppel 2014). Researchers of subcortical structures have often focused on non-cognitive rather than cognitive functions (Koziol and Budding 2009; Noback et al. 2005). This bias for non-cognitive functions, which has historical roots (e.g., “the reptilian brain”; MacLean 1988), is also prevalent in the neuropsychology community; for example, clinicians tend to highlight motor rather than cognitive impairments in BG and cerebellar disorders (Jankovic 2008; Whaley et al. 2011; see also Janacsek et al. 2022).

4. Where Does the Subcortex Fit in a Model of Cognition?

In this section, we consider different perspectives concerning the role of the subcortex in cognition. We identified three main models in the current literature (see Table 1). The first, which we consider a “traditional” perspective, is based on the premise that subcortical regions have no role in higher cognition. The second advocates a role for the subcortex in some elemental forms of higher cognition, but mainly in species that do not possess a developed neocortex. The third model proposes that higher cognition depends on a dynamic, whole-brain network that requires the integrated computations of the cortex and subcortex. Although we assume these models are relevant for a wide range of cognitive domains, we will focus on two domains, attention and numerical cognition, in evaluating the viability of these three models.
With respect to attention, a basic dichotomy has been made between exogenous (lower-order) and endogenous (higher-order) orienting of attention (Klein and Lawrence 2011). In orienting attention tasks, participants are presented with a cue followed by a peripheral target to which they are instructed to respond. When studying exogenous orienting, the peripheral location where a target might appear is stimulated by a cue that is uninformative about the location of the upcoming target. Exogenous attention is assumed to be driven by bottom-up stimulation and is a fast and reflexive process. However, when studying endogenous attention, a property of a central cue (e.g., the cue’s color), which does not stimulate the possible target locations, provides information about the target’s location. This information of the cue needs to be learned and has a symbolic internal value. The information is aligned with the participant’s goal and is internally selected for further processing. Endogenous, or top-down, attention has been considered to be a voluntary process. Whereas exogenous attention has been linked to both subcortical and cortical regions, endogenous attention has been considered to require computations performed by the neocortex (Corbetta et al. 2000; Peelen et al. 2004). With respect to numerical cognition, the literature has long emphasized the role of neocortical regions, both for non-symbolic (e.g., quantity discrimination) and symbolic (e.g., arithmetic) numerical abilities (Arsalidou and Taylor 2011; Arsalidou et al. 2018).
By the first model, or what we will call the Solitary Cortex Model, higher cognition is exclusively associated with cortical processing. The absence of the subcortex in this literature may reflect a belief that the subcortical operations are irrelevant for cognition or benign neglect, a failure to consider the subcortex in the development of neurofunctional models. This model predicts the following: (i) Species that do not have developed cortical regions (e.g., honeybees, fish) will not exhibit higher cognitive abilities since their brain lacks the anatomical substrate that enables these abilities. (ii) The human subcortex is not functionally involved in higher cognitive abilities. Based on this hierarchical perspective, neocortical regions exclusively allow the emergence of higher cognitive abilities, with subcortical regions only channeling information to the cortex for more complex computations. For example, studies of attention have highlighted how subcortical visual areas (e.g., superior colliculus (SC)) play a role in reflexive, exogenous attention, whereas volitional, endogenous attention is controlled solely by frontoparietal cortical regions (Corbetta et al. 2000; Peelen et al. 2004). Similarly, for arithmetic, a frontoparietal network is assumed to be essential for numerical abilities such as discriminating between quantities, estimating ratios, ordering numerals, and arithmetic operations (Grabner et al. 2009; Dehaene and Cohen 1997; Dehaene et al. 2003, 2004; Andres et al. 2011; Ansari and Dhital 2006; Arsalidou and Taylor 2011). This neurofunctional literature has not considered the possible role of subcortical regions in symbolic abilities, such as endogenous attention and arithmetic.
The second model is the Cortical Superiority Model. While variants of this model acknowledge the potential role of the subcortex, this literature emphasizes the dominance of the cortex by suggesting the following: (i) For species that lack developed cortical regions, functional computations supporting cognition can occur in subcortical substrates. (ii) For species with a developed cortex, higher cognitive abilities depend mainly on neocortical activity, supplanting more ancient subcortical computations. According to this model, the evolution of the cortex allowed new regions to take over potential contributions of subcortical regions to cognition. The subcortex may be important to support elementary processes such as processing sensory input, orienting to salient stimuli, and responding in a reflexive manner. Thus, the cortex-less housefly can discriminate between an appetizing piece of fruit and a threatening fly swatter to trigger the appropriate reflexive response (consume or flee). A species with a cortex can build upon this limited behavioral repertoire, suppressing these elementary subcortically controlled responses to produce more adapted behaviors. For example, cortical regions can integrate the current sensory input with the organism’s internal states, desires, and goals (see Finger 1994). An ontological variant of this model might be that human infants are born with a “bundle of reflexes” that, through development, come under cortical inhibitory suppression that enables goal-directed behavior (Rafal and Henik 1994; delay of gratification, for a review see Mischel et al. 1989).
In contrast to the Solitary Cortex Model, the Cortical Superiority Model suggests that subcortical regions can have a functional role in higher cognitive processes, but this will be most evident in species that lack cortical tissue. The Cortical Superiority Model can account for evidence showing the essential role of the subcortex in species without a developed cortex (Güntürkün and Bugnyar 2016) in endogenous attention (Saban et al. 2017) and numerical abilities (Gross et al. 2009; Agrillo et al. 2010). This model also accounts for evidence showing only cortical activity for such operations in species with a cortex (Westerberg and Klingberg 2007; Frith and Dolan 1996; Dehaene et al. 2004; Corbetta et al. 2000).
The third model, the Dynamic Network Model, emphasizes that complex cognitive abilities are a product of an interactive network that spans the cortex and subcortex. By this model, subcortical regions have a functional role in higher cognition, with the following predictions: (i) Species that do not have a developed cortex will be capable of certain higher cognitive processes, those that can be supported by the subcortex. (ii) In species with a developed cortex, subcortical structures will continue to perform their specialized computations, and through their interactions with the cortex, these computations can enable more elaborated, and in some cases, qualitatively distinct cognitive operations. (iii) Computations that are supported by the subcortex will be found in similar form in many species.
The Dynamic Network Model offers an interactive perspective on endogenous attention. By this view, subcortical computations will support aspects of endogenous orienting across species, independent of how developed their cortical layers are. Behavioral studies, using the classic Posner task, have demonstrated that endogenous orienting of attention depends on subcortical visual regions (e.g., SC or thalamus) both in humans (Saban et al. 2018a, 2018b) and archer fish (Saban et al. 2017; You and Mysore 2020). However, in humans, the engagement of endogenous attention can last for longer durations than in fish (Saban et al. 2017), probably because of additional cortically based computations (Peelen et al. 2004) to maintain goals.
In the field of numerical cognition, humans can rapidly enumerate and add collections of objects, as well as compare the numerosity of different sets. Collins et al. (Collins et al. 2017) used a stereoscopic procedure to identify visual computations that can be performed subcortically. With this method, they showed that monocular pathways in the subcortex are sufficient to support quantity discrimination. Interestingly, the contribution of subcortical regions (e.g., SC) was only evident when the judgments were made on non-symbolic stimuli (e.g., clusters of dots), but not when the quantities were specified with Arabic numbers. Consistent with these results, species that lack a developed cortex (e.g., fish, anurans, honeybees, parrots, spiders) can also make non-symbolic judgments of quantity (Agrillo et al. 2010; Gross et al. 2009; Howard et al. 2018; Nelson and Jackson 2012; Pepperberg 2006). Indeed, a recent comparative review of the neurobiology of numerical cognition concluded that numerical quantity might be processed “… without the need to speculate on complex networks or sophisticated brains” (Lorenzi et al. 2021), suggesting that neocortical regions are not ubiquitously essential.
Non-symbolic numerical computations may be foundational for the capacity to perform symbolic numerical computations (Dehaene 2011). For example, the operation of a non-symbolic and evolutionarily ancient system for approximation of quantity is thought to be necessary for the development of symbolic arithmetic abilities (Adams et al. 2017; Halberda et al. 2012). Thus, more complex numerical operations that are phylogenetically novel and associated with the parietal cortex (Dehaene et al. 2004) may be scaffolded from non-symbolic operations such as the Approximate Number System (ANS). The ANS, which is subcortically based (Collins et al. 2017; Lorenzi et al. 2021) and allows a general representation of numbers (Adams et al. 2017), might be necessary for the development of a counting procedure. Indeed, performance in non-symbolic approximation of quantities tasks is predictive of school success in mathematics (Starr et al. 2013).
The Dynamic Network Model stands in contrast to the Cortical Superiority Model, in that the cortex is seen as exploiting subcortical computations, rather than suppressing them. Thus, the emphasis is on the dynamic interaction through which cortical processing has access to computations performed in subcortical regions and modulates this activity as a function of higher-level goals to support more complex, adaptive behaviors. This account predicts that with the evolution of the cortex, the functional domain of a cognitive ability can be broadened, allowing subcortical computations to be employed in a more flexible manner.

5. How Does the Subcortex Support the Emergence of Cognition?

In this section, we elaborate on the Dynamic Network Model by adding a novel conceptual framework for how the subcortex contributes to the evolution of cognition. The core hypothesis is that subcortical computations are essential for the development of a new cognitive function. We suggest a general neural hypothesis by which the Subcortex is Essential for the Early Development (SEED) of complex cognition.
According to our SEED hypothesis, since subcortical regions developed early in evolution and have remained functional up to the present, they are essential for the emergence of cognitive computations that enable organisms to adapt to an ever-changing environment. The SEED hypothesis has five principles.
First, new cognitive skills do not emerge in isolation; rather, each new cognitive function builds upon previous cognitive computations. Second, subcortical computations serve as the scaffolding for developing new cognitive functions. Third, similarly to other theories (Janacsek et al. 2022), a subcortical structure can support many functions via core computations. Fourth, a given function can be supported by many structures. Fifth, cortical regions have access to subcortical computations and can exploit them to establish a dynamic neural network, which enables more adaptive cognitive representations.
The SEED hypothesis is aligned with previous theories and empirical evidence. There is converging evidence that subcortical structures, such as the BG and cerebellum, play an essential role in the proceduralization of both motor and cognitive processes (Knowlton et al. 2016; Frank et al. 2004; Pascual-Leone et al. 1993; Callu et al. 2013; Molinari et al. 1997). Different subcortical structures are involved in the learning of skills through repeated practice and reinforcement over time (Knowlton et al. 2016; Nicolson et al. 2010; Pascual-Leone et al. 1993; Saban et al. 2017, 2018b). Subcortical and cortical structures act as a feedback loop (Bostan and Strick 2018) to facilitate processes, enabling efficient and automatic execution of tasks. Both in humans and other species, subcortical computations can be repurposed and manipulated, allowing cortical regions access to previous computations. We propose that a subcortical region serves as an essential evolutionary “seed”, allowing proceduralization of processes required for the emergence of a new cognitive function. Therefore, subcortical computations can allow the development of new expertise in various domains, from orienting of attention to playing the piano or solving math problems.
The SEED hypothesis emphasizes a neural principle of reuse and accessibility, which provides a useful framework for considering dynamic interactions between the subcortex and cortex. In order to develop new cognitive abilities, the brain has to find a “neuronal niche”, a set of circuits that are sufficiently close to the required function (Dehaene and Cohen 2007). These niches frequently come from ancient subcortical structures. Existing subcortical neural mechanisms can exert a powerful influence on the development of cognitive function. We propose that subcortical computations for one function can be accessed and reused to form a novel skill.
Several theories are in line with this principle of neural reuse and accessibility. Paul Rozin (Rozin 1976) proposed that computations that initially evolved to solve specific problems become accessible to other systems through evolution as well as within the individual lifetime of an organism. Change or expansion of a function, because it is more generally available or accessible, “would have adaptive value when an area of behavioral function could profit from programs initially developed for another purpose”. This idea has been reframed and elaborated upon in Gallese’s “neural exploitation” hypothesis (Gallese and Cuccio 2018), Hurley’s “shared circuits model” (Hurley 2008), Dehaene’s “neuronal recycling” theory (Dehaene and Cohen 2007), and Anderson’s “massive redeployment” hypothesis (Anderson 2007). The core idea is that neural networks are dynamic and acquire new uses after establishing an initial function.
For instance, subcortical computations to support reflexive orienting of attention (exogenous) can be used to develop an endogenous system, one that can operate on symbolic inputs (Carrasco 2011; Klein and Lawrence 2011). Similarly, subcortical non-symbolic numerical computations of quantity can be exploited for the emergence of symbolic arithmetic procedures such as counting (Rugani et al. 2009; Lorenzi et al. 2021).
The hypothesis that complex cognition emerges from ancient, elementary operations has been discussed in the literature examining the relationship between tool use and language (Corballis 1991; Thibault et al. 2021). Corballis (Corballis 1991) proposed that our unique ability for language was scaffolded from a system that had the computational capacity to build extended and flexible representations, or what is called a “generative learning device” (GAD). The GAD enables us to generate forms and meanings from a few elements, providing the basis for both language and tool use skills. While Corballis proposed that the GAD reflected a specialization of the left hemisphere, a recent fMRI study has shown that the syntactic operations associated with tool use and language activate the BG, one major subcortical structure (Thibault et al. 2021). Interestingly, learning a novel tool use task was also found to improve performance in a complex language task, pointing to shared computations across these two domains.
Similarly, in a recent study we demonstrated that subcortical mechanisms—lower visual channels—play a causal role in cognitive transfer of complex skills such as symbolic arithmetic (Saban et al. 2021b). We found that exposing only one monocular channel to a visuospatial training resulted in a larger transfer effect in the trained monocular channel compared with the untrained monocular channel. Such cognitive transfer was found between spatial abilities and mathematical subtraction problems (far transfer). Importantly, the benefits of the trained eye were not observed in old problems nor in other tasks that did not involve visuospatial abilities (the Stroop task, a multiplication task). These results challenge the exclusive role of the cortex in cognitive transfer and complex arithmetic.
These recent studies not only further support the evolutionary relation between different cognitive abilities (tool use and language or visuospatial and math), but also strengthen the notion that the subcortex mediates the development of complex cognition (i.e., language and math). In addition, the results suggest a new subcortical mechanism for the emergence of cognitive skills, which could be shared across different species.
Whereas the SEED hypothesis focuses on how new cognitive abilities emerge over the course of evolution, it is important to recognize that ancient subcortical computations continue to be essential for higher cognition, the critical idea of the Dynamic Network Model. Indeed, researchers have found that the subcortex contributes to many higher cognitive functions in humans spanning attention, arithmetic, language, executive functions, and more (Collins et al. 2017; Conway 2020; Gabay and Behrmann 2014; Saban et al. 2017, 2018, 2019, 2021a, 2021b; Soloveichick et al. 2021; Ullman 2001; Walenski et al. 2007). In addition, higher cognitive functions are exhibited by many different organisms, including species that do not possess developed cortical structures, such as insects, pigeons, and fish (Agrillo et al. 2010; Gross et al. 2009; Güntürkün and Bugnyar 2016; Howard et al. 2018; Kattner et al. 2017; Portavella et al. 2004; Saban et al. 2017; Schlegel and Schuster 2008).
Similarly, subcortical regions play a role in complex cognitive abilities during childhood when the cortex is still developing. For instance, the BG and cerebellum are involved in language and numerical abilities in studies examining developmental language disorder, dyslexia, and dyscalculia (Evans and Ullman 2016; Leisman et al. 2014; Lum et al. 2013; Nicolson et al. 2010; Nicolson and Fawcett 2007; Nicolson et al. 2001; Ullman et al. 2020). It has been suggested that a number of developmental disorders (e.g., DLD, dyslexia) might be partly explained by a procedural circuit deficit (Ullman et al. 2020), including the BG. Together, these studies further support our notion that subcortical regions have a role in the proceduralization of processes (see also Ullman et al. 2020; Evans and Ullman 2016). Thus, impairment in a subcortical structure can lead to a procedural deficit.
Figure 2 below depicts how the SEED hypothesis explains the development of new cognitive functions over the course of evolution. There are bidirectional relations between the cortex and the subcortex. The subcortex is essential for the early development of higher cognitive abilities, which depend on previous essential cognitive computations. The role of the subcortex is to serve as a scaffolding needed for the emerging cognition. The cortex accesses different subcortical computations and can orchestrate a wider range of operations.

6. Methods for Subcortical–Cortical Investigations

In order to explore the contribution of the subcortex to complex cognition, researchers will need to utilize a range of methods (see Table 2). First, behavioral methods can dissociate the contributions of subcortical and cortical processing to task performance. Visual information is monocularly segregated until it reaches binocular neurons in extrastriate regions of the visual cortex (Menon et al. 1997). Hence, visual input from the two eyes is segregated in the subcortex. A stereoscope can be used to create dichoptic stimuli in which each eye receives distinct information. By comparing performance between conditions solely dependent on monocular processing channels and conditions in which the information is integrated between the two eyes, one can make inferences about the kinds of visual computations that can be performed subcortically. This approach has been used to demonstrate the causal role of visual subcortical channels in attention, arithmetic, and other cognitive processes (Collins et al. 2017; Gabay and Behrmann 2014; Saban et al. 2018a, 2019, 2021a; Soloveichick et al. 2021).
Second, perturbation methods such as lesion studies provide a means to evaluate the causal role of a specific brain region in a cognitive function. To date, much of this work has been descriptive, designed to ask if damage to a particular region impairs performance in a particular cognitive domain. To move to the more nuanced question of how these regions contribute to task performance, these neuropsychological investigations should be tailored to test specific functional hypotheses. The SEED hypothesis can provide a guide in designing such studies, seeking to isolate elementary operations that may serve as the building blocks for more complex cognition. For example, we have found that patients with cerebellar degeneration or BG dysfunction have dissociable impairments in arithmetic procedures. While neuropsychological research has traditionally involved small samples, online methods can be employed for more efficient data collection from patients with subcortical pathology (Saban and Ivry 2021; Binoy et al. 2023).
Third, although most protocols for neuroimaging have generally been designed to maximize coverage and quality of activity in the cortex, some protocols have been developed to focus on the subcortex (e.g., Sylvester et al. 2007). Novel high-resolution scanning such as that made possible with 7T fMRI can provide a more sensitive technique to study neural activation in smaller subcortical regions (Wilkey et al. 2020).
Finally, comparative approaches can shed light on the evolutionary development of cognition and demonstrate potential subcortical–cortical relations. Studies involving species that do not possess a developed cortex (e.g., fish) have provided important insights regarding the cognitive abilities that can be supported by the subcortex (Güntürkün and Bugnyar 2016; Leadner et al. 2020; Saban et al. 2017). Studies involving species with a more highly developed cortex (e.g., non-human primates) can shed light on the interaction of subcortical–cortical networks and how the cortex might affect subcortical cognitive computations (Dorris et al. 2002; Gao et al. 2018). Finally, comparing neural connectivity between species and correlating it with different measures of cognitive abilities offers a way to test the dynamic network model.

7. Conclusions

More than a decade ago, Parvizi (2009) raised concerns with a “cortico-centric” view of the human brain, advocating for a broader perspective that recognized the vital contributions of the subcortex to cognition. One such approach to the organizational principle of the brain centers on the idea of neural reuse, whereby computational specializations that evolved to solve a specific problem become exploited in a different task domain (Anderson 2010; Rozin 1976). We believe that our SEED hypothesis provides a parsimonious and fruitful framework for considering how the subcortex and cortex interact to enable complex cognition. From an evolutionary perspective, we view the subcortex as providing the “seeds” for higher-order cognition, being used to build more elaborate operations. Understanding higher cognition requires consideration of whole-brain dynamics. Our perspective advocates for a theoretical shift from a classical perspective of the neocortex as the hub of high-level cognition to a more integrated view in which our cognitive abilities reflect the interplay between subcortical and cortical neural mechanisms.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, methodology, software, validation, formal analysis, resources, writing, visualization, supervision, funding acquisition, W.S. and S.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Israel Science Foundation grant number 574/22.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the comments from Rich Ivry on the initial version of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Adams, John, Patrick Barmby, and Alex Mesoudi. 2017. The Nature and Development of Mathematics: Cross Disciplinary Perspectives on Cognition, Learning and Culture. Oxfordshire: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  2. Agrillo, Christian, Laura Piffer, and Angelo Bisazza. 2010. Large Number Discrimination by Mosquitofish. PLoS ONE 5: e15232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Agrillo, Christian, Laura Piffer, Angelo Bisazza, and Brian Butterworth. 2012. Evidence for two numerical systems that are similar in humans and guppies. PLoS ONE 7: e31923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Andres, Michael, Barbara Pelgrims, Nicolas Michaux, Etienne Olivier, and Mauro Pesenti. 2011. Role of Distinct Parietal Areas in Arithmetic: An FMRI-Guided TMS Study. NeuroImage 54: 3048–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Anderson, Michael L. 2007. The massive redeployment hypothesis and the functional topography of the brain. Philosophical Psychology 20: 143–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Anderson, Michael L. 2010. Neural Reuse: A Fundamental Organizational Principle of the Brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 245–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Ansari, Daniel, and Bibek Dhital. 2006. Age-Related Changes in the Activation of the Intraparietal Sulcus during Nonsymbolic Magnitude Processing: An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18: 1820–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Arsalidou, Marie, and Margot J. Taylor. 2011. Is 2 + 2 = 4? Meta-Analyses of Brain Areas Needed for Numbers and Calculations. NeuroImage 54: 2382–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Arsalidou, Marie, Matthew Pawliw-Levac, Mahsa Sadeghi, and Juan Pascual-Leone. 2018. Brain Areas Associated with Numbers and Calculations in Children: Meta-Analyses of FMRI Studies. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 30: 239–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Binoy, Sharon, Rachel Woody, Richard B. Ivry, and William Saban. 2023. Feasibility and Efficacy of Online Neuropsychological Assessment. Sensors 23: 5160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bostan, Andreea C., and Peter L. Strick. 2018. The Basal Ganglia and the Cerebellum: Nodes in an Integrated Network. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 19: 338–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Brodmann, Korbinian. 1902. Zur Methodik der hypnotischen Behandlung. 5. Fortsetzung und Schluß. Zschr Hypnotism 10: 314–75. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bush, Eliot C., and John M. Allman. 2004. The scaling of frontal cortex in primates and carnivores. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101: 3962–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Callu, Delphine, Joelle Lopez, and Nicole El Massioui. 2013. Cerebellar Deep Nuclei Involvement in Cognitive Adaptation and Automaticity. Learning and Memory 20: 344–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Carrasco, Marisa. 2011. Visual Attention: The Past 25 Years. Vision Research 51: 1484–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Chittka, Lars, and Jeremy Niven. 2009. Are bigger brains better? Current Biology 19: R995–R1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Clayton, Nicola S., and Nathan J. Emery. 2015. Avian models for human cognitive neuroscience: A proposal. Neuron 86: 1330–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Collins, Elliot, Joonkoo Park, and Marlene Behrmann. 2017. Numerosity Representation Is Encoded in Human Subcortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114: E2806–E2815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Colver, Allan, and Sarah Longwell. 2013. New understanding of adolescent brain development: Relevance to transitional healthcare for young people with long term conditions. Archives of Disease in Childhood 98: 902–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Conway, Christopher M. 2020. How Does the Brain Learn Environmental Structure? Ten Core Principles for Understanding the Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Statistical Learning. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 112: 279–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Corballis, Michael C. 1991. The Lopsided Ape: Evolution of the Generative Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press on Demand, vol. 366. [Google Scholar]
  22. Corbetta, Maurizio, J. Michelle Kincade, John M. Ollinger, Marc P. McAvoy, and Gordon L. Shulman. 2000. Voluntary Orienting Is Dissociated from Target Detection in Human Posterior Parietal Cortex. Nature neuroscience 3: 292–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Dehaene, Stanislas, and Laurent Cohen. 1997. Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic. Cortex 33: 219–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Dehaene, Stanislas. 2011. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press, USA. [Google Scholar]
  25. Dehaene, Stanislas, and Laurent Cohen. 2007. Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron 56: 384–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Dehaene, Stanislas, Manuela Piazza, Philippe Pinel, and Laurent Cohen. 2003. Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20: 487–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Dehaene, Stanislas, Nicolas Molko, Laurent Cohen, and Anna J. Wilson. 2004. Arithmetic and the Brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 14: 218–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Dorris, Michael C., Raymond M. Klein, Stefan Everling, and Douglas P. Munoz. 2002. Contribution of the Primate Superior Colliculus to Inhibition of Return. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14: 1256–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Evans, Tanya M., and Michael T. Ullman. 2016. An Extension of the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis from Developmental Language Disorders to Mathematical Disability. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Fernandez, Lara, Nigel C. Rogasch, Michael Do, Gillian Clark, Brendan P. Major, Wei Peng Teo, Linda K. Byrne, and Peter G. Enticott. 2020. Cerebral Cortical Activity Following Non-Invasive Cerebellar Stimulation—A Systematic Review of Combined TMS and EEG Studies. Cerebellum 19: 309–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Finger, Stanley. 1994. History of Neuropsychology. In Neuropsychology. Cambridge: Academic Press, pp. 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Frank, Michael J., Lauren C. Seeberger, and Randall C. O’Reilly. 2004. By Carrot or by Stick: Cognitive Reinforcement Learning in Parkinsonism. Science 306: 1940–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Frith, Chris, and Ray Dolan. 1996. The Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Higher Cognitive Functions. Cognitive Brain Research 5: 175–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Gabay, Shai, and Marlene Behrmann. 2014. Attentional Dynamics Mediated by Subcortical Mechanisms. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics 76: 2375–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Gallese, Vittorio, and Valentina Cuccio. 2018. The neural exploitation hypothesis and its implications for an embodied approach to language and cognition: Insights from the study of action verbs processing and motor disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex 100: 215–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Gao, Zhenyu, Courtney Davis, Alyse M. Thomas, Michael N. Economo, Amada M. Abrego, Karel Svoboda, Chris I. De Zeeuw, and Nuo Li. 2018. A Cortico-Cerebellar Loop for Motor Planning. Nature 563: 113–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Geschwind, Norman. 1970. The organization of language and the brain. Science 170: 940–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Grabner, Roland H., Daniel Ansari, Karl Koschutnig, Gernot Reishofer, Franz Ebner, and Christa Neuper. 2009. To Retrieve or to Calculate? Left Angular Gyrus Mediates the Retrieval of Arithmetic Facts during Problem Solving. Neuropsychologia 47: 604–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Gross, Hans J., Mario Pahl, Aung Si, Hong Zhu, Jürgen Tautz, and Shaowu Zhang. 2009. Number-Based Visual Generalisation in the Honeybee. Edited by Hiromu Tanimoto. PLoS ONE 4: e4263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Güntürkün, Onur, and Thomas Bugnyar. 2016. Cognition without Cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20: 291–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Halberda, Justin, Ryan Ly, Jeremy B. Wilmer, Daniel Q. Naiman, and Laura Germine. 2012. Number Sense across the Lifespan as Revealed by a Massive Internet-Based Sample. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 11116–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Howard, Scarlett R., Aurore Avarguès-Weber, Jair E. Garcia, Andrew D. Greentree, and Adrian G. Dyer. 2018. Numerical Ordering of Zero in Honey Bees. Science 360: 1124–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Hurley, Susan. 2008. The shared circuits model (SCM): How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Janacsek, Karolina, Tanya M. Evans, Mariann Kiss, Leela Shah, Hal Blumenfeld, and Michael T. Ullman. 2022. Subcortical Cognition: The Fruit Below the Rind. Annual Review of Neuroscience 45: 361–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Jankovic, Joseph. 2008. Parkinson’s disease: Clinical features and diagnosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 79: 368–76. [Google Scholar]
  46. Jerison, Harry J. 1985. Animal intelligence as encephalization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences 308: 21–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Kattner, Florian, Aaron Cochrane, Christopher R. Cox, Thomas E. Gorman, and C. Shawn Green. 2017. Perceptual Learning Generalization from Sequential Perceptual Training as a Change in Learning Rate. Current Biology 27: 840–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Klein, Raymond M., and Michael A. Lawrence. 2011. Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention. Edited by Michael I. Posner. New York: Guilford Press, p. 57. [Google Scholar]
  49. Knowlton, Barbara J., Alexander L. M. Siegel, and Teena D. Moody. 2016. Procedural Learning in Humans. Collection in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology, 2nd ed. Oxford: Academic Press, vol. 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Koziol, Leonard F., and Deborah Ely Budding. 2009. Subcortical Structures and Cognition: Implications for Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Springer Science & Business Media. [Google Scholar]
  51. LaBar, Kevin S., Darren R. Gitelman, M-Marsel Mesulam, and Todd B. Parrish. 2001. Impact of signal-to-noise on functional MRI of the human amygdala. Neuroreport 12: 3461–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Leadner, Keren, Liora Sekely, Raymond M. Klein, and Shai Gabay. 2020. Evolution of Social Attentional Cues: Evidence from the Archerfish. Cognition 207: 104511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Lebel, Catherine, and Christian Beaulieu. 2011. Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. Journal of Neuroscience 31: 10937–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Leisman, Gerry, Orit Braun-Benjamin, and Robert Melillo. 2014. Cognitive-Motor Interactions of the Basal Ganglia in Development. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 8: 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Lorenzi, Elena, Matilde Perrino, and Giorgio Vallortigara. 2021. Numerosities and Other Magnitudes in the Brains: A Comparative View. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Lum, Jarrad A. G., Michael T. Ullman, and Gina Conti-Ramsden. 2013. Procedural Learning Is Impaired in Dyslexia: Evidence from a Meta-Analysis of Serial Reaction Time Studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34: 3460–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. MacLean, Paul D. 1988. Triune brain. In Comparative Neuroscience and Neurobiology. Boston: Springer, pp. 126–28. [Google Scholar]
  58. Menon, Ravi S., Seiji Ogawa, John P. Strupp, and Kâmil Uǧurbil. 1997. Ocular Dominance in Human V1 Demonstrated by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Journal of Neurophysiology 77: 2780–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Mischel, Walter, Yuichi Shoda, and Monica L. Rodriguez. 1989. Delay of gratification in children. Science 244: 933–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Molinari, Marco, Maria G. Leggio, Alessandra Solida, Roberto Ciorra, Sandro Misciagna, Maria C. Silveri, and Laura Petrosini. 1997. Cerebellum and Procedural Learning: Evidence from Focal Cerebellar Lesions. Brain 120: 1753–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Nelson, Ximena J., and Robert R. Jackson. 2012. The role of numerical competence in a specialized predatory strategy of an araneophagic spider. Animal Cognition 15: 699–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Nicolson, Roderick I., and Angela J. Fawcett. 2007. Procedural Learning Difficulties: Reuniting the Developmental Disorders? Trends in Neurosciences 30: 135–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Nicolson, Roderick I., Angela J. Fawcett, and Paul Dean. 2001. Developmental Dyslexia: The Cerebellar Deficit Hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences 24: 508–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Nicolson, Roderick I., Angela J. Fawcett, Rebecca L. Brookes, and Jamie L. Needle. 2010. Procedural Learning and Dyslexia. Dyslexia 212: 194–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Niven, Jeremy E. 2005. Brain evolution: Getting better all the time? Current Biology 15: R624–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Noback, Charles R., David A. Ruggiero, Norman L. Strominger, and Robert J. Demarest, eds. 2005. The Human Nervous System: Structure and Function (No. 744). Totowa: Springer Science & Business Media. [Google Scholar]
  67. Parks, Ashley N., and Jeroen B. Smaers. 2018. The evolution of the frontal lobe in humans. In Digital Endocasts: From Skulls to Brains. Tokyo: Springer, pp. 205–18. [Google Scholar]
  68. Parvizi, Josef. 2009. Corticocentric Myopia: Old Bias in New Cognitive Sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 354–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Pascual-Leone, Alvaro, Jordan Grafman, Katherine Clark, M. Stewart, Steve Massaquoi, Jou-Shin Lou, and Mark Hallett. 1993. Procedural Learning in Parkinson’s Disease and Cerebellar Degeneration. Annals of Neurology 34: 594–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Peelen, Marius V., Dirk J. Heslenfeld, and Jan Theeuwes. 2004. Endogenous and Exogenous Attention Shifts Are Mediated by the Same Large-Scale Neural Network. NeuroImage 22: 822–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Pepperberg, Irene M. 2006. Cognitive and communicative abilities of Grey parrots. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100: 77–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Poeppel, David. 2014. The neuroanatomic and neurophysiological infrastructure for speech and language. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 28: 142–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. Portavella, Manuel, Blas Torres, and Cosme Salas. 2004. Avoidance Response in Goldfish: Emotional and Temporal Involvement of Medial and Lateral Telencephalic Pallium. Journal of Neuroscience 24: 2335–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Rafal, Robert, and Avishai Henik. 1994. The Neurology of Inhibition: Integrating Controlled and Automatic Processes. In Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 1–51. [Google Scholar]
  75. Roth, Gerhard, and Ursula Dicke. 2005. Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 250–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Rozin, Paul. 1976. The Selection of Foods by Rats, Humans, and Other Animals. Advances in the Study of Behavior 6: 21–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Rugani, Rosa, Laura Fontanari, Eleonora Simoni, Lucia Regolin, and Giorgio Vallortigara. 2009. Arithmetic in Newborn Chicks. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 276: 2451–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Saban, William, and Richard B. Ivry. 2021. PONT: A Protocol for Online Neuropsychological Testing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 33: 2413–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Saban, William, Asael Y. Sklar, Ran R. Hassin, and Shai Gabay. 2021a. Ancient Visual Channels Have a Causal Role in Arithmetic Calculations. Scientific Reports 11: 22795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Saban, William, Gal Raz, Roland H. Grabner, Shai Gabay, and Roi Cohen Kadosh. 2021b. Primitive Visual Channels Have a Causal Role in Cognitive Transfer. Scientific Reports 11: 8759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Saban, William, Liora Sekely, Raymond M. Klein, and Shai Gabay. 2017. Endogenous Orienting in the Archer Fish. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114: 7577–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Saban, William, Liora Sekely, Raymond M. Klein, and Shai Gabay. 2018a. Monocular Channels Have a Functional Role in Endogenous Orienting. Neuropsychologia 111: 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Saban, William, Noam Weinbach, and Shai Gabay. 2019. Monocular Channels Have a Functional Role in Phasic Alertness and Temporal Expectancy. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 81: 752–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Saban, William, Raymond M. Klein, and Shai Gabay. 2018b. Probabilistic versus ‘Pure’ Volitional Orienting: A Monocular Difference. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics 80: 669–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Saban, William, Shai Gabay, and Eyal Kalanthroff. 2018c. More than Just Channeling: The Role of Subcortical Mechanisms in Executive Functions—Evidence from the Stroop Task. Acta Psychologica 189: 36–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Schlegel, Thomas, and Stefan Schuster. 2008. Small Circuits for Large Tasks: High-Speed Decision-Making in Archerfish. Science 319: 104–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  87. Soloveichick, Margarita, Ruth Kimchi, and Shai Gabay. 2021. Functional Involvement of Subcortical Structures in Global-Local Processing. Cognition 206: 104476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Starr, Ariel, Melissa E. Libertus, and Elizabeth M. Brannon. 2013. Number Sense in Infancy Predicts Mathematical Abilities in Childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110: 18116–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Sylvester, Richard, Oliver Josephs, Jon Driver, and Geraint Rees. 2007. Visual FMRI Responses in Human Superior Colliculus Show a Temporal–Nasal Asymmetry That Is Absent in Lateral Geniculate and Visual Cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 97: 1495–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Thibault, Simon, Raphaël Py, Angelo Mattia Gervasi, Romeo Salemme, Eric Koun, Martin Lövden, Véronique Boulenger, Alice C. Roy, and Claudio Brozzoli. 2021. Tool Use and Language Share Syntactic Processes and Neural Patterns in the Basal Ganglia. Science 374: eabe0874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Ullman, Michael T. 2001. A Neurocognitive Perspective on Language: The Declarative/Procedural Model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 717–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Ullman, Michael T., F. Sayako Earle, Matthew Walenski, and Karolina Janacsek. 2020. The Neurocognition of Developmental Disorders of Language. Annual Review of Psychology 71: 389–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Van Horik, Jayden O., Nicola S. Clayton, and Nathan J. Emery. 2012. CONVERGENT Evolution of Cognition in Corvids, Apes and Other Animals. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  94. Walenski, Matthew, Stewart H. Mostofsky, and Michael T. Ullman. 2007. Speeded Processing of Grammar and Tool Knowledge in Tourette’s Syndrome. Neuropsychologia 45: 2447–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Westerberg, Helena, and Torkel Klingberg. 2007. Changes in Cortical Activity after Training of Working Memory—A Single-Subject Analysis. Physiology & Behavior 92: 186–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Whaley, Nathaniel Robb, Shinsuke Fujioka, and Zbigniew K. Wszolek. 2011. Autosomal dominant cerebellar ataxia type I: A review of the phenotypic and genotypic characteristics. Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases 6: 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  97. Wilkey, Eric D., Benjamin N. Conrad, Darren J. Yeo, and Gavin R. Price. 2020. Shared Numerosity Representations Across Formats and Tasks Revealed with 7 Tesla FMRI: Decoding, Generalization, and Individual Differences in Behavior. Cerebral Cortex Communications 1: tgaa038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. You, Wen Kai, and Shreesh P. Mysore. 2020. Endogenous and Exogenous Control of Visuospatial Selective Attention in Freely Behaving Mice. Nature Communications 11: 1986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. The number of PubMed results as a function of year when using the keywords “cognition” and cortical regions (frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobe) compared with “cognition” and subcortical regions (cerebellum, basal ganglia, superior colliculus, and thalamus).
Figure 1. The number of PubMed results as a function of year when using the keywords “cognition” and cortical regions (frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobe) compared with “cognition” and subcortical regions (cerebellum, basal ganglia, superior colliculus, and thalamus).
Jintelligence 11 00121 g001
Figure 2. Foundational computations supported by the subcortex (e.g., quantity evaluation, exogenous attention) can be repurposed to develop new cognitive abilities (counting, endogenous attention). The cortex has access to subcortical computations, creating a dynamic network that allows for the emergence of complex cognition (symbolic arithmetic, modulated attention). Future studies should examine the potential contributions of subcortical structures to other cognitive computations (e.g., carrying) and how they are related to more basic functions (e.g., working memory).
Figure 2. Foundational computations supported by the subcortex (e.g., quantity evaluation, exogenous attention) can be repurposed to develop new cognitive abilities (counting, endogenous attention). The cortex has access to subcortical computations, creating a dynamic network that allows for the emergence of complex cognition (symbolic arithmetic, modulated attention). Future studies should examine the potential contributions of subcortical structures to other cognitive computations (e.g., carrying) and how they are related to more basic functions (e.g., working memory).
Jintelligence 11 00121 g002
Table 1. Summary of the three models.
Table 1. Summary of the three models.
ModelSolitary CortexCortical SuperiorityDynamic Network
Feature
  • Subcortical involvement in humans
NONOYES
2.
Subcortical involvement in species without a developed neocortex
NOYESYES
3.
Neocortical regions’ influence on subcortical regions
NO
influence
Superiority
(suppression/migration)
Dynamic
(modulation/joint work)
Table 2. Four methods for subcortical investigations.
Table 2. Four methods for subcortical investigations.
MethodThe Main Benefits of the MethodQuestion Addressed
(1)
Dichoptic presentation
It enables us to examine the functional contribution of monocular neural substrates (mostly subcortical) in human cognition.What is the involvement of monocular regions in a cognitive function?
(2)
Perturbation methods such as lesion studies
They allow us to infer the specific causal contribution of a given subcortical mechanism.What is the role of a specific subcortical region in a cognitive function?
(3)
Imaging studies focusing on subcortical regions
They allow us to detect neural activity of subcortical regions and the neural dynamics of subcortical–cortical networks.What is the role of different subcortical regions in a cognitive function and the interactions between cortical and subcortical regions?
(4)
Animal cognition studies
They provide insights regarding the cognitive ability of a neural system lacking a developed cortex.What is the evolutionary origin of a cognitive function?
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Saban, W.; Gabay, S. Contributions of Lower Structures to Higher Cognition: Towards a Dynamic Network Model. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 121. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060121

AMA Style

Saban W, Gabay S. Contributions of Lower Structures to Higher Cognition: Towards a Dynamic Network Model. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(6):121. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060121

Chicago/Turabian Style

Saban, William, and Shai Gabay. 2023. "Contributions of Lower Structures to Higher Cognition: Towards a Dynamic Network Model" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 6: 121. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060121

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop